ENCOURAGING MUSEUM VISITOR ENGAGEMENT USING
SPONTANEOUS TALK-IN-INTERACTION AUDIO GUIDES
Brian Elliston
1
and Elizabeth FitzGerald
2
1
Horizon DTC, University of Nottingham, Jubilee Campus, Wollaton Road, Nottingham, NG8 1BB, U.K.
2
Institute of Educational Technology, The Open University, Walton Hall, Milton Keynes, MK7 6AA, U.K.
Keywords: Audio Guides, Mobile Learning, Conversation Analysis, Footing, Human-computer Interaction, Informal
Learning.
Abstract: We describe the building and testing of a museum audio tour with content recorded as spontaneous
interactive dialogue between two curators as they walked around an art gallery. The aim was to produce a
guide which would increase the amount of topically relevant talk shared by people visiting a museum in
groups of two or more. Conversation analysis is used to show how a pair of visitors engaged more with the
content of the guide than they would have with audio produced as traditional scripted monologue. Examples
of a variety of engagement types are detailed and a supporting rationale drawing on Goffman’s theory of
‘footing’ is discussed. The approach potentially offers a low cost way for organisations involved in informal
learning to produce flexible in-house audio content for mobile and e-learning, which improves visitor
engagement both with the content and with one another, and leads to a more enjoyable visitor/learner
experience than traditional forms of audio.
1 INTRODUCTION
We describe the development and testing of a
museum audio guide with content recorded as
spontaneous, interactive dialogue between two
curators as they themselves walked around an art
gallery. This contrasts with the traditional content of
such guides, where a script is pre-prepared then
recorded as a monologue. The aim was to increase
the quantity of talk which those visiting the museum
with friends or family would have together, as well
as its ‘quality’, in terms of the engagement it
demonstrated the visitors to be having with the
exhibits and the content of the guide. ‘Engagement’
is taken here to mean a combination of attention,
interest, enjoyment and implied learning (Falk and
Dierking 1992; 2008); (Allen 2002); (Vavoula and
Sharples 2009). The research links to aspects of the
Sotto Voce project (Grinter et al., 2002); Woodruff
et al., 2002); (Woodruff and Aoki, 2004), which
used audio guide content designed to mimic the short
turns of natural conversation with a view to
facilitating interaction within visitor groups. It also
responds to Heath and vom Lehn’s (2004, 2008)
criticisms of the design of museum exhibits and
guides as failing to account for visitor groups’ actual
experience of these institutions; that for people
visiting with others the social and learning
experience are closely linked.
We begin by describing the aims and rationale
for the study and go on to describe how the audio
guide was produced and tested. Using conversation
and discourse analysis, we then detail how
engagement with (and through) the guide was
manifest in visitors’ talk. Goffman’s theories of
footing and participation (Goffman 1974, 1981);
(Levinson, 1988) are then drawn upon to provide an
explanatory framework for the interaction, from
which the study’s findings can be generalised. We
finish by discussing some of the limitations of the
study, pragmatic issues in deploying such audio
guides and the broader implications that emerge for
audio in mobile and other forms of e-learning.
2 THE STUDY
Vom Lehn et al. (2002:15) argue, “Despite the
acknowledgement of the importance of social
interaction for the museum experience, research of
visitor behavior tends to concentrate on the cognitive
aspects and the learning outcomes of museum visits
363
Elliston B. and FitzGerald E..
ENCOURAGING MUSEUM VISITOR ENGAGEMENT USING SPONTANEOUS TALK-IN-INTERACTION AUDIO GUIDES.
DOI: 10.5220/0003889303630372
In Proceedings of the 4th International Conference on Computer Supported Education (CSEDU-2012), pages 363-372
ISBN: 978-989-8565-07-5
Copyright
c
2012 SCITEPRESS (Science and Technology Publications, Lda.)
and pays less attention to the social organization of
communication and collaboration at the ‘exhibit
face.’” It is as part of redressing this balance that this
study can be viewed. Recent research has often
blended analysis of visitors’ spoken interaction with
that of their embodied action (vom Lehn, 2006);
(vom Lehn and Heath 2007); (Heath and von Lehn
2008) with the emphasis increasingly being on
engagement with the exhibit directly (with a view to
improving the design of ‘interactive’ exhibits) rather
than through the mediation of an audio or other type
of guide. However, such research has limitations
when applied to exhibits which in and of themselves
do not provide a learning element; unlike the
interactive exhibits in many museums, a painting in
a gallery, or a tree in a nature reserve lack an
intrinsic ability to teach about themselves, and
require some form of guidance for learning to occur.
This study analysed the engagement visitors
exhibited with the content of an audio guide through
their verbal interactions (Stainton, 2002); (His,
2002; 2008); (vom Lehn and Heath, 2007), (Falk
and Dierking, 2008), (Smith and Tinio, 2008), the
rationale being that ‘on-topic’ interactions naturally
reflect visitors’ engagement with the content of the
museum itself. In assessing spontaneous dialogue
guides (hereafter SDGs), we looked for points in
visitors’ talk which linked directly to the
spontaneous, interactive nature of the talk on the
guide. Such points were taken as instances of
engagement that would not have occurred when
using a scripted monologue guide (hereafter SMG).
3 BUILDING AN AUDIO GUIDE
WITH SPONTANEOUS
DIALOGUE
3.1 Participants and Process
Two knowledgeable volunteers, referred to in this
paper as ‘commentators’, were recruited from the
Museums and Art Galleries Service of Nottingham
City Council in Nottingham, UK. Each selected four
paintings in Nottingham Castle Museum’s Long
Gallery and were recorded walking round and
discussing the paintings together. The commentators
were briefed beforehand that they could (indeed
should) speak about whatever came to mind (facts,
opinions, anecdotes, “anything at all”), but that their
aim was to record a commentary together for a non-
expert first-time visitor. It was stressed that they
could talk as informally and casually as they liked,
were free to interact with one another “as felt
natural” and that they could say as much or as little
as they wished about an exhibit.
3.2 Product
The recording was subsequently divided into
discrete painting-by-painting commentaries varying
in length from 3.54 minutes to 6.24 minutes (mean
4.44 minutes, median 4.41 minutes). Each track was
analysed to identify sequence endings (Schegloff
2007) or points of topic change (Brown and Yule,
1983); (Gardner, 1987) and between three and six
pauses of two seconds each were inserted in each
commentary at such points. This was to enable
listeners to pause the recording without disrupting
the content, but not disrupt the flow of the dialogue
if left running. It is notable that the short sequences
of talk commonly found in states of incipient talk
(Schegloff and Sacks, 1973) are naturally amenable
to this process. No other editing took place. Each
track was labelled with an identifying picture
number and transferred to a handheld device
(Personal Digital Assistant, or PDA) with integrated
speaker to enable participants to hear the audio.
4 TESTING AN AUDIO GUIDE
WITH SPONTANEOUS
DIALOGUE
4.1 Participants and Process
Two adult volunteers were recruited from the
University of Nottingham as example visitors. Both
were occasional (but infrequent) museum visitors
and both had in the past used SMGs. They visited
the Nottingham Castle Museum, were provided with
the PDA audio guide, shown its operation and how
to locate paintings through the track numbering
system. They were briefed that they could listen to
as much or as little of each commentary as they
wished, could pause and restart a commentary at will
and could talk together when and as they chose.
Both participants were given lapel radio
microphones that recorded their spoken
conversations with each other during their visit.
Immediately after the visit, a semi-structured
interview was held in which they were asked for
their impressions of the experience and the content
of the audio, pros and cons of this type of content,
and anything they felt particularly memorable.
CSEDU2012-4thInternationalConferenceonComputerSupportedEducation
364
4.2 Data Analysis
This study aimed to go beyond a user feedback
approach, and to identify engagement primarily from
the language data obtained during in situ use of the
guide. The recording was transcribed into
approximately 7700 words, using notation based on
that of Gail Jefferson (Atkinson and Heritage, 1984).
In the transcription given in this paper, speakers A
and B are the visitors, C and D the two
commentators on the SDG. Extracts are numbered
with the title of the painting under discussion.
Conversation analysis (Schegloff and Sacks, 1973);
(Sacks et al., 1974); (Atkinson and Heritage, 1984),
(Schegloff, 2007) was used to identify visitor talk
structurally dependent on the guide’s content being
spontaneous talk-in-interaction. 4000 transcribed
words from a pair of visitors using a SMG in the
same gallery some years previous (developed for the
CAGE project described below) further informed the
study by identifying certain interactions not
particular to SDGs.
5 RESULTS
5.1 Overview of Visitors’ Talk
Visitors attended to seven of the eight painting
commentaries for their full duration (the eighth
painting they talked off topic over the end of the
commentary). They frequently also engaged in their
own talk before, and during pauses in the
commentary. A limited amount of visitor talk did
overlap talk on the guide, but was always ‘on-topic’;
related to the guide content, the viewing experience,
or comprising visitors’ topical expansions. Talk
accounting for the technicalities of the guide
(agreeing starts, stops and volume changes) were not
counted as on-topic. In related work, the CAGE
project, aimed at encouraging visitor movement
within a museum (Lonsdale et al., 2004; 2005),
(Rudman et al., 2008), had tested a location aware
SMG in the same gallery, audio recording visitors as
they went around the paintings listening to the guide
and talking together. For each painting, the content
of that guide became increasingly ‘detailed’ as it
went along, and finished with a prompt for visitors
to look at similar or related paintings in the same
gallery. Rudman et al., (2008:156) report of the
CAGE project, “One group of participants was
observed to spend over three minutes (a long time to
spend in front of one painting) pointing out details to
each other and discussing what they were hearing”.
The same painting was included in the SDG with
audio commentary lasting six and a half minutes.
Visitors were seen to attend throughout, adding 30
on-topic ‘turns’ of their own talk to the experience,
both before and during listening (turn here refers to
the successive individual contributions that go to
make up a conversation, where the speakers ‘take it
in turns’ to talk).
The number of on-topic turns produced in front
of a painting by visitors using the SDG ranged from
8 to 61 (mean c.35, median 39). In contrast, a very
talkative pair of visitors using the CAGE guide
produced around 14 to 32 turns, in front of the eight
paintings they talked most about (mean c.20 turns,
median 17.5). Furthermore, these turns tended to be
markedly shorter than those produced by the visitors
with the SDG. While suggestive of possible future
results with a larger participant sample, this limited
comparison cannot be claimed to be truly
representative; it is notoriously difficult in any event
to quantify ‘engagement’ in anything more than a
supporting role to more foregrounded qualitative
methods. Moreover, factors such as visitors’ time
pressure, preconceived interests and the precise
chronological point within a visit have hard-to-
observe effects on the kinds of measures used in
quantitative analyses of engagement.
5.2 Visitor Engagement
5.2.1 Multiple Voicing as Engagement
At the beginning of Extract 1, speaker C at lines 1-3,
5, and 7-12 puts forward an argument, but voicing
the assertion as from an ‘unattributed other’ with “I
read about”. The position is weakened (“maybe”,
“kind of”) suggesting a limited alignment of the
speakers ‘personal’ voice with this ‘other’. However
this is seemingly contradicted by the gentle closing
laughter at turn end, signalling at least sympathy
with the idea; incorporating the assertion into her
‘personal’ voice (see Goffman 1974:531 and
Bakhtin 1981, 1986). In response at lines 13-19
speaker D at first makes an agreeing token, then
backtracks qualifying this as “maybe”, then proposes
an alternative rationalised position (a common
progression in spoken interactions) emphasised as
personally voiced (“but I would have thought”) and
finishing with a strong assertion (“must”).
Commentator C’s response in line 22 mirrors the
personal nature of the talk (“I’m”), but as a gentle
contradiction of D’s view. It prompts a strong
overlapped response beginning as double voiced
presentation of the speaker’s voice through the
ENCOURAGINGMUSEUMVISITORENGAGEMENTUSINGSPONTANEOUSTALK-IN-INTERACTIONAUDIO
GUIDES
365
artist’s ‘voice’ (line 23, “or he was fairly sure”).
Line 25 has speaker D shifting into his personal
voice again which is more conciliatory (“I don’t get
the feeling”).
Extract 1: From ‘Homer Singing his Iliad at the Gates of
Athens’.
1 C The other thing that I read about
2 this I don’t know if you’ve
heard 3
this Da[vid but e:rm is that] but=
4 A [quite a nice factoid]
5 C =maybe [Lethiere] was was erm kind
6 B [Mmmmmmmm]
7 C of having a bit of a go at his
8 patrons that he was a a poor
9 neglected artist outside hhhhhh
10 the the gates of Paris maybe and 11
no one appreciated him very much 12
hhhh hhhh
13 D ahhh (.) well y- yeah maybe but I
14 would have thought a painting of
15 this this (.)scale e::r detail and
16 complexity which is a huge amount
17 of work I can’t believe how many
18 hours that took (.) must have been
19 a commission
20 B Mmmmm
21 (1.0)
22 C I’m not [sure ]
23 D [Or he] was fairly sure
24 that he could flog it to someone
25 (1.0) I don’t get the feeling that
26 this is something he did just for
27 himself
28 C Don’t you think so=
29 D =I don’t
This segment highlights how talk-in-interaction
incorporates more ‘voices’, or positions within the
talk than a scripted monologue could hope to, where
it would be unusual to attribute a personalised voice
to the narrator (who claims no ownership, or
responsibility for their talk). While such narration
may state differing opinions and identify their
origins, the subtle shifts and negotiations of voice
whereby position is developed and becomes
established across turns in talk, are missing. The
range of voices in dialogue guides (in addition to the
dialogic relationship visitors find themselves in with
the painting under discussion itself (Bakhtin, 1981))
increase the opportunity for visitors to develop their
own voice through the range of positions they are
offered (representing degrees of opinion and
authority) and on which they can draw, and because
listeners experience the development of ideas, rather
than having established opinions presented in a
‘final formulation’. The interactive elements of
negotiating agreement and disagreement enable
listeners to engage with the process of coming to a
position and thus their ability and willingness to
develop and express their own position.
5.2.2 Engagement through Personalization
In Extract 2, the responses of visitors at lines 6 and 9
to the preceding piece of commentary are significant
in that they show incorporation into visitors’ talk not
only of the informational content of the guide, but
the presentation of that content.
Extract 2: From ‘In Love’.
1 C ((continuing)) putting his bow
2 away (1.0) you’ve got these kind
3 of over ripe apples on the table
4 and falling to the ground
5 B (ye the) apples
6 A She’s [spotted them too]
7 C [and you’ve kind ]of got
8 some brown leaves as well
9 B Yeah (.) sharp
Visitor A’s response, “She’s spotted them too”,
begins with an explicit third person orientation to the
previous speaker as a person rather than ‘presenter’
(Heritage 2007) which reflects the commentators use
of “you’ve got”, linking the visitors with her own
talk. Prior to this extract the visitors had discussed
the apples in the painting, and line 6 references an
informal activity (“spotted”) applied by the visitor
both to his own previous talk and that of the
commentator (“too”). Thus the commentator is
‘personally’ made part of the mutual participation
framework in which the visitors are currently
engaged (namely, following the guide’s
identification of various features in the picture). This
response from the visitors implicitly makes relevant
the voice of the commentator in a way that she is
personally accountable for her words.
Extract 3: From ‘In Love’.
1 C Well I think descriptively we’ve
2 got erm we’ve got a lovely garden
3 it’s er looks like summertime to
4 me (0.5) erm (0.5) a girl who to
5 me looks er
6 A Nowt gets past this lass
A related example (Extract 3) appears earlier in
the dialogue on the same painting, where the
commentator’s self reference (twice in a single turn
saying, “looks to me”) reinforces that she expects
her words to be attributed to her personally. Visitor
A’s response accepts and builds on this framework
by referencing her talk as personal.
CSEDU2012-4thInternationalConferenceonComputerSupportedEducation
366
Such response has a ‘knock on’ effect as
engagement with the guide continues, insofar as
visitors may build up a sense of a commentator’s
‘approach’ or ‘character’. This can itself provide a
resource for comment, as in Extract 4.
Extract 4: From ‘Bistre 2’.
1 A He’s disparaging of anyone who
2 says anything about a painting
3 isn’t he
4 B Yeah
In this case, the visitors go on to expand this
sequence opening to discuss their own reactions to
his view, but its origins remain in an assessment of
the personality of one of the commentators, built up
over the course of the guide speaking about more
than one painting.
5.2.3 Engagement through a Shared
Environment
Extract 5: From ‘The Imposition’.
1 D What’s not often considered
2 looking at paintings is what size
3 they are cos we’ve got some here
4 which are huge it’s not the
5 biggest one we’ve got but this is
6 ye know five [foot square more or
7 less]
8 C [five foot isn’t it
9 yeah]
10 B Mmmmm
11 D Another painting down there (1.0)
12 tiny just a few inches and what
13 determines the decision to do that
14 now it may be where they’re going
15 to hang it if it’s a commission ye
16 know we’ve got a damp patch about
17 so big
18 C Yeah
19 D can you help cover it hh[hh]
20 C [hh]hh=
21 =[hhhhhh]
22 A [hhhhhh]
23 B [hhhhhh]
24 D But why is this absolutely so vast
25 because it must limit (.) unless
26 it’s a commission it must limit
27 what you can do with it
28 B Mmmmmm
29 D it limits who you can sell it to
30 B I think it needs to be that
31 [big though]
32 D [if it’s up] for sale
33 C yeh [I kept asking whether she=
34 A [it wouldn’t have the same=
35 C =did small ones hhhhh]
36 A =impact if it was] small it
In Extract 5, commentator D (lines 1-27) raises the
question of how artists choose the size for a
painting, and does so in a way which generates a
shared participation context by referencing
deictically the immediate physical environment
within which both the commentators and the visitors
experience the painting (“we’ve got some here
which are huge”, “another painting down there tiny
just a few inches”).
In line 24 the commentator poses the question
directly (almost in exasperation), “but why is this
absolutely so vast”. Visitor B (lines 28 and 30) and
A (34 and 36) do offer answers, also drawing on the
shared environment. In this sequence of dialogue the
visitors incorporate the guide as first pair part in
their own dialogue, providing a second pair part and
sequence closing third (Schegloff, 2007) – hence the
‘dialogic’ nature of the guide institutes, both in
terms of content and structure, the talk between the
visitors.
5.2.4 Engagement through Assessment (1)
Extract 6: From ‘Violas’.
1 C ((turn continues)) and I think
2 what I like about it is its
3 simplicity which makes me feel I
4 could live with it I suppose that
5 in in my house I might always find
6 something different in it it’s
7 quite strange
8 (2.0)
9 D Yes well for me it’s one of those
10 paintings that could slide into
11 interior decoration (1.0) there’s
12 there’s plenty in this gallery
13 which I like but I would not want
14 to have every day on my living
15 room wall
16 C Yes
17 D Not that my living room’s big
18 enough for [some of these but=
19 B [It’s a very modern=
20 D =but that one certainly yes]
21 B =( ) ye could see it fitting into]
22 ye
23 D it’s very calm soft shades
24 A Ye toilet
25 D e:rm
26 B Yeah
27 D Very [very attractive ]
28 B [well considering] the art
29 collection of my landlord
Dialogue in guides has the potential to encourage
visitor engagement with paintings which may not in
themselves be immediately ‘interesting’, by having
commentators bounce personal reactions to the work
ENCOURAGINGMUSEUMVISITORENGAGEMENTUSINGSPONTANEOUSTALK-IN-INTERACTIONAUDIO
GUIDES
367
off one another (Extract 6 lines 1-16) and so
implicitly encouraging visitors to do the same, either
by having provided a starting point, or being
suggestive that there is value in such talk. This we
see in the visitors’ responses in lines 19, 21/22, 24,
26 and 28/29.
It appears that when guides successfully engage
in this kind of activity, they are inspiring a particular
topicality in the participation framework built
around a shared experience of ‘standing before a
painting in a gallery and assessing it with a friend’,
and a situation where both visitors and
commentators assume the same role and status.
5.2.5 Engagement through Assessment (2)
Related to the previous two sources of engagement,
first where a question from the commentator projects
an ‘answer’ from the visitors, and second where
assessment talk in the commentary stimulates such
talk in the visitors, is the situation of visitors
responding by directly linking their assessment to
one on the guide, as in Extract 7.
Extract 7: From ‘The Imposition’.
1 C I::: er what I really like about
2 it is the amount of different
3 materials and texture that she
4 uses in it as well as this really
5 kind of dynamic sweep I guess and
6 I really like black and white
7 paintings I don’t know if that
8 says something about my character
9 does it hhhh
10 B Yes she’s right it has got quite a
11 kind of variety of textures in
12 something seemingly quite simple
Visitor B, in response to factual detail (though
couched in terms of an assessment (“What I really
like about this is...”) makes an agreeing assessment
(line 10) which aligns his position with that of the
commentator, not merely in terms of rephrasing the
informational content to agree with ‘the assessment’,
but also explicitly agreeing with the commentator as
an individual (“she’s right”). The visitor responds
here to the way the information is presented as well
as the informational content itself.
Orientation of visitor talk to the speakers on the
guide as well as to one another is also evident in
Extract 8.
Extract 8: From ‘A View of Nottingham from the East’.
1 D Fantastic (0.5) love it
2 B I do like it [as well] actually
3 A [Mmmmmmm]
In response to D’s assessment, visitor B shows
the conventions of turn projection at line 2 not only
by producing his own assessment but also by
orienting it back to D’s turn (“as well”).
Prior to Extract 9 the commentator states that she
likes the painting, but then explains why she thinks
many people don’t. She here employs considerable
equivocation (“I think”, “maybe”) and casual
language (“grubbier”), for a perception she does not
herself share. The visitor’s contrastive response
assessment in line 7 (“I like
”) therefore serves to
contradict these ‘other visitors’, agreeing with the
commentator’s earlier positive personal assessment.
Extract 9: From ‘Violas’.
1 C I think one of the things people
2 maybe don’t like is the white
3 frame which is e::r maybe a bit
4 grubbier than it would have been
5 done (0.5) would have been in in
6 its heyday
7 A I like
the white frame
8 B Yeah
5.2.6 Engagement through Laughter
A further example of engagement with dialogue
guides is visitors responding with and to the shared
laughter on the guide (Extract 10). Coates (2007)
argues collaboratively-negotiated laughter signals
appreciation and amusement, generates mutual
solidarity (through the shared knowledge or
experience needed to appreciate the humour on
which it is based) and also marks the ending of
particular frames of experience.
Extract 10: From ‘Violas’.
1 D Hhh I don’t trust experts
2 C Don’t you hhhh[hhh hhhhhh]
3 D [No hhhhhhh]
4 B [hhhhhhh]
5 A [hhhhhhh]
So laughter becomes a signal of engagement not
only with overt jokes, but with a participation
framework shared by visitors and commentators. See
lines 19-23 from Extract 5 for a further instance.
5.2.7 Engagement through Revoicing
In Extract 11, visitor B’s agreement (line 17) both
reflects the content of the commentator’s assessment
(lines 1/2, 5, 7, 9, 11 and 14/15) and echoes her
words (“quite tempting”). Such ‘revoicing’ (Tannen
1989) shows the natural, spoken language of SDG
talk providing a resource for visitors to incorporate
CSEDU2012-4thInternationalConferenceonComputerSupportedEducation
368
into their own talk, something far less available in
the more formal, written English of a SMG text.
Extract 11: From ‘The Imposition’.
1 C =[painting so I know it’s
2 probably]
3 A =[this if you don’t have an audio
4 guide]
5 C [really]
6 B [ye:ah ]
7 C [quite tempting to try and to]uch
8 A [cos that’s quite interesting]
9 C [this kind of touch]
10 B [there’s the book]
11 C [This kind of textured ( )]
12 A [No one’s gonna look at the book]
13 B I know
14 C but then we have to try and stop
15 people touching aswell
16 (2.0)
17 B And (that’s) right it was is er
18 quite tempting to go and
5.3 User Feedback
A semi-structured interview was conducted with the
test visitors prior to analysis of the language data so
linguistic findings would not interfere with the
interview process. Nevertheless, many of the users’
reflections correspond to the findings discussed
above. Both participants had previous experience
with SMGs and reported “enjoying” the SDG more;
they recalled that in their experience SMG content
rapidly became boring. “Stories” and “natural
language” were expressed as particular positives of
the SDG, and that the commentators “did not know
everything”, so the visitors did not feel obliged to
accept their interpretation. They also reported that
the guide “informed your understanding” and that
they felt they and the commentators were “having
the same kind of conversation”. Certain
‘coincidental’ talk (not described above) was said to
be particularly memorable, one example being when
a visitor, looking at a painting stated “I could have
painted that”, which was moments later echoed by
one of the commentators on the guide. Visitors also
liked when, “what the guide was saying and what
we’d said came together or contradicted each other”.
They noted the humour, and when the guide enabled
them to change or develop their own opinion. Both
respondents offered the view that if they had used
the guide on their own it would probably also have
been more engaging and that they might have been
“sucked into” the conversation on the guide.
6 DISCUSSION
6.1 Theoretical Explanation
The role of assessment as a key feature in
encouraging engagement has been shown here to
emerge as much from the dialogic form of the
assessment as from the actual content. In talk-in-
interaction, assessment is negotiated and acquires
meaning across turns, and it is this which creates
engagement. The response to assessment in audio
guides can be further explored with reference to
Pomerantz’ (1984:62) “speaker’s procedural rule”
whereby the recipient of an assessment turn of talk
has an orientation to respond to that assessment (see
also Heritage and Raymond, 2005). The conjecture,
equivocation and qualification with which
commentators interweave their assessments has both
a ‘micro’ effect on listeners’ reaction to a particular
assertion and a ‘macro’ effect on their reaction to the
overall authority of the guide; it ‘demands’ closer
attention, increasing the likelihood of the listener’s
responding to what they hear actively rather than
passively.
It is important that the engagement described is
understood as responsive behaviour which can be
generalised beyond this particular study. Goffman’s
work on ‘footing’ (Goffman, 1974; 1981 and
Levinson, 1988) provides an explanatory framework
for the nature of the spoken interactions described.
For Goffman (1981:128), ‘footing’ constitutes “the
alignment we take up to ourselves and the others
present as expressed in the way we manage the
production or reception of an utterance”. It is the
negotiation of footing across the two conversations
(the experts’ and the visitors’) that shows us the
particular engagement visitors have with a
spontaneous dialogue guide. Levinson (1988)
describes how, footing is founded on deixis; the way
language ‘points’, indexes and references, and
through which we create and demonstrate our
orientation to the people and context in which we
find ourselves. It is through deixis (for example the
way a visitor aligns themselves with a
commentator’s assessment, references a
commentator as “she” rather than “it”, or engages in
laughter based on a shared referential understanding)
that relevant, on-topic responses constitute
engagement.
Goffman describes group talk as comprising a
single addressed recipient but also ratified
unaddressed recipients; those not ‘directly
addressed but who have the possibility of becoming
next speaker. Visitors listening to a guide cannot
ENCOURAGINGMUSEUMVISITORENGAGEMENTUSINGSPONTANEOUSTALK-IN-INTERACTIONAUDIO
GUIDES
369
produce the next turn in that dialogue, but can and
do speak a next turn to their co-visitor. In doing this
however, they must draw on and re-relate the footing
of the guide conversation in ways such as those
described. This means the audio guide users do not
adopt a typical audience role. Goffman (1974:540)
suggests that in theatrical contexts words are spoken
for not to an audience, “appreciation not action is
their appropriate response”. This also applies to
experiences such as viewing a TV talk show or
listening to a lawyer/witness exchange as a juror.
These contexts differ fundamentally from those
where verbal response is being encouraged; where
action not appreciation is the appropriate response.
It might be suggested that engagement as it is
considered in this study will rely heavily on a
personal chemistry between the commentators. This
too would benefit from further investigation, and
empathy and affiliation may play a role. However, it
is argued here that it is structural features of
interaction that are most significant. We have
highlighted laughter for example as a function of
discourse as well as a response to humour. Unless
there is a positive dislike between the commentators,
their engagement in talk-in-interaction should
necessarily produce the structures, devices and
organisation identified here as key to encouraging
listener engagement. We therefore offer
explanations for why and how visitors have engaged
with a guide, supported by evidence from user
feedback.
6.2 Issues of Wider Deployment
Spontaneous dialogue guides (SDGs) potentially
offer a cheap, easy and flexible approach to
providing effective audio guidance for visitors to
museums, galleries, heritage centres, nature reserves
and other contexts of informal learning. However it
remains unclear how far one of their ‘strengths’ -
drawing on the shared, physical experience of
viewing an exhibit - might be changed if deployed
beyond this space, for example on a website. This
deserves further study as one particularly attractive
application of SDGs is precisely this area.
A potential limitation lies in using SDG audio as
a ‘minor partner’ in a multimedia context (Tellis
2004), or with technologies integrating the visiting
experience with post visit web based interactions
(Hsi 2002, 2003); i.e. where audio must be designed
to accommodate the needs of the other media. SDG
talk may be better exploited where the technology is
assembled to support its strengths.
This might include the ability for users to stop
and start the audio by voice actuation - the guide
pausing upon detecting visitor talk. The value of this
is supported by observations that users needed to
negotiate pressing the pause button with their co-
visitors, which proved disruptive to the listening.
Such technology would also facilitate the sequential
integration of visitor and guide talk since they both
follow that inherent in incipient talk.
A further mundane, but potentially significant
practical consideration is that the addition of
assessment talk within SDG commentaries makes
them relatively lengthy; in the CAGE project SMGs
were considerably shorter than the SDGs produced
here. It is likely that visitors will not wish to stand
and listen for too long, so this should be an
important consideration in designing the audio
tracks. ‘Broadcastr’ (www.broadcastr.com), a
mobile phone application designed to deliver user-
generated location-based story-telling, deliberately
limits the size (and hence length) of audio clips that
can be uploaded onto its site – they consider 3
minutes to be the optimal length of time for someone
to want to listen to an audio clip.
This small study has only been able to consider
the guide from the perspective of adults within small
visitor groups. Heath et al., (2002) discuss adult
visitor interactions with strangers in museums,
which change visitors’ relations with audio guides
(and exhibits) for, as Benford (2008) stresses, this
fundamentally alters the participation framework.
McManus (1987) describes how families with
children spend longer discussing exhibits but less
time engaging with guides, while vom Lehn (2006)
describes the importance for children of “bodily
enactment” with exhibits (see also Blud, 1990 and
Hein, 1998). Nor has this study considered student
groups, which constitute a major field within
museum guide research because of their blend of
formal and informal learning (Tselios et al., 2008);
(Reynolds et al., 2009); (Sung et al., 2010).
7 CONCLUSIONS AND FUTURE
WORK
This project was designed to test the viability of
creating museum audio guides using experts’
spontaneously generated dialogue in context. It was
hypothesised that creating audio this way would
engage listeners with learning content through
discourse features within it. The ways in which such
engagement did occur have been surprisingly
CSEDU2012-4thInternationalConferenceonComputerSupportedEducation
370
effective in increasing visitors’ spoken interactions
around the content of the guide, and their reported
enjoyment of the experience. Further investigation
beyond this initial study has much to show us, and a
larger corpus of more varied visitors will provide
further insights and increase our understanding of
how such guides achieve or fail to achieve their
goals. More varied learning contexts with different
exhibit types and client demographics, such as
natural history and science museums, zoos, botanical
gardens, or school and college field trip locations
must also be considered. It would also be
informative to investigate the functioning of a
spontaneous monologue guide, and to begin
explication of how commentators are producing
their talk, including more detail of the negotiation
process and the way the audience is conceptualised
and accounted for. With this data it may then
become possible to manipulate and ‘design’ the
production process more effectively and enable the
content to be delivered in collaboration with other
media, informing both the theory of talk-in-
interaction and the practice of automated verbal
information delivery.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
Thanks to Nottingham City Council Museums and
Art Galleries Service, volunteers in the user trial and
Peter Lonsdale from the CAGE Project. Thanks also
to Mike Sharples and Svenja Adolphs for their
invaluable assistance and feedback. Brian Elliston is
supported by the Horizon Doctoral Training Centre
at the University of Nottingham (RCUK Grant No.
EP/G037574/1).
REFERENCES
Allen, S., 2002. Looking for Learning in Visitor Talk: A
Methodological Exploration. In G. Leinhardt, K.
Crowley and K. Knutson (eds.), Learning
Conversations in Museums. Mahwah: Lawrence
Erlbaum, pp. 259-303.
Atkinson, J. M. and Heritage, J. (eds.), 1984. Structures of
Social Action: Studies in Conversation Analysis.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bakhtin, M. M., 1981. Discourse in the Novel. In M. M.
Bakhtin, The Dialogic Imagination: Four Essays.
Austin: University of Texas Press.
Bakhtin, M. M., 1986. The Problem of Speech Genres. In
M. M. Bakhtin, Speech Genres and Other Late Essays.
Austin: University of Texas Press.
Benford, S., 2008. Location Based technologies for
Learning: November 2008. Becta. Available from
http://dera.ioe.ac.uk/1505/1/becta_2009_emergingtech
nologies_locationtech_report.pdf [last accessed 29th
October 2011].
Blud, L. M., 1990. Social Interaction and Learning Among
Family Groups Visiting a Museum. Museum
Managament and Curatorship, 9, pp. 43-51.
Brown, G. and Yule, G., 1983. Discourse Analysis.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Coates, J., 2007. Talk in a Play Frame: More on Laughter
and Intimacy. Journal of Pragmatics, 39, pp. 29-49.
Falk, J. H. and Dierking, L. D., 1992. The Museum
Experience. Washington: Whalesback.
Falk, J. H. and Dierking, L. D., 2008. Enhancing Visitor
Interaction and Learning with Mobile Technologies. In
L. Tallon and K. Walker (eds.), Digital Technologies
and the Museum Experience: Handheld Guides and
Other Media. Lanham: AltaMira, pp. 19-34.
Gardner, R., 1987. The Identification and Role of Topic in
Spoken Interaction. Semiotica, 65(1/2), pp. 129-141.
Goffman, E., 1974. Frame Analysis. Boston: Northeastern
University Press.
Goffman, E., 1981. Forms of Talk. Philadelphia:
University of Philadelphia.
Grinter, R. E., Aoki, P. M., Hurst, A., Szymanski, H.,
Thornton, J. D. and Woodruff, A., 2002. Revisiting the
Visit: Understanding How Technology Can Shape the
Museum Visit. In Proc. ACM Conf. CSCW ’02,
NewOrleans, USA. New York: ACM.
Heath, C., Luff, P. K., vom Lehn, D., Hindmarsh, J. and
Cleverly, J., 2002. Crafting Participation: Designing
Ecologies, Configuring Experience. Visual
Communication, 1(1), pp. 9-34.
Heath, C. and vom Lehn, D, 2004. Configuring Reception:
(Dis-)Regarding the “Spectator” in Museums and
Galleries. Theory, Culture and Society, 21(6), pp. 43-
65.
Heath, C. and vom Lehn, D., 2008. Configuring
“Interactivity”: Enhancing Engagement in Science
Centres and Museums. Social Studies of Science,
38(1), pp. 63-91.
Hein, G. E., 1998. Learning in the Museum. London:
Routledge.
Heritage, J., 2007. Intersubjectivity and Progessivity in
Person (and Place) Reference. In T. Stivers and N. J.
Enfield (eds.), Person Reference in Interaction:
Linguistic, Cultural and Social Perspectives.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 255-280.
Heritage, J. and Raymond, G., 2005. The Terms of
Agreement: Indexing Epistemic Authority and
Subordination in Talk-in-interaction. Social
Psychology Quarterly, 68(1), pp. 15-38.
Hsi, S., 2002. The Electronic Guidebook: A study of User
Experiences Using Mobile Web Content in a Museum
Setting. In Proc. IEEE Intl. Workshop on Wireless and
Mobile Techniologies in Education (WMTE), Vaxjo,
Sweden.
ENCOURAGINGMUSEUMVISITORENGAGEMENTUSINGSPONTANEOUSTALK-IN-INTERACTIONAUDIO
GUIDES
371
Hsi, S., 2003. A Study of User Experiences Mediated by
Nomadic Web Content in a Museum. Journal of
Computer Assisted Learning, 19, pp. 308-319.
Hsi, S., 2008. Designing for Mobile Visitor Engagement.
In L. Tallon and K. Walker (eds.), Digital
Technologies and the Museum Experience: Handheld
Guides and Other Media. Lanham: AltaMira, pp. 125-
146.
Levinson, S. C., 1988. Putting Linguistics on a Proper
Footing: Explorations in Goffman’s Concepts of
Participation. In P. Drew and A. J. Wootton (eds.),
Erving Goffman: Exploring the Interaction Order.
Cambridge: Polity Press, pp. 161-227.
Lonsdale, P., Baber, C., Sharples, M., Byrne, W.,
Arvanitis, T. N., Brundell, P. and Beale, R., 2004.
Context Awareness for MOBIlearn: Creating an
Engaging Learning Experience in an Art Museum. In
Proc. MLearn 2004: Learning Anytime, Everywhere,
Rome, Italy. Learning and Skills Development Agency
(LSDA), pp. 115-118.
Lonsdale, P., Beale, R. and Byrne, W., 2005. Using
Context Awareness to Enhance Visitor Engagement in
a Gallery Space. In Proc. People and Computers XIX:
the Bigger Picture, HCI (BCS Conference). London:
Springer.
McManus, P., 1987. It’s the Company You Keep ... The
Social Determination of Learning-Related Behavior in
a Science Museum. International Journal of Museum
Management and Curatorship, 6, pp. 263-270.
Pomerantz, A., 1984. Agreeing and Disagreeing with
Assessments: Some Features of Preferred/Dispreferred
Turn Shapes. In J. M. Atkinson and J. Heritage (eds.),
Structures of Social Action: Studies in Conversation
Analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp.
57-101.
Reynolds, R., Speight, C. and Walker, K., 2009. Bridging
Formal and Informal Learning Using Mobile Digital
Museum Trails. 3
rd
WLE Mobile Learning
Symposeum: Mobile Learning Cultures across
Education, Work and Leisure, London, UK, 27 March
2009.
Rudman, P., Sharples, M., Lonsdale, P., Vavoula, G. and
Meek, J., 2008. Cross-Context Learning. In L. Tallon
and K. Walker (eds.), Digital Technologies and the
Museum Experience: Handheld Guides and Other
Media. Lanham: AltaMira, pp. 147-166.
Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A. and Jefferson, G., 1974. A
Simplest Systematics for the Orgainzation of Turn-
Taking for Conversation. Language, 50(4), pp. 696-
735.
Schegloff, E. A., 2007. Sequence Organization in
Interaction: A Primer in Conversation Analysis 1.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Schegloff, E. A. and Sacks, H., 1973. Opening Up
Closings. Semiotica 8, pp. 289-327.
Smith, J. K. and Tinio, P. P. L., 2008. Audibly Engaged:
Talking the Walk. In L. Tallon and K. Walker (eds.),
Digital Technologies and the Museum Experience:
Handheld Guides and Other Media. Lanham:
AltaMira, pp. 63-78.
Stainton, C., 2002. Voices and Images: Making
Connections Between Identity and Art. In G.
Leinhardt, K. Crowley and K. Knutson (eds.),
Learning Conversations in Museums. Mahwah:
Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 213-257.
Sung, Y.-T., Chang, K.-E., Hou, H.-T. and Chen, P.-F.,
2010. Designing an Electronic Guidebook for
Learning Engagement in a Museum of History.
Computers in Human Behavior, 26, pp. 74-83.
Tannen, D., 1989. Talking Voices: Repetition, Dialogue
and Imagery in Conversational Discourse.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Tellis, C., 2004. Multimedia Handhelds: One Device,
Many Audiences. In Proc. Museums and the Web.
Available from http://www/archimuse.com/mw2004/
papers/tellis/tellis.html [last accessed 29th October
2011].
Tselios, N., Papadimitriou, I., Raptis, D., Yiannoutsou, N.,
Komis, V. and Avouris, N., 2008. Design for Mobile
Learning in Museums. In J. Lumsden (ed.), User
Interface Design and Evaluation for Mobile
Technology. London: IGI, pp. 253-269.
Vavoula, G. and Sharples, M., 2009. Meeting the
Challenges in Evaluating Mobile Learning.
International Journal of Mobile and Blended
Learning, 1(2), pp. 54-75.
vom Lehn, D., 2006. Embodying Experience: a Video-
Based Examination of Visitors’ Conduct and
Interaction in Museums. European Journal of
Marketing, 40(11/12), pp. 1340-1359.
vom Lehn, D. and Heath, C., 2007. Social Interaction in
Museums and Galleries: A Note on Video-Based Field
Studies. In R. Goldman, R. Pea, B. Barron and S.
Derry (eds.), Video Research in the Learning Sciences.
Mahwah/London: LEA, pp. 287-301.
vom Lehn, D., Heath, C. and Hindmarsh, J., 2002. Video
Based Field Studies in Museums and Galleries. Visitor
Studies Today!, V(III), pp. 15-17, 20-23.
Woodruff, A. and Aoki, P. M., 2004. Conversation
Analysis and the User Experience. Digital Creativity,
15(4), pp. 232-238.
Woodruff, A., Aoki, P., Grinter, R., Hurst, A., Szymanski,
M. and Thornton, J., 2002. Eavesdropping on
Electronic Guidebooks: Observing Learning
Reseources in Shared Listening Environments. In
Proc. 6
th
International Conference on Museums and
the Web, Boston, USA, April 2002. Boston: Archives
and Museum Informatics, pp. 21-30.
CSEDU2012-4thInternationalConferenceonComputerSupportedEducation
372