Analysis of the Reaction Time and Dominance in Elderly Men: A
Pilot Study
Francesca Campoli
1a
, Vincenzo Bonaiuto
1b
, Lucio Caprioli
1c
, Saeid Edriss
1d
, Emilio Panichi
1e
,
Michele Panzarino
2f
, Cristian Romagnoli
2g
, Giuseppe Annino
3h
and Elvira Padua
2i
1
Sports Engineering Lab, Dept. Industrial Eng., Univ. Rome Tor Vergata, Rome, Italy
2
Dept. of Human Science & Prom. of Quality of Life, San Raffaele Rome Univ., Rome, Italy
3
Dept. of Medicine Systems, Univ. Rome Tor Vergata, Rome, Italy
Keywords: Elderly Men, Reaction Time, Dominance Limb.
Abstract: The study analyses reaction times in elderly subjects and investigates the reactivity of the dominant and non-
dominant hand and foot. 20 men (73.3 ± 3.1 years), healthy, free from injury and without any physical problem
that could affect the test results. The tests are the baseline reaction times, the plate tapping test and foot tapping
test. Descriptive statistical procedures are presented as mean ± SD and the percentage changes (Δ%) were
calculated for each test. The significance level was P<0.05. In baseline reaction time test, the non-dominant
hand showed a shorter reaction time than the dominant hand of about 28 msec (p = 0.05). In the plate tapping
test, on the other hand, the dominant hand was more reactive than the non-dominant hand (7%, p = 0.002). In
the foot test, the difference between the dominant and non-dominant feet was 3% (p = 0.1). The difference in
performance between the tests performed could be due to the difficulty of the required motor task. The study
lays the foundation for developing a motor work protocol focused on reactivity, a motor ability that
physiologically degenerates with age and is of fundamental importance for the individual's physical and
cognitive well-being.
1 INTRODUCTION
Reactivity is a useful measure to define how quickly
an organism can respond to a particular stimulus,
specifically the reaction time is defined as the time
interval between the moment in which the brain
represents a sensory stimulus and the moment in
which the subsequent behavioral response takes place
(Welford AT, 1980). In fact, reactivity has been
extensively studied as its practical implications can
have a great consequence, at any age.
Many factors have been shown to influence
reaction times, such as age, gender, left or right hand,
a
https://orcid.org/0009-0004-1342-5881
b
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-2328-4793
c
https://orcid.org/0009-0005-4049-5225
d
https://orcid.org/0009-0000-0224-8294
e
https://orcid.org/0009-0003-6591-1147
f
https://orcid.org/0009-0009-0572-1206
g
https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0904-634X
h
https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8578-6046
i
https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5227-2567
physical fitness, fatigue, distraction, breathing cycle,
type of stimulus and the decline in processing speed
are some of the hypotheses formulated to explain the
changes observed in reaction times at different ages
(Hultsch DF. et al, 2002; Adam JJ et al, 1999).
Adult human reaction times in response to simple
tasks slow with age at a rate of 2–6 ms per decade
(Fozard JL et al, 1994; Gottsdanker R, 1982). Simple
reaction time shortens from infancy into the late 20s,
then increases slowly until the 50s and 60s, and then
lengthens faster as the person gets into his 70s and
beyond (Jevas and Yan, 2001; Rose et al., 2002).
Luchies et al. (2002) also reported that this age effect
was more marked for complex reaction time tasks.
158
Campoli, F., Bonaiuto, V., Caprioli, L., Edriss, S., Panichi, E., Panzarino, M., Romagnoli, C., Annino, G. and Padua, E.
Analysis of the Reaction Time and Dominance in Elderly Men: A Pilot Study.
DOI: 10.5220/0012186300003587
In Proceedings of the 11th International Conference on Sport Sciences Research and Technology Support (icSPORTS 2023), pages 158-161
ISBN: 978-989-758-673-6; ISSN: 2184-3201
Copyright © 2023 by SCITEPRESS Science and Technology Publications, Lda. Under CC license (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0)
In an experiment using a computer mouse, Peters
and Ivanoff (1999) found that right-handed people
were faster with their right hand (as expected), but
left-handed people were equally fast with both hands.
The preferred hand was generally faster.
As age increases, problems in cognitive abilities
also increase, such as divided attention, memory
decline, etc. (Lu et al, 2017). Dementia and other
diseases with cognitive impairment have become a
major global problem as the number of older adults
increases and they affects individual quality of life
(Bruce et al, 2014). Reaction time is related to
cognitive functions (Christ B.U et al, 2018; Chen K
et al, 2017). A study by Phillips et al. (2013) found
that patients with mild cognitive impairment and
Alzheimer's disease had significantly longer reaction
times than normal aging control groups. MacDonald
et al. (2008) found that reaction time variability in
older adults was usually associated with slower
reaction times and worse recognition of stimuli, and
suggested that variability might be a useful measure
of general neural integrity.
The hemispheres of the cerebrum are specialized
for different tasks. The left hemisphere is regarded as
the verbal and logical brain, and the right hemisphere
is thought to govern creativity, spatial relations, face
recognition, and emotions, among other things. Also,
the right hemisphere controls the left hand, and the
left hemisphere controls the right hand. This has made
researchers think that the left hand should be faster at
reaction times involving spatial relationships (such as
pointing at a target). The results of Boulinquez and
Bartélémy (2000) and Bartélémy and Boulinquez
(2001 and 2002) all supported this idea.
The present study aims to demonstrate this
hypothesis by relating it to the motor task in the
elderly over 70 years old and it want to establish a
starting and reference point on reaction times in
elderly subjects in order to then be able to intervene
on the ideal motor activity to be performed in elderly
Moreover the study wants to investigate the motor
behavior recorded in different tasks, simple reaction
and frequency of movement in upper and lower limbs.
2 METHODS
2.1 Sample
20 elderly men (73.3 ± 3.1 years), healthy, free from
injury and without any physical problem that could
affect the test results. Subjects with a history of
balance deficits, neurologic disorders or
musculoskeletal injury were excluded from the study.
Nineteen men have a dominance on both right
hand and foot; just one of them has a dominance to
the other side both on his hand and foot.
Written informed consent was obtained from all
the participants after familiarization and explanation
of the benefit and risks involved in the procedures of
this study. All participants were informed about the
study protocol and gave their informed consent to
participate and they were informed that they were free
to withdraw from the study at any time without
penalty. This study was approved by the Internal
Research Board of the University of Rome "Tor
Vergata".
2.2 Assessment
Baseline Reaction Times Test
The test, performed with the Norway ergotest Muscle
Lab, consists of a succession of 5 green stimuli
(diameter 6-8 cm.) displayed in the centre of the PC
monitor. The background is black. No focus point is
ever given to the subjects in order to assess their
ability to detect the signal without the help of cues.
The subject is required to press the space key on the
computer keyboard in response to the presentation of
a stimulus. In all tests, responses given in under 180
ms, which according to the literature is the threshold
for a simple visual reaction, were not accepted as
correct as there would be insufficient time to organize
a response and reaction. Instead, these are considered
anticipated responses, that is, as action initiated
before the appearance of the stimulus. If the subject
fails to respond, the stimulus disappears from the
screen after 6 s. The disappearance of a stimulus is
followed by an interval which varies according to a
fixed sequence between 0.8 and 2.5 s. The test is
preceded by 6 practice trials in which the program
indicates whenever an error is made so as to facilitate
learning by trial and error. At the end of the test, the
program provides the single response times. The
number of anticipated responses and omissions and
the results of the test trials are memorized on
computer and may be printed as required (De Danti et
al. 1998).
Plate Tapping Test
Each subject was asked to touch two clips (diameter
of 3 cm) as quickly as possible to the right and left on
a desk. The test is performed by the dominant hand
(DH) and the non-dominant hand (NDH) and a 10-
second test is performed three times with each hand
and the best result is recorded (Eurofit, 1988).
Analysis of the Reaction Time and Dominance in Elderly Men: A Pilot Study
159
Foot Tapping Test
The subjects sitting on a chair must touch with their
feet as quickly as possible the right and left part of the
floor bordered by a central line.
The test performed three times with each foot and
the best result is recorded in 10 seconds (Eurofit,
1988).
2.3 Statistical Analysis
The results are expressed as mean ± SD. Preliminary
assumption testing was conducted to check for
normality, linearity, univariate and multivariate
outliers, homogeneity of variance-covariance
matrices, and multicollinearity. Paired t-test were
performed to assess the significance of differences.
The corresponding P values are provided for each
analysis. The value of statistical significance was
accepted with P<0.05. IBM - SPSS 20.0 for Windows
(SPSS, Inc. Chicago. IL, USA) was used for
statistical analysis.
3 RESULTS
Results for each test are presented in table 1.
Table 1: Data are mean ± SD.
In baseline reaction time test (figure 1), the non-
dominant hand performs a lower reaction time than
the dominant hand of about 28 msec (10%, p=0.05).
In the plate tapping test, on the other hand, the
dominant hand is more reactive than the non-
dominant hand ((7%, p = 0.002).
In the foot test, the difference between the
dominant and non-dominant feet is 3% not
statistically significant (p = 0.1).
4 CONCLUSIONS
The study represents a valid starting point to carry out
further studies on a larger sample and to place the
difference between men and women.
this exploratory study aimed to quantify the
reaction times of a representation of the elderly
population in order to then be able to adapt and
propose an ad hoc motor activity protocol focused on
reactivity, a declining ability in this age group.
The difference in performance between the
performed tests could be due to the difficulty of the
required motor task. In the reaction time test, motor
participation is reduced to the hand portion only,
while the tapping tests, hand and foot, involve a larger
body compartment in which more aspects converge,
not just fully reactive.
It would be also interesting going through what
the dominance value could induce by looking at a
larger sample of people with a left hand/foot
dominance in order to reinforce Peters and Ivanoff’s
study.
Moreover, it’s really important to keep ongoing
with that theory by thinking of other aspects: gender,
physical activity level, frequency of movement
including other tasks and cognitive diseases.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
The authors are grateful to the organization the
organization ANCS (Accademia Nazionale di
Cultura Sportiva) for recruiting the sample.
REFERENCES
Adam JJ, Paas FG, Buekers MJ, Wuyts IJ, Spijkers WA,
Wallmeyer P. Gender differences in choice reaction
time: Evidence for differential strategies.
Ergonomics. 1999
Barthélémy, S., and P. Boulinguez. 2002. Orienting
visuospatial attention generates manual reaction time
asymmetries in target detection and pointing.
Behavioral Brain Research 133(1): 109-116.
Botwinick, J. 1966. Cautiousness in advanced age. Journal
of Gerontology 21: 347-353.
Boulinguez. P. and S. Barthélémy. 2000. Influence of the
movement parameter to be controlled on manual RT
asymmetries in righthanders. Brain and Cognition
44(3): 653-661.
De Tanti, A; Inzaghi, M G; Bonelli, G; Mancuso, M; et al.
Normative data of the MIDA battery for the evaluation
of reaction times. Europa Medicophysica; Turin Vol.
34, Fasc. 4, (Dec 1998): 211.
Eurofit, (1988) European Tests of Physical Fitness. Rome.
Italy: Council of Europe, Committee for the
Development of Sport
Hultsch David F., MacDonald Stuart W. S., Dixon Roger
A. Variability in reaction time performance of younger
and older adults. The Journals of Gerontology: Series
B, Volume 57, Issue 2, 1 March 2002.
dominant limb non-dominant limb Δ p
Reaction time test (sec) 0.277 ± 0.07 0.249 ± 0.02 10% 0.05
Plate tapping test (taps) 31.20 ± 6.00 29.06 ± 6.00 7% 0.002
Foot tapping test (taps) 30.00 ± 4.90 29.00 ± 4.50 3% 0.1
icSPORTS 2023 - 11th International Conference on Sport Sciences Research and Technology Support
160
Jevas, S. and J. H. Yan. 2001. The effect of aging on
cognitive function: a preliminary quantitative review.
Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 72: A-49.
Lajoie, Y. and S. P. Gallagher. 2004. Predicting falls within
the elderly community: comparison of postural sway,
reaction time, the Berg balance scale and the Activities-
specific Balance Confidence (ABC) scale for
comparing fallers and non-fallers. Archives of
Gerontology and Geriatrics 38(1): 11-25.
Luchies, C. W., J. Schiffman, L. G. Richards, M. R.
Thompson, D. Bazuin, and A. J. DeYoung. 2002.
Effects of age, step direction, and reaction condition on
the ability to step quickly. The Journals of Gerontology,
Series A 57(4): M246.
Myerson, J. S. Robertson, and S. Hale. 2007. Aging and
intraindiviual variability in performance: Analysis of
response time distributions. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behaviour 88(3): 319-337
Noble, C. E., B. L. Baker, and T. A. Jones. 1964. Age and
sex parameters in psychomotor learning. Perceptual
and Motor Skills 19: 935-945.
Peters, M. and J. Ivanoff. 1999. Performance asymmetries
in computer mouse control of right-handers, and left
handers with left- and righthanded mouse experience.
Journal of Motor Behaviour 31(1): 86-94.
Redfern, M. S., M. Muller, J. R. Jennings, J. M. Furman.
2002. Attentional dynamics in postural control during
perturbations in young and older adults. The Journals
of Gerontology, Series A 57(8): B298.
Rose, S. A., J. F. Feldman, J. J. Jankowski, and D. M. Caro.
2002. A longitudinal study of visual expectation and
reaction time in the first year of life. Child Development
73(1): 47.
Welford AT. Choice reaction time: Basic concepts. In:
Welford AT, editor. Reaction Times. New York:
Academic Press; 1980
Fozard JL, Vercryssen M, Reynolds SL, Hancock PA,
Quilter RE. Age differences and changes in reaction
time: the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. J
Gerontol 49: P179–P189, 1994.
Gottsdanker R. Age and simple reaction time. J Gerontol
37: 342– 348, 1982.
Bruce, W.; Rafik, G.; Frank, K.; Mihaela, P.; Alex, M.
Design of Games for Measurement of Cognitive
Impairment. In Proceedings of the IEEE-EMBS
International Conference on Biomedical and Health
Informatics (BHI), Valencia, Spain, 1–4 June 2014.
Lu, M.-H.; Lin, W.; Yueh, H.-P. Development and
Evaluation of a Cognitive Training Game for Older
People: A Design-based Approach. Front.
Psychol. 2017
Christ, B.U.; Combrinck, M.I.; Thomas, K.G.F. Thomas
Both Reaction Time and Accuracy Measures of
Intraindividual Variability Predict Cognitive
Performance in Alzheimer’s Disease. Front. Hum.
Neurosci. 2018.
Chen, K.; Weng, C.; Hsiao, S.; Tsao, W.; Koo, M.
Cognitive decline and slower reaction time in elderly
individuals with mild cognitive impairment. Jpn.
Psychogeriatr. Soc. 2017
Phillips, M.; Rogers, P.; Haworth, J.; Bayer, A.; Tales, A.
Intra-Individual Reaction Time Variability in Mild
Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer’s Disease:
Gender, Processing Load and Speed Factors. PLoS
ONE 2013.
Analysis of the Reaction Time and Dominance in Elderly Men: A Pilot Study
161