Imagery Improves Reaction Time in Elite Sprinters
Muhammad Talha Iftikhar
1
, Cliff J. Mallett
1
and Mohammad Asghar Javed
2
1
School of Human Movement and Nutrition Sciences,The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
2
Department of Sports Sciences, The University of Lahore, Lahore, Pakistan
Keywords: Mental Practice, 100 Meters Start, Elite Athletes.
Abstract: Motor imagery has been found to be helpful for developing skills in sport. Motor imagery (MI) helps an
athlete to visualize simple or complex motor activities in the absence of physical practice. Few studies have
inspected the effects of motor imagery on trained individuals. The purpose of this study was to investigate the
effects of motor imagery on reaction time. Differences in reaction time can make a difference in terms of the
overall performance (time, ranking). Twenty-four male and female National elite athletes (12 male; age:
22.92+1.73 years and 12 female; age: 22.67+1.67 years), who participated in this study, were classified into
two (2) groups. Participants were classified according to data from a pretest in which they recorded their
reaction time (ms) on starting blocks and a 30 meters race time (s). The control group (N=12) carried out the
practice physically and the imagery (intervention) group (N=12) firstly carried out the practice mentally and
then physically with the control group. Motor Imagery was conducted on the experimental group for fifteen
(15) minutes every day for two (2) weeks. At the end of two (2) weeks, a post-test was conducted to examine
any intervention effects. The data were analyzed by a paired t-test. The findings revealed that imagery group
athletes improved more than the control group (p < 0.05). A couple of the athletes from the physical practice
group (no intervention) showed better results than the imagery group, but the researcher observed the potential
reason behind this enhancement might have been due to the competitive atmosphere created due the
experiment for which they put their best to beat the other group in the post test.
1 INTRODUCTION
Motor learning is considered central to enhance motor
skill acquisition. Furthermore, mental practice of
these motor skills has the potential to supplement the
practical experiences for a better and faster learning
of motor skills. Mental practice involves athletes to
review certain skills and perfecting them in their mind
without performing them practically
(Mohammadpour et al., 2012). Recent studies have
found substantial evidence to support the fact that the
use of imagery has little effect on the improvement of
sport performance but when it comes to elite sporting
endeavors, even an improvement in reaction time can
have dramatic impact (Nelson et al., 1994). For
example, difference between the world top sprinters
are in milliseconds (i.e., Bolt: 9.58s, Gay: 9.69s,
Blake: 9.69s, Powell: 9.72s, Gatlin: 9.74s, Carter:
9.78s). Thus, even a minor improvement in reaction
time might have an important performance effect in
athletic events. The objective of the present study is
to assess the implications and role of mental practice
on the effective reaction time responses
(performance) of elite athletes.
The philosophy behind the emergence of sports
psychology, as a branch of psychology, is focused
partly on providing assistance to coaches in teaching
motor skills to the athletes (Mohammadpour et al.,
2012). Self-focused attention is a process that surges
personal awareness as well as it is exceptionally
connected with personal evaluation (Silvia and
Phillips, 2013). As with other forms of self-focused
attention, imagery is hypothesized to include a
process of evaluation. The images generated are
believed to prompt comparisons between the
imagined activities and actual performances (Hall,
2001). In this manner, researchers have demonstrated
the positive effects of imagery to enhance
performance. Imagery can be used to help athletes
who are injured to continue to develop their motor
skills as well as a supplementary activity to physical
practice.
The key difference between mental and physical
practice is associated with the practical experience of
the physical practice (Haier et al., 2005). All
Iftikhar, M., Mallett, C. and Javed, M.
Imagery Improves Reaction Time in Elite Sprinters.
DOI: 10.5220/0006898300270033
In Proceedings of the 6th International Congress on Sport Sciences Research and Technology Support (icSPORTS 2018), pages 27-33
ISBN: 978-989-758-325-4
Copyright © 2018 by SCITEPRESS Science and Technology Publications, Lda. All rights reserved
27
processes of motor planning and preparation are
activated when an athlete initiates the mental
imagination of a certain skill or practice, with the
same magnitude of performing that activity. Motor
imagery has been connected with neural changes
(Davis et al., 2008; Davis et al., 2012) that are reliable
with the methods that persuade self-focused attention
(Ma and Han, 2011; Qin and Northoff, 2009).
Davis supports the idea that self-focused imagery
used by athletes may stimulate personal evaluations
that change psychological conditions as well as
physiological conditions, which are considered
beneficial for sport performance. For example, when
presented with self-referenced stimuli and asked to
imagine a previous performance, athletes with higher
success rates report more positive impact, less
negative effect, and amplified blood oxygen-level
dependent (BOLD) activation in the appropriate
premotor cortex, that is, the sensorimotor cortex
(Davis et al., 2012). The slower the stimulus
recognition, the slower the reaction movement. A
delay of even one hundredth of a second can cost an
athlete a podium position in the professional track and
field sprint events.
Electrophysiological studies on the brain have
confirmed that cerebral potential amplitude surges
when a new skill is acquired, specifically in the
premotor prospective, which is the “decision-to-act”
part of reaction time; elite athletes have higher
potential of predictability compared to novice athletes
(Collet, 1999). This neuroscientific research supports
the view that reaction time is both learned and
trainable.
An elite athlete can prepare himself for the
optimal performance before any major competition
by mentally rehearsing a routine before physically
engaging themselves. Through imagery, athletes can
build their confidence for a match and focus on
playing at their peak. The value of imagery is
supported by research, in the absence of physical
practice, such as during travel, weather conditions,
injury or any other unfortunate circumstances. It
allows an athlete to review former actions and skills,
so they can add accuracy and correct errors (Morris et
al., 2005). Motor imagery is very useful for
rehabilitation in order to boost an athlete’s
performance in the future and it is currently
categorized as a very important area of work
regarding the motor imagery researchers (Poolton et
al., 2006). International athletes like Michael
Johnson, gold medalist of four Olympic and eight
World Championships, former world and Olympic
record holder in the 200m and 400m as well as the
world record holder in the indoor 400m, talks about
of what he calls the “danger zone” in which he creates
a competitive mind-set on the day of the race, in
which he uses positive thoughts and images to block
out all the distractions (Vealey and Greenleaf, 2001).
Visual self-processing approaches like imagery
are repeatedly applied to enhance sport performance
through various affective and motivational functions
(Martin et al., 1999). Like other forms of self-
perception, mental imagery is an internal
psychological activity that stimulates conscious poly-
sensory experiences of objects observed in the past
practices or may follow (Hall, 2001; Vealey and
Greenleaf, 2001). Motor imagery is one of the most
significant processes for mental practice. This process
involves reviewing of a certain sport skill in their
mind without practically performing it. Learners
review different parts of performance and visualize
doing it successfully and even become the world
champions.
According to Personnier and colleagues (2008),
mental and physical practice have a shared neural
mechanism, and the required time for performing a
task is equivalent to the time needed for imagining the
same. And when the level of a task elevates, the time
required for practically performing it along with
mentally imagining it also increases.
Experimental research by Brouziyne and
Molinaro (2005) shows that the novice athletes could
achieve the skill of performing a golf shot. In this
research, the highest level of improvement, among
the three groups, was observed in the combined
physical and mental practice group. This research
also revealed that imagery is capable of developing
motor skills and performance enhancement, even in
the novice performers (Brouziyne and Molinaro).
Preliminary evidence exists to support the idea that
development in motor performance next to physical
practice has higher effect than mental practice alone,
but mental practice empowers an athlete to learn
motor prediction and motor learning (Gentili et al.,
2006).
Majumdar and Robergs (2011) researched on two
different parts of a sprinter’s response time, reaction
time and movement time. From the beginning of
motion of the rear foot, off the block, until that same
foot hits the ground; so, the response time is a
collective measure of both reaction time and
movement time that is from initial stimulus to initial
foot strike (Majumdar and Robergs, 2011). Reaction
time is the ability of an individual to move the whole
or a part of his body in the shortest possible time, for
example: swimmer leaving the starting pad; a thrower
putting the shot; karate player moves his hand, or a
wrestler makes a certain move to perform a wrestling
icSPORTS 2018 - 6th International Congress on Sport Sciences Research and Technology Support
28
technique. Reaction time is the interval between
reception of the stimulus by a certain part of the body
and responding to it. (Mohammadpour et al., 2012).
Reaction time is known as a vital feature in the
world of Track and Field. This technical resource
makes the elite athletes superior from the rest. Magill
and Anderson defined reaction time as the time
interval between the inception of a stimulus to the
initiation of a response (Magill and Anderson, 2013).
According to Iulian, 2012, foundation of the one’s
reaction pathway to the brain is the neuromuscular
and psychomotor connections. Athlete’s ability to
react against a stimulus is dependent upon his
learning experience, age, gender and his mental state
(Iulian, 2012).
Multiple researchers have explored the effects of
imagery on the force production (Guillot, and Collet,
2008; Lebon et al 2010). Studies have inspected the
effects of imagery on a bicep curls, leg presses,
standing long jumps, bench presses and a range of
other movements, and found positive results on some
fundamentals of force production (Guillot and Collet,
2008; Lebon et al., 2010). Many athletes and coaches
already use imagery to improve performance
(Holmes and Collins, 2001) but few studies have
observed the effects of imagery contribution on
reaction time to improve athletic performance.
Undoubtedly, in a sporting performance, the
athlete needs to perform a certain skill as fast as
possible, as the key to success relies on that certain
reaction time in which he makes that decision
psychologically and applies it physically. The
importance of reaction time is revealed when you
observe an athlete wins over his opponent only with
a fraction of a second’s difference. Many examples
can be observed in a 100m sprint event where
milliseconds can separate medalists from non-
medalists. Furthermore, a good versus poor start
might provide sprinters with increased self-efficacy
to perform at their best in a race.
1.1 Objective of Study and Hypothesis
The primary aim of this study was to assess the role
of imagery on improving reaction time responses
(performance) of elite athletes in Pakistan. The
primary hypothesis was that imagery will improve
reaction time in elite sprinters.
2 METHOD
2.1 Participants
The sample comprised of twenty-four (24) elite
national athletes, twelve (12) male and twelve (12)
female, were selected for this study. Average age of
the athletes was 22.83+1.69. Although they had been
participating sprint training for at least three (3) days
a week for more than four (4) months, but they had
no prior formal imagery training with a sport
psychology consultant or coach.
2.2 Research Instruments
Motor Imagery, Starting Blocks and Electronic
Timer.
2.3 Study Design
2.3.1 Procedure
Sample of twenty-four (24) male and female athletes
performed three (3) block starts and 30-meter runs,
from which the best time for both variables were
recorded for further analyses. Based on pretest
performance, athletes were divided into two groups,
controlled and experimental. Controlled group
comprised six (6) male and six (6) female elite
athletes with the mean age of 23+1.76 years. Same
number of male and female athletes were included in
the experimental group with average age of
22.67+1.67 years.
Controlled group of twelve (12) athletes’ camp
training under their coaches was not interrupted
during the two (2) weeks experiment. Experimental
group was physically trained for two (2) weeks along
with controlled group and before their physical
training session, they had Motor Imagery (MI)
practice every day for fifteen (15) minutes.
For Motor Imagery (MI), PETTLEP model
(Holmes and Collins, 2001) was used. Fifteen (15)
minutes of imagery was divided into two
components: for the first five (5) minutes the athletes
were guided through deep breathing, so that they can
relax and gain control of their physiological responses
such as heart rate. For the next ten (10) minutes sport
specific motor imagery script was created, in which
athletes were instructed to focus on their event-
specific personal feelings and thoughts. The athletes
were guided through the event-specific imagery and
asked to use the last three (3) minutes for their own
imagery in which they could concentrate on minute
details when taking a crouch start.
Imagery Improves Reaction Time in Elite Sprinters
29
At the end of two (2) weeks training period, all the
athletes were asked to record their reaction time (ms)
and 30 meters time (s) again for the posttest.
2.3.2 Data Analysis
IBM SPSS Statistics 22 was used to perform the
statistical analyses. Descriptive statistics (means and
standard deviations) were calculated for all variables.
Paired samples t-tests were applied to conclude
differences between treatment and no-treatment
conditions for all dependent variables. To compare
the improvement in both the groups independent
sample t-test was used. The statistical significance
level was set at p<0.05.
3 RESULTS
To test the main hypothesis, a paired sample t-test
was conducted on the pretest and the posttest results
of control (see Table 2) and Imagery (see Table 4)
groups respectively. The results of paired t-test
showed non-significant (p>0.05) difference between
mean times of pre and post results of the participants
of the control group. The results of the Imagery group
(intervention) showed that there was a significant
(p<0.05) difference between mean times of pre and
post results for the participants in this study. Indeed,
the motor imagery training showed a significant
(p<0.05) effect on the performance of the elite
athletes (imagery intervention).
4 DISCUSSION
The aim of this study was to investigate the effects of
motor imagery on reaction time. An experimental
research was conducted to analyze this issue. The
study on reaction time revealed positive impact of
mental practice while comparing pre and post test
results. In this experiment all the participants were
very interested and put their best efforts in two (2)
weeks training session and on the other hand the
imagery group showed great enthusiasm and focus in
their both physical and mental practice sessions. The
analysis of the current study has been analyzed by
paired t-test. The findings revealed that Experimental
group improvement was more rapid and significant
than the controlled group. Within the first half of the
experiment, improved running style and body
coordination in the imagery group participants was
observed by the researcher and the coaches, which
leads the author to believe that they were not only
rehearsing the crouch start but also the other
components in the imagery sessions. Couple of
athletes from the physical practice group showed
better results than the imagery group, but the
researcher observed the reason behind this
enhancement was due to the competitive atmosphere
created due the experiment for which they put their
best to beat the other group in the post test. Future
studies may target these intrinsic factors that might
help in improving training effort.
There were 24 participants in this study and
results cannot be generalized to the whole sprinting
population in Pakistan, future studies might look into
greater sample size for more accurate results. This
investigation contributes to the current knowledge
that Motor Imagery (MI) can be used to improve
reaction time in Pakistani elite sprinters which leads
to a better race time.
All the athletes included in this research were
highly motivated and showed maximum interest. The
study was conducted in a limited time; for more
intensive results, this kind of research should be
conducted for longer period and should also focus on
different components of the race.
REFERENCES
Brouziyne, M., and Molinaro, C. (2005). Mental imagery
combined with physical practice of approach shots for
golf beginners. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 101(1),
203-211.
Collet, C. (1999). Strategic aspects of reaction time in
world-class sprinters. Perceptual and motor skills,
88(1), 65-75.
Davis IV, H., Liotti, M., Ngan, E. T., Woodward, T. S., Van
Snellenberg, J. X., van Anders, S. M., ... and Mayberg,
H. S. (2008).
fMRI BOLD signal changes in elite swimmers while
viewing videos of personal failure. Brain Imaging and
Behavior, 2(2), 84-93.
Davis IV, H., van Anders, S. M., Ngan, E. T., Woodward,
T. S., Van Snellenberg, J. X., Mayberg, H. S., and
Liotti, M. (2012). Neural, mood, and endocrine
responses in elite athletes relative to successful and
failed performance videos. Journal of Clinical Sport
Psychology, 6(1), 6-21.
Gentili, R., Papaxanthis, C., and Pozzo, T. (2006).
Improvement and generalization of arm motor
performance through motor imagery practice.
Neuroscience, 137(3), 761-772.
Guillot, A., and Collet, C. (2008). Construction of the motor
imagery integrative model in sport: a review and
theoretical investigation of motor imagery use.
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology,
1(1), 31-44.
icSPORTS 2018 - 6th International Congress on Sport Sciences Research and Technology Support
30
Haier, R. J., Jung, R. E., Yeo, R. A., Head, K., and Alkire,
M. T. (2005). Structural brain variation, age, and
response time. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral
Neuroscience, 5(2), 246-251.
Hainaut, J. P., and Bolmont, B. (2005). Effects of mood
states and anxiety as induced by the video-recorded
Stroop Color-Word Interference Test in simple
response time tasks on reaction time and movement
time. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 101(3), 721-729.
Hall, C. (2001). Imagery in sport and exercise. In R.N.
Singer, H.A. Hausenblas, and C.M. Janelle (Eds.),
Handbook of sport psychology (2nd ed., pp.529-549).
New York: John Wiley and Sons.
Holmes, P. S., and Collins, D. J. (2001). The PETTLEP
approach to motor imagery: A functional equivalence
model for sport psychologists. Journal of Applied Sport
Psychology, 13(1), 60-83.
Iulian, A. D. (2012). Study regarding the relation between
the accumulation of psycho-motor experience in the
practice of sprint events, and the value of the start
reaction time. Gymnasium, 13(2), 63.
Lebon, F., Collet, C., and Guillot, A. (2010). Benefits of
motor imagery training on muscle strength. The Journal
of Strength and Conditioning Research, 24(6), 1680-
1687.
Magill, R., and Anderson, D. (2013). Motor learning and
control: Concepts and applications (10th ed.). New
York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Majumdar, A. S., and Robergs, R. A. (2011). The science
of speed: Determinants of performance in the 100 m
sprint. International Journal of Sports Science and
Coaching, 6(3), 479-493.
Martin, K. A., Moritz, S. E., and Hall, C. R. (1999). Imagery
use in sport: A literature review and applied model. The
Sport Psychologist, 13(3), 245-268.
Mohammadpour, M., Hashemian, S., Yaghoubi, H.,
Valiolah, S., Vahid, I., and Armandnia, M. (2012). The
effect of mental practice on response time via Nelson's
speed of movement test. European Journal of
Experimental Biology, 2(6), 2444-2450.
Morris, T., Spittle, M., and Watt, A. P. (2005). Imagery in
sport. Human Kinetics.
Personnier, P., Paizis, C., Ballay, Y., and Papaxanthis, C.
(2008). Mentally represented motor actions in normal
aging: II. The influence of the gravito-inertial context
on the duration of overt and covert arm movements.
Behavioural Brain Research, 186(2), 273-283.
Poolton, J. M., Masters, R. S. W., and Maxwell, J. P.
(2006). The influence of analogy learning on decision-
making in table tennis: Evidence from behavioural data.
Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 7(6), 677-688.
Qin, P., and Northoff, G. (2011). How is our self related to
midline regions and the default-mode network?.
Neuroimage, 57(3), 1221-1233.
Sigmundsson, H. (2005). Do visual processing deficits
cause problem on response time task for dyslexics?.
Brain and Cognition, 58(2), 213-216.
Silvia, P. J., and Phillips, A. G. (2013). Self-awareness
without awareness? Implicit self-focused attention and
behavioral self-regulation. Self and Identity, 12(2), 114-
127.
Vealey, R. S. and Greenleaf, C. A. (2001). Seeing is
believing: Understanding and using imagery in sport. In
J. M.
Williams (Ed.), Applied sport psychology: Personal growth
to peak performance (5th ed.) (pp. 306-348). Mountain
View, CA: Mayfield.
Imagery Improves Reaction Time in Elite Sprinters
31
APPENDIX
Table 1: Paired Samples Statistics for Reaction Time (ms) and 30m time (s) Control Group.
Mean
N
Std. Deviation
Std. Error Mean
Correlation= r
Sig.
Pair 1
204.33
12
48.044
.983
.994
.000
202.75
12
42.879
Pair 2
4.433
12
.563
.975
.994
.000
4.425
12
.557
SD=Standard deviation, SE=Standard error of mean, r=Correlation, p=Probability value
Table 2: Paired Samples Test for Reaction Time (ms) and 30m time (s) Control Group.
Paired Differences
t
df
Sig. (2-
tailed)
Mean
Std.
Deviation
Std. Error
Mean
95% Confidence Interval of the
Difference
Lower
Upper
Pair 1
Reaction Time Pretest
- Reaction Time
Posttest
1.583
7.280
2.101
-3.042
6.209
.753
11
.467
Pair 2
30m Pretest - 30m
Posttest
.008
.0607
.0175
-.0302
.0469
.475
11
.644
SD=Standard deviation, SE=Standard error of mean, CI=Confidence Interval, Df= Degree of Freedom, p=Probability value
Table 3: Paired Samples Statistics for Reaction Time (ms) and 30m time (s) Experimental Group.
Mean
N
Std. Deviation
Std. Error
Mean
Correlation= r
Sig.
Pair 1
Reaction Time Pretest
207.50
12
57.450
16.584
.983
.000
Reaction Time Posttest
195.83
12
48.889
14.113
Pair 2
30m Pretest
4.411
12
.513
.148
.975
.000
30m Posttest
4.315
12
.482
.139
SD=Standard deviation, SE=Standard error of mean, r=Correlation, p=Probability value
Table 4: Paired Samples Test for Reaction Time (ms) and 30m time (s) Experimental Group.
Paired Differences
t
df
Sig. (2-
tailed)
Mean
Std.
Deviation
Std. Error
Mean
95% Confidence Interval of
the Difference
Lower
Upper
Pair 1
Reaction Time Pretest
- Reaction Time
Posttest
11.667
12.950
3.738
3.439
19.895
3.121
11
.010**
Pair 2
30m Pretest - 30m
Posttest
.097
.115
.0332
.023
.170
2.906
11
.014**
SD=Standard deviation, SE=Standard error of mean, CI=Confidence Interval, Df= Degree of Freedom, p=Probability value
icSPORTS 2018 - 6th International Congress on Sport Sciences Research and Technology Support
32
Table 5: Group Statistic Pre-Post Difference.
N
Mean
Std. Deviation
Std. Error Mean
Reaction Time
Imagery
12
11.667
12.95
3.738
Control
12
1.583
7.280
2.101
30m Time
Imagery
12
0.966
0.115
0.033
Control
12
0.008
0.061
0.018
SD=Standard deviation, SE=Standard error of mean
Table 6: Independent Samples Test for Pre and Post Difference.
t-test for Equality of Means
t
df
Sig. (2-tailed)
Mean
Difference
Std. Error
Difference
95% Confidence Interval of the
Difference
Lower
Upper
RT Improvement (ms)
2.351
22
.028
10.083
4.288
1.190
18.977
30m Improvement (s)
2.349
22
.028
8.833
3.760
1.035
16.631
SD=Standard deviation, SE=Standard error of mean, CI=Confidence Interval, Df= Degree of Freedom, p=Probability value
Imagery Improves Reaction Time in Elite Sprinters
33