BIOFEEDBACK SYSTEMS FOR STRESS REDUCTION
Towards a Bright Future for a Revitalized Field
Egon L. van den Broek
1,2,3
and Joyce H. D. M. Westerink
4
1
TNO Technical Sciences, P.O. Box 5050, 2600 GB Delft, The Netherlands
2
Human-Media Interaction (HMI), University of Twente, P.O. Box 217, 7500 AE Enschede, The Netherlands
3
Karakter University Center, Radboud University Medical Center Nijmegen, P.O. Box 9101,
6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlands
4
Philips Corporate Technologies, Research, Brain, Body & Behavior Group, High Tech Campus 34,
5656 AE Eindhoven, The Netherlands
Keywords:
Biofeedback, Neurofeedback, Stress, Mental healthcare, Biosignals, Closed loop systems.
Abstract:
Stress has recently been baptized as the black death of the 21st century, which illustrates its threat to current
health standards. This article proposes biofeedback systems as a means to reduce stress. A concise state-of-
the-art introduction on biofeedback systems is given. The field of mental health informatics is introduced. A
compact state-of-the-art introduction on stress (reduction) is provided. A pragmatic solution for the pressing
societal problem of illness due to chronic stress is provided in terms of closed loop biofeedback systems. A
concise set of such biofeedback systems for stress reduction is presented. We end with the identification of
several development phases and ethical concerns.
1 INTRODUCTION
Throughout its existence, biofeedback has been crit-
icized almost continuously (Moss and Gunkelman,
2002; Moss et al., 2004). The latest boost of criticism
and a response on it dates from around the change of
the century. Amongst many other issues of criticism,
the lack of proven efficacy, the absence of standards,
and the fuzzy relation between biosignals and psycho-
logical constructs were mentioned. Therefore, to es-
tablish a solid ground for the current article, we will
define the core concepts (i.e., biosignals and biofeed-
back) at hand and denote their relations.
Physiological or biosignals can be conceptualized
as (bio)electrical signals recorded on the surface of
the body. These bio(electrical) signals are related to
ionic transport that arises as a result of electrochemi-
cal activity of cells in specialized tissue (e.g., the ner-
vous system), so-called autonomic responses. This
results in (changes in) electric currents produced by
the sum of electrical potential differences across the
tissue. This process is the same regardless of where
in the body the cells are located (e.g., the heart, mus-
cles, skin, or the brain) (S¨ornmo and Laguna, 2005).
So, biosignals reflect the physiological activity of a
person. This latter definition also includes both non-
electrical biosignals, such as skin temperature, and
signals obtained through invasive recording tech-
niques.
Biosignals are employed to enable biofeedback,
as the name already reveals. On May 18, 2008,
the Association for Applied Psychophysiology and
Biofeedback (AAPB), the Biofeedback Certification
International Alliance (BCIA), and the International
Society for Neurofeedback and Research (ISNR)
1
jointly agreed on a standard definition of biofeedback:
Biofeedback is a process that enables an individual
to learn how to change physiological activity for
the purposes of improving health and performance.
Precise instruments measure physiological activity
such as brainwaves, heart function, breathing, muscle
activity, and skin temperature. These instruments
rapidly and accurately “feed back” information to
the user. The presentation of this information – often
in conjunction with changes in thinking, emotions,
and behavior supports desired physiological
changes. Over time, these changes can endure
without continued use of an instrument.
1
For more information, see: http://www.aapb.org/,
http://www.bcia.org/, and http://www.isnr.org/.
499
L. van den Broek E. and H. D. M. Westerink J..
BIOFEEDBACK SYSTEMS FOR STRESS REDUCTION - Towards a Bright Future for a Revitalized Field.
DOI: 10.5220/0003894904990504
In Proceedings of the International Conference on Health Informatics (BSSS-2012), pages 499-504
ISBN: 978-989-8425-88-1
Copyright
c
2012 SCITEPRESS (Science and Technology Publications, Lda.)
This was one of the results of a joint task force,
initiated by the AAPB and the ISNR in 2001 (Moss
and Gunkelman, 2002; Moss et al., 2004). Other re-
sults included a series of white papers and a review
of the clinical efficacy of biofeedback (Moss et al.,
2004). The definitions of both biosignals and biofeed-
back provide the premises for this article. To also un-
derstand the criticism biofeedback has been subjected
to, we need to go back in time, to the invention of
biofeedback, and put the work on biofeedback into
historical perspective.
The origin of biofeedback takes us back to 1932,
the year in which Johannes Heinrich Schultz (1884
1970) published a book on a relaxation technique
he baptized autogenic training. In 2003, the twenti-
eth edition of this book was published, which marks
its continuing influence (Schultz, 2003). However,
Schultz did not provide direct biofeedback, his tech-
nique relies on introspection, and no signals are fed
back to the user. It took more than 25 years un-
til biofeedback, as has just been defined, was re-
ported (Mandler et al., 1958). They referred to it as
autonomic feedback, which they defined as: the re-
lationship between autonomic response and the sub-
ject’s reported perception of such response-induced
stimulation (Mandler et al., 1958, p. 367).
The work of (Mandler et al., 1958) was conducted
more than half a century ago, in the early years of
computing machinery. At that time, computers were
invented for highly trained operators, to help them
do massive numbers of calculations (Sifakis, 2011).
However, much has changed since then; nowadays,
everybody uses them in one of their many guises.
Today we are in touch with various types of com-
puters throughout our normal daily lives, including
our smartphones (Agrawal, 2011). Computation is
on track to become even smaller and more perva-
sive. Not only computing machinery miniaturized,
biomedical apparatus has also done so (Ouwerkerk
et al., 2008). Consequently, biosignals (or physio-
logical signals) still receive increasing interest as an
interface between users and their computing devices.
It is envisioned that computers will become a window
to the world as a whole, to our social life, and even to
ourselves (Davies, 2011).
Computers are slowly becoming dressed, hug-
gable, and tangible. Concepts such as stress and
emotions, which were originally the playing field of
philosophers, sociologists, and psychologists (Izard
et al., 2010), have already become entangled in com-
puters (and in computer science) as well. With this,
it has become much easier for us than before to ac-
cept biosignals as relevant reflections of our lives, and
biofeedback as a means to alter them. Also biofeed-
back standards can emerge more easily with ICT help.
Thus two of the traditional criticisms of biofeedback
are on the verge of being tackled. And while the
embedding of biofeedback into psychological con-
structs remains unclear, the first efficacy evaluations
of biofeedback have started to prove its usefulness
(Gruzelier et al., 2006).
In the next section, we will provide a concise in-
troduction on mental health informatics, as opposed
to general (physical) health informatics, with an em-
phasis on (chronic) stress. In Section 3, we will pro-
vide a concise overview of our working model for
stress reduction: a closed loop biofeedback model.
Last, in Section 4, we will provide a general discus-
sion.
2 MENTAL HEALTH
INFORMATICS AND STRESS
(REDUCTION)
In 1935, Flanders Dunbar noted that the Scientific
study of emotion and of the bodily changes that ac-
company diverse emotional experiences marks a new
era in medicine (Dunbar, 1954, p. vii). We know
now that many physiological processes that are of
profound significance for health can be influenced by
way of emotions (Kaklauskas et al., 2011). For ex-
ample, it has been shown that emotions influence our
cardiovascular system and, consequently, can shorten
or prolong life (Lucas et al., 2009). Moreover,chronic
stress also plays an important role with chronic dis-
eases (Berg and Upchurch, 2007), cancer (e.g., coping
strategies) (Taylor and Stanton, 2007), and rehabilita-
tion (Novak et al., 2010), to mention three.
Flanders Dunbar drew the conclusion: In this
knowledge, we have the key to many problems in
the prevention and treatment of illness, yet we are
scarcely begun to use what we know. We lack perspec-
tive, concerning our knowledge in this field, and are
confused in our concepts of the interrelationship of
psychic, including emotional, and somatic processes
in health and disease. (Dunbar, 1954, p. vii). Nev-
ertheless, emotions remained rather spiritual and hu-
man’s health has usually been explained in physical
(e.g., injuries) and physiological terms (e.g., bacte-
ria and viruses). The field of biofeedback also suf-
fered severely from this attitude. It is only since
the last decades that it has generally been acknowl-
edged that emotions have their impact on health and
illness (Kaklauskas et al., 2011).
Now emotions have been acknowledged by tradi-
tional medicine, stress is being given a position in he-
HEALTHINF 2012 - International Conference on Health Informatics
500
alth informatics. This shift was accelerated by the
general increase in the need for health informatics that
has emerged due to the massive growth of the market
for new systems that improve productivity, cut costs,
and support the transition of health care from hospi-
tal to the home (Dumaij and Tijssen, 2011). Health
informatics is already or will soon be applied for the
support/assistance of independent living, chronic dis-
ease management, facilitation of social support, and
to bring the doctor’s office to people’s homes. Par ex-
cellence, this is where informatics and
mental health
care
blend together.
Recently, stress has been baptized as being the
black death of the 21st century. Although this is a very
bold statement, it illustrates that (chronic) stress is an
important threat to modern societies, both in terms of
severity and in terms of proportion. As such, it is now
receiving top priority in health care and is the #1 pri-
ority in mental health care.
Humans can make cognitive representations of
events, from the past as well as for the future. This
ability distinguishes them from (most) animals. These
representations aid our daily work and living; how-
ever, they also have their down side. In stressful life
events, cognitive representations can facilitate worry-
ing and, consequently, catalyze chronic stress, which
is unknown to animal species (Brosschot, 2010).
When stress is experienced, often similar phys-
iological responses emerge as during the stressful
events from which it originates: the repetition of
such physiological responses can cause pervasive and
structural chemical imbalances in people’s physiolog-
ical systems, including their autonomic and central
nervous system, their neuroendocrine system, their
immune system, and even in their brain (Brosschot,
2010). A thorough understanding of stress is still
missing. This can be explained by the complexity of
human’s physiological systems, their continuous in-
teraction, and their integral dynamic nature.
Current day treatments of stress focuses on the
treatment of either cognitive representations, our (un-
conscious) habit memory system, or both (Schwabe
et al., 2010). In general, under stressful events, the
habit memory system tends to dominate over the cog-
nitive memory (or representations) system; however,
their precise relation remains unknown (Schwabe
et al., 2010). This lack of understanding makes treat-
ment inherently complex and requires a very high
level of expertise from the clinician. Moreover, most
indicators of the patients’ progress rely on behavior
measures and the clinician’s expertise.
A possible alternative for the clinicians expertise
is the use of biofeedback systems for stress reduc-
tion. On the one hand, these systems have the obvious
influencing
algorithm
signal processing +
pattern recognition
Human
biosensors
(bio)feedback
actuators
machine
feedback
biosignals
Figure 1: The (general) biofeedback closed loop model. For
details on the model’s signal processing + pattern recogni-
tion component, we refer to (van den Broek et al., 2010).
Within the scope of this article, the model’s domain of ap-
plication is mental health care, in particular stress reduction.
disadvantages that they can be obtrusive, can process
only a few or even a single signal, and their means
to interact with their user are severely limited as well.
On the other hand, biofeedback systems are becom-
ing reliable and robust, affordable, and can be applied
when and where ever needed or appreciated (without
any additional costs). Moreover, biofeedback systems
for stress reduction enable an anonymous treatment
and can be (automatically) personalized. These two
aspects could be potential strengths of such systems
but could also turn out to be their weaknesses.
Taken together, both the pros and cons of biofeed-
back systems for stress reduction are strong. More-
over, stress is a complex phenomenon, which is far
from completely unraveled. Hence, these limitations
need to be respected and the claims surrounding them
should be chosen with care. For example, given the
current state of the art of science and engineering,
biofeedback systems for stress reduction might only
be employed successfully with people who do not yet
suffer severely (or already for a vast amount of time)
from stress. However, even then, biofeedback sys-
tems for stress reduction can still be useful for a large
portion and for a still increasing part of the popula-
tion.
3 THE CLOSED LOOP
To enable autonomous biofeedback systems, a closed
loop model has to be adopted, incorporating both
measurement and feedback components. Such a
BIOFEEDBACK SYSTEMS FOR STRESS REDUCTION - Towards a Bright Future for a Revitalized Field
501
model can, but does not necessarily involve the in-
tervention of a therapist. Consequently, (closed loop)
biofeedback systems can be positioned in the large
market of health care-related consumer electronics.
For over a century, closed loop models have
been known in science and engineering, in particu-
lar in control theory and electronics (Neamen, 2010).
Closed loop models can be defined as control systems
with an active feedback loop. This loop allows the
control unit to dynamically compensate for deviations
in the system.
The output of the system is fed back through a
sensor measurement to a control unit, which takes the
error between a reference and the output to change the
inputs to the system under control.
A relatively new class of closed loop models are
biofeedback systems: closed loops that take a human
into the loop; see also Figure 1. The descriptions of
these biofeedback systems target various areas but are
essentially the same, comprising: sensors, processing,
influencing algorithm (feedback decision), and actua-
tors. In essence, biofeedback systems for stress re-
duction are described by four basic steps:
1. Sensing. Data collection starts at the sensors,
where a raw signal is generated that contains an
indication of a person’s mental state, e.g. his
stress level. Relevant signals can include both
overt and covert bodily signals, such as facial
camera recordings, movements, speech samples,
and biosignals.
2. Signal Processing + Pattern Recognition. Ex-
ploiting signal features that could contain stress
level information; for example, the number of
peaks in the ElectroDermal Activity (EDA) signal
can be counted, serving as a measure for stress.
For more information on this step, we refer to
(van den Broek et al., 2010).
3. Influencing Algorithm. Given the obtained af-
fective state of the user, a decision is made as to
what feedback to provide to the user. Next, we
will provide various examples of this.
4. Feedback Actuators. The feedback is provided
by a set of actuators. Such actuators can di-
rectly communicate with our body, either physi-
cally (Hatzfeld et al., 2010) or chemically (Mielle
et al., 2010). Alternatively, actuators can commu-
nicate indirectly and influence our environment as
we sense it either consciously or unconsciously;
for instance, a song can be played or lighting can
be activated to create a certain ambiance. The op-
timal way to present this feedback information is
part of the field of Human-Computer Interaction.
The loop (always) closes with a new measurement of
the sensors, which is again evaluated as to whether
or not the intended level of stress has indeed been
reached. If the intended level of stress has indeed
been reached, the system will perform no further ac-
tion. Thus the system guides the user towards a cer-
tain state. Also in the field of Brain-Computer Inter-
action (BCI) closed loops play an important role (van
Gerven et al., 2009). For traditional BCI devices (e.g.,
those allowing locked-in patients to communicate),
however, there is no target state included in a feed-
back algorithm, but instead it is the user who guides
the system to a certain state.
The feedback is determined by biosignals but is
most often of another modality itself. With the vast
progress of informatics, omnipresent computing (also
known as Ambient Intelligence (AmI), Ubiquitous
Computing (UbiComp), the Internet of Things, and
the World Wide Wisdom Web (W4) (Friedewald and
Raabe, 2011) are about to enter our lives. This pro-
vides a seemingly infinite number of possibilities to
provide the (bio)feedback. We will mention three of
them:
1. (Ambient) Lighting: (i) The color of light can
be altered to provide the experience of another
color to people, which can either activate or re-
lax them (K¨uller et al., 2009) and (ii) The Ratio-
nalizer concept: an EDA-based emotion sensing
wristband that offers stress level feedback to stock
traders, using a LED bowl (Ouwerkerk, 2011).
2. Audio and Video: (i) The RelaxTV concept uses
a biosensor to monitor the relaxation of a person.
Biofeedback techniques were developed to use
breathing guidance for deep relaxation of a televi-
sion viewer (Ouwerkerk, 2011) and (ii) A person-
alized affective music player to augment music
experience and direct the listener’s mood (Janssen
et al., 2012), for instance to a relaxed state.
3. Tactile Feedback: (i) Research is conducted with
tactile sensation as emotion elicitor, by means of
an augmented PC mouse, which can be conve-
niently used in the context of work occupational
stress (Suk et al., 2009) and (ii) (Plasier, 2011)
introduced an electronic singing bowl. Tradi-
tional Tibetan versions of these bowls are used to
provide relaxing body vibrations along with the
sounds.
This triplet illustrates the latest developments on
biofeedback. However, many more have been pro-
posed and even more yet are currently being investi-
gated. It can be expected that the field will mature
further and the systems will become robust. Then,
within 10 years from now, consumers can buy such
biofeedback systems in their electronics stores.
HEALTHINF 2012 - International Conference on Health Informatics
502
4 DISCUSSION
This article proposed biofeedback systems as a means
to reduce stress, which is becoming a pregnant is-
sue in our modern society. First, the necessary ba-
sic information was provided in terms of a definition
and a sketch of the origin of biofeedback systems.
Next, the field of mental health informatics was intro-
duced followed by the article’s topic: stress (reduc-
tion). (Closed loop) biofeedback systems were intro-
duced as a feasible solution for the pressing societal
problem of illness due to chronic stress. Moreover,
several examples of biofeedback systems for stress re-
duction were provided.
Traditionally, research towards biofeedback sys-
tems has been approached from a range of sciences
(e.g., psychology, medicine, and computer science)
and often the research explores the feasibility of such
systems and has not yet actually implemented them.
However, the closed-loop model allows us to identify
the three main phases of (subsequent) development:
1. Computational modeling founded on theory, with-
out experimental validation. Such systems have
been proposed by themselves; however, in the per-
spective of biofeedback systems, such models can
be considered as the very first phase of the actual
development.
2. Stress elicitation and measurement, with or
without classification component. This type
of research is conducted in three environ-
ments (Healey, 2008): i) controlled laboratory re-
search; ii) semi-controlled research (e.g., as in
smart homes); and iii) ambulatory research.
3. Development of the actual biofeedback systems,
in which one can distinguish: i) the initial off-line
modeling and ii) online, real-time modeling.
This division is not as strict as it may appear; of-
ten mixtures of these three phases of development
are employed and iterations and loops are applied.
Nevertheless, it should be noted that, so far, a vast
amount of research on applied biofeedback for stress
reduction has not implemented the required closed
loop model (Janssen et al., 2012). Instead most stud-
ies present either theoretical computational modeling
or solely stress elicitation and measurement. More-
over, most research has been conducted in (semi-)
controlled settings. And even though ambulatory re-
search with loose constraints, conducted in the real
world, is still relatively rare (Healey, 2008) (cq.
(Janssen et al., 2012)), we do expect a rise of closed
loop biofeedback systems for stress reduction.
Building biofeedback systems for stress reduction
is a process in which law and ethics will claim their
place too (Floridi, 2010). Law considerations com-
prise: i) rules of privacy, ii) the constitutional back-
ground, and iii) privacy under law, including physi-
cal, decisional, and information privacy. Ethical con-
siderations emerge from the notion that biofeedback
systems would extend the scope of traditional infor-
mation collection. One of the ethical issues is that
biofeedback systems may introduce the risk of so-
cial exclusion for those who do use them, or maybe
even for those who do not. This makes the balance
between intelligence (e.g., AmI) and privacy even
more sensitive than, for example, with biometrics.
Taken together, perhaps more than anything else, hu-
man dignity should be a leading denominator in fu-
ture research on biofeedback systems for stress reduc-
tion (Coeckelbergh, 2011).
Stress is heading to become the #1 in (chronic)
diseases. As such its societal impact is enormous.
With this article we hope to bring the need for a solu-
tion to the attention of the health informatics society.
Closed loop biofeedback systems for stress reduction
are proposed as a feasible solution and examples of
such systems have been described. Taken together,
we propose to increase the efforts towards biofeed-
back systems for stress reduction, if not for the sake
of science then at least for the sake of society.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We are grateful to the ve reviewers for their com-
ments on an earlier draft of this article. We thank
Lynn Packwood (Human Media Interaction, Univer-
sity of Twente, NL) for her accurate proof reading.
REFERENCES
Agrawal, D. P. (2011). Designing wireless sensor networks:
From theory to applications.
Central European Journal
of Computer Science
, 1(1):2–18.
Berg, C. A. and Upchurch, R. (2007). A developmental-
contextual model of couples coping with chronic ill-
ness across the adult life span.
Psychological Bulletin
,
133(6):920–954.
Brosschot, J. F. (2010). Markers of chronic stress: Pro-
longed physiological activation and (un)conscious
perseverative cognition.
Neuroscience & Biobehav-
ioral Reviews
, 35(1):46–50.
Coeckelbergh, M. (2011). Human development or human
enhancement? A methodological reflection on capa-
bilities and the evaluation of information technolo-
gies.
Ethics and Information Technology
, 13(2):81–
92.
Davies, S. (2011). Still building the Memex.
Communica-
tions of the ACM
, 54(2):80–88.
BIOFEEDBACK SYSTEMS FOR STRESS REDUCTION - Towards a Bright Future for a Revitalized Field
503
Dumaij, A. C. M. and Tijssen, E. C. G. (2011). On-line
health companion contact among chronically ill in the
Netherlands.
Health and Technology
, 1(1):5–23.
Dunbar, H. F. (1954).
Emotions and bodily changes: A sur-
vey of literature on psychosomatic interrelationships
1910–1953
. New York, NY, USA: Columbia Univer-
sity Press, fourth edition.
Floridi, L. (2010).
The Cambridge handbook of informa-
tion and computer ethics
. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
Friedewald, M. and Raabe, O. (2011). Ubiquitous comput-
ing: An overview of technology impacts.
Telematics
and Informatics
, 28(2):55–65.
Gruzelier, J., Egner, T., and Vernon, D. (2006).
Validat-
ing the efficacy of neurofeedback for optimising per-
formance
, volume 159 of
Progress in Brain Research
,
chapter 27, pages 421–431. Amsterdam, The Nether-
lands: Elsevier B.V.
Hatzfeld, C., Kern, T. A., and Werthsch¨utzky, R. (2010).
Design and evaluation of a measuring system for hu-
man force perception parameters.
Sensors and Actua-
tors A: Physical
, 162(2):202–209.
Healey, J. A. (2008).
Sensing affective experience
, vol-
ume 8 of
Philips Research Book Series
, chapter 8,
pages 91–100. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer
Science + Business Media B.V.
Izard et al., C. E. (2010). Special section: On defining emo-
tion.
Emotion Review
, 2(4):363–385.
Janssen, J. H., van den Broek, E. L., and Westerink, J. H.
D. M. (2012). Tune in to your emotions: A robust
personalized affective music player.
User Modeling
and User-Adapted Interaction
, 22:[in press].
Kaklauskas, A., Zavadskas, E. K., Pruskus, V., Vlasenko,
A., Bartkiene, L., Paliskiene, R., Zemeckyte, L., Ger-
stein, V., Dzemyda, G., and Tamulevicius, G. (2011).
Recommended biometric stress management system.
Expert Systems with Applications Expert Systems
with Applications
, 38(11):14011–14025.
K¨uller, R., Mikellides, B., and Janssens, J. (2009). Color,
arousal, and performance a comparison of three ex-
periments.
Color Research & Application
, 34(2):141–
152.
Lucas, R. E., Diener, E., and Larsen, R. J. (2009).
Mea-
suring positive emotions
, volume 39 of
Social Indica-
tors Research Series
, pages 139–155. Dordrecht, The
Netherlands: Springer Science+Business Media B.V.
Mandler, G., Mandler, J. M., and Uviller, E. T. (1958). Au-
tonomic feedback: The perception of autonomic ac-
tivity.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology
,
56(3):367–373.
Mielle, P., Tarrega, A., S´emon, E., Maratray, J., Gorria, P.,
Liodenot, J. J., Liaboeuf, J., Andrejewski, J.-L., and
Salles, C. (2010). From human to artificial mouth,
from basics to results.
Sensors and Actuators B:
Chemical
, 146(2):440–445.
Moss, D. and Gunkelman, J. (2002). Task force report on
methodology and empirically supported treatments:
Introduction.
Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeed-
back
, 27(4):271–272.
Moss, D., LaVaque, T. J., and Hammond, D. C. (2004).
Introduction to White Papers Series Guest Edito-
rial.
Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback
,
29(3):151–152.
Neamen, D. A. (2010).
Microelectronics: Circuit analy-
sis and design
. New York, NY, USA: McGraw-Hill
Higher Education, 4th edition.
Novak, D., Ziherl, J., Olenˇsek, A., Milavec, M., Podobnik,
J., Mihelj, M., and Munih, M. (2010). Psychophys-
iological responses to robotic rehabilitation tasks in
stroke.
IEEE Transactions on Neural Systems and Re-
habilitation Engineering
, 18(4):351–361.
Ouwerkerk, M. (2011).
Unobtrusive emotions sensing in
daily life
, volume 12 of
Philips Research Book Series
,
chapter 2, pages 21–39. Dordrecht, The Netherlands:
Springer Science+Business Media B.V.
Ouwerkerk, M., Pasveer, F., and Langereis, G. (2008).
Unobtrusive sensing of psychophysiological param-
eters: Some examples of non-invasive sensing tech-
nologies
, volume 8 of
Philips Research Book Series
,
chapter 15, pages 163–193. Dordrecht, The Nether-
lands: Springer Science + Business Media B.V.
Plasier, S. A. J. (2011). A study of monaural beat effects
on brain activity using iBowl. MSc thesis, Signal Pro-
cessing Systems Group, Department of Electrical En-
gineering, Eindhoven University of Technology, Eind-
hoven, The Netherlands.
Schultz, I. H. (2003).
Das autogene Training: Konzen-
trative Selbstentspannung Versuch einer klinisch-
praktischen Darstellung
. Stuttgart, Germany: Georg
Thieme Verlag, 20th edition.
Schwabe, L., Wolf, O. T., and Oitzl, M. S. (2010). Memory
formation under stress: Quantity and quality.
Neuro-
science & Biobehavioral Reviews
, 34(4):584–591.
Sifakis, J. (2011). A vision for computer science – the sys-
tem perspective.
Central European Journal of Com-
puter Science
, 1(1):108–116.
S¨ornmo, L. and Laguna, P. (2005).
Bioelectrical Signal
Processing in Cardiac and Neurological Applications
.
Burlington, MA, USA: Elsevier Academic Press.
Suk, H.-J., Jeong, S.-H., Yang, T.-H., and Kwon, D.-S.
(2009). Tactile sensation as emotion elicitator. Kansei
Engineering International
, 8(2):147–152.
Taylor, S. E. and Stanton, A. L. (2007). Coping resources,
coping processes, and mental health.
Annual Review
of Clinical Psychology
, 3:377–401.
Van den Broek, E. L., Nijholt, A., and Westerink, J.
H. D. M. (2010). Unveiling affective signals. In
Barakova, E., de Ruyter, B., and Spink, A., editors,
ACM Proceedings of Measuring Behavior 2010: Se-
lected papers from the 7th International Conference
on Methods and Techniques in Behavioral Research
,
page a6, Eindhoven, The Netherlands. New York, NY,
USA: ACM.
Van Gerven, M., Farquhar, J., Schaefer, R., Vlek, R., Geuze,
J., Nijholt, A., Ramsay, N., Haselager, P., Vuurpijl,
L., Gielen, S., and Desain, P. (2009). The brain-
computer interface cycle.
Journal of Neural Engineer-
ing
, 6(4):1–10.
HEALTHINF 2012 - International Conference on Health Informatics
504