Developmental-Affordances
An Approach to Designing Child-friendly Environment
Fitri Arlinkasari
1,2
and Debra Flanders Cushing
2
1
Faculty of Psychology, YARSI University, JL. Letjen Suprapto, Cempaka Putih, Jakarta Pusat, Indonesia
2
School of Design, Queensland University of Technology, 2 George Street, Brisbane, QLD 4001, Australia
fitri.arlinkasari@hdr.qut.edu.au, debra.cushing@qut.edu.au
Keywords: Affordances, child-friendly environment, child development.
Abstract: A child-friendly environment is a place that provides children with opportunities for their activities, or from
the ecological perspective, a rich-affordances environment. However, children’s environments are often
designed by adults who may have an insufficient understanding of children’s needs, potentially causing a
disconnect between affordances provided and those actualised by children. To address this issue, we posit
developmental-affordances as an approach to designing a place for children, which integrates the theoretical
perspectives of affordances and child development. Affordance theory indicates that an environment affords
people with opportunities for action, and emphasises the relative functions of the environment according to
the perceiver’s capabilities to respond to those opportunities. However, affordances can be more effective for
designing a child-friendly place if it is informed by an understanding of the developmental stages. This
knowledge will illuminate designers with ideas for environmental features and activities that naturally attract
children as the configuration of affordances are actualised to support their development. Moreover, as child
development takes place within a specific context, designers should also note the influence of social and
physical properties of an environment that might support and thwart children’s motivation to actualise the
potential affordances.
1 INTRODUCTION
Acknowledging the global movement involving
Child Friendly Cities Initiatives (CFCI), research on
children within urban environments has increased
since the 1990s (McGlone, 2016). The movement
successfully triggered children’s participation to
evaluate as well as design their city in various ways,
include how they perceive public urban spaces. Most
prominently, important results have been generated
from the Growing Up In Cities (GUIC) and
Environmental Child-Friendliness (ECF)
frameworks, which provide us with indicators of
child-friendly environments for assessing and
designing effective places for children.
The Growing Up In Cities (GUIC) project,
initiated by UNCESCO in 1996, successfully
depicted environmental qualities of local
environments perceived by children across different
countries. Employing a participatory research design,
GUIC generated children’s perception of negative
and positive themes that define the social and
physical quality of their local environment (table 1).
The outcomes of GUIC also affirmed the findings by
Nordström in 1990 (cited in Nordström, 2010) that
the physical setting is connected to one’s social life;
thus a quality assessment of an environment must not
separate the two.
Table 1: Indicators of Children's Environmental Quality
(source: Chawla, 2002).
Social Qualities
Physical
Qualities
Positive
- Social
integration
- Freedom from
social threats
- Cohesive
community
identity
- Secure tenure
- Tradition of
community
self-help
- Green areas
- Provision of
basic services
- Variety of
activity
settings
- Freedom from
physical
dangers
- Freedom of
movement
- Peer gathering
places
94
Arlinkasari, F. and Cushing, D.
Developmental-Affordances - An Approach to Designing Child-friendly Environment.
In Proceedings of the Annual Conference on Social Sciences and Humanities (ANCOSH 2018) - Revitalization of Local Wisdom in Global and Competitive Era, pages 94-99
ISBN: 978-989-758-343-8
Copyright © 2018 by SCITEPRESS Science and Technology Publications, Lda. All rights reserved
Negative
- Sense of
political
powerlessness
- Insecure
tenure
- Racial
tensions
- Fear of
harassment
and crime
- Boredom
- Social
exclusion and
stigma
- Lack of
gathering
places
- Lack of
activity
settings
- Lack of basic
services
- Heavy traffic
- Trash/litter
- Geographic
isolation
Another notable framework to identify essential
properties of child-friendly environments is
Environmental Child Friendliness (ECF) developed
by Horelli according to her research in the Finnish-
context. EFC comprises ten dimensions: housing and
dwelling; basic services; participation; safety and
security; family, kin, peers and community; urban and
environmental qualities; resources provision and
distribution poverty; ecology; sense of belonging and
continuity. The EFC also outlines “young people’s
life as a physical, psychosocial, cultural, economic
and even political entity” (Horelli, 2007, p.270). The
ten dimensions can be regarded as normative aspects
of an ideal child-friendly environment, but the form
and details of this environment are shaped by the
social-cultural context (Horelli, 2007).
From the mentioned frameworks, we can
conclude that a child-friendly environment is
indicated by opportunities that support children to
implement their needs and goals (e.g. to move freely,
to interact with others, to access services, to manage
exciting activities, and to feel safe). To create this
kind of place, a thorough understanding of children’s
needs and their socio-cultural context is fundamental
because it impacts children’s ability to access and
make use of the opportunities within a setting.
However, despite this need, children’s
environments are often designed by adults who don’t
have sufficient knowledge about the developmental
needs of children. Moreover, the process of designing
and planning spaces usually excludes children which
potentially causes a disconnection between
opportunities designed into an environment and those
actualised by children. In turn, the environment
becomes an ineffective place for children’s
development.
Yet, UNICEF (2009) stressed that healthy
development is the indicator of a child-friendly
environment. Therefore, this is a key area for further
research and consideration. Specifically, this gap
requires an approach that can lead to deeper
understanding in two areas. First, the functionality of
an environment depends in part on the perceiver’s
capabilities, which can be examined by advocating
affordances theory. Second, the utilisation of
affordances can support child’s development, which
can be better understood through human development
theories. This paper will explain how the integration
of two approaches will provide insight into a more
effective way to identify child-friendliness of a
setting as the basis for future design.
The nature of this research is a theoretical review
which collects a number of studies and project reports
of environmental design that utilise two theoretical
perspectives, namely affordances theory and
developmental psychology theories. This paper has
two aims. First, to provide an understanding of child-
friendly environment indicators. Second, to propose a
design approach that integrates the theory of
affordances and child development to meet the
indicators of the child-friendly environment.
2 AFFORDANCES THEORY
First developed in 1979 by James Gibson,
‘affordances’ denotes a transactional relationship
between perceiver and their environment, indicated
by what an environment affords the perceiver:
"The affordances of the environment are
what it offers the animal, what it provides or
furnishes, either for good or ill. The verb to
afford is found in the dictionary, but the noun
affordance is not. I have made it up. I mean by
it something that refers to both the
environment and the animal in a way that no
existing term does. It implies the
complementarity of the animal and the
environment...” (Gibson, 1979, p. 127)
In Gibson’s view, “people and animals do not
construct the world that they live in but are attuned to
the invariants of information in the environment”
(Greeno, 1994, p.337). This means properties of the
environment enable or afford the perceiver particular
opportunities to interact with that environment.
Gibson argued that environments consist of
affordances, defined as activity possibilities, as the
primary objects of human’s perception. That is why
individuals perceive the environment regarding what
behaviour it affords (i.e. a tree affords climbing, a
door affords opening, a chair affords sitting).
Furthermore, the activities are guided by how a
person detects or perceives information, often visual
cues, that specifies what the environment affords that
Developmental-Affordances - An Approach to Designing Child-friendly Environment
95
person. Gibson suggests that the environment or
object offers what it does because it is what it is. An
affordance is invariant and does not change even if
the perceiver’s needs change (Gibson, 1986).
However, an affordance exists relative to the action
capabilities of the perceiver. In Gibson’s view as
explained by Tudge, Shanahan, & Valsiner (1997),
the perceiver also must pick up “self-information” (or
assessment about his own capabilities) to respond to
the information provided by the environment:
“If perception of the environment is co-
perception of the self, then information that
specifies the environment also specifies the
self, or the actor's position in the environment.
If the environment affords some action for the
perceiver, it is in relation to the perceiver's
action capabilities.” (Tudge et al., 1997, p.
82).
3 DEVELOPMENTAL NATURE
OF AFFORDANCES
Although affordance theory does not specifically
examine human development, it is widely used by
developmental psychologists to understand the
process of learning the world through environmental
interaction. For Gibson, the world contains invariant
information that can be directly accessed by human
perception systems that adapt to retrieve this
information through direct perception, within
exploration actions (Moore and Marans, 1997).
Dynamic invariances are only revealed when humans
move actively, capturing information in their
environment. The exploration actions must be
repeated to be able to detect new invariances that exist
in the environment, so humans can achieve "real-life
perception" about the world (Richardson, 2000).
As exploration is a continuous action across the
lifespan, it leads to the development of an internal
structure that enables the new affordances which
previously have not been accessed, and in turn
support the new exploratory ability. In the course of
development, each bit of learning affords the next -
there is a development of affordances because new
systems for information production through
integrated perception, cognition and action systems
have developed (Richardson, 2000, p.107).
Furthermore, perception informs what action can be
done, and therefore all developmental action is based
upon the adaptive utilisation of the environment.
Briefly, Heft (1988) posits that affordances have
a developmental nature, in which one's
developmental capability determines the function of
an environment. As such, new affordances can
emerge as an implication of the rise of one's
developmental maturity and experience within the
environment. For example, older children can
perceive and actualise more affordances from streets
in their neighbourhood than young children because
of their well-developed independent mobility and
diverse experience in that place. The older children
can use streets in various ways, such as a place to
hang out with friends, to access transportation, to
observe the everyday occurrences in the city. On the
other hand, the younger children may perceive streets
as a less functional place because they spend most of
their time at home and limited independent mobility.
A number of researchers have examined the
place-affordances sought by young people according
to their developmental needs, or developmental
affordances’, include play (Maier, Fadel and Battisto,
2009), and independent mobility (Kyttä, 2003;
Ramezani and Said, 2013). Previous studies also
explored affordances through what an individual feels
from doing an activity within a specific setting (Kyttä,
2003). For example, a room allows a child to have
privacy (as a feeling) which supports the activity of
emotional-regulation or as the implication of an
activity (e.g. feel relaxed when visiting a park)
(Oerter, 1998). Thus, it is possible to examine
affordances through activities and experiences.
The perceiver’s capabilities can be the starting
point for examining affordances within an
environment (Clark & Uzzell, 2006; Parke in Altman
& Wohwil, 1978). From previous explanations, we
can assume that the capabilities of the perceiver are
an implication of their maturity level. Thus,
capabilities are developmental-related attributes
which are unique within each developmental stage
(Newman and Newman, 2012). However, we still do
not thoroughly understand how environmental
interaction can support child development and what
drives the children to use specific affordances.
Therefore, we need further research to investigate the
association between voluntary activities and the
broader set of human developmental tasks.
4 DEVELOPMENTAL TASKS:
THE MOTIVATION FOR
ENVIRONMENTAL
INTERACTION
Each stage of development has its own developmental
tasks which must be fulfilled as an indication of the
ANCOSH 2018 - Annual Conference on Social Sciences and Humanities
96
readiness for the next period of life. To fulfil the
developmental tasks, children as active agents are
often encouraged to explore the physical properties of
their environment (Loebach, 2004). Van Vliet (1983)
suggests that children are naturally active in a
continuous search of new interactions with the
environment, coupled with their developing mobility.
Gradually, the child begins his exploration activities
with their current capabilities and is challenged to
increase the difficulty level of the activity in order to
positively influence the acquisition of new skills. As
Moore states, "Skills motivate interaction [with the
environment], interaction stimulates the learning of
skills" (Moore, 1986, p.15). Hence, the motivation for
environmental interaction is naturally driven by
developmental tasks and exists in all children of every
developmental stage.
Self-directed exploration of an environment also
leads children to naturally seek opportunities to
continue to challenge their actual capabilities in order
to achieve their potential capabilities. The scholars of
sociocultural paradigm (e.g. Vygotsky) believe that
these opportunities are provided in children’s
environments, and thus young people will be much
more developed if they actively interact with their
environment (Vygotsky, 1994; Mistry, Contreras and
Dutta, 2012). Their psychological system or the
ability to make meaning of experiences and take
action will develop through these environmental
interactions. By using their current stage of
development, the child will strive to achieve their
potential development with the support of the
environment (Loebach, 2004). For this reason, the
environment must provide children with an
appropriate degree of familiarity as well as
unfamiliarity, extending from the routine to
exploratory, from known to the yet-be-discovered
(Moore and Young in Altman and Wohlwill, 1978;
Matthews, 1992)
Although the urge to interact with the
environment is intrinsic, it is inevitable that
environmental properties also invite a person to
interact within that environment (Heft, 2013). From
an ecological perspective, children and the
environment simultaneously initiate the interaction.
Children's environmental interaction is influenced by
attributes of personal stimulus characteristic
(Bronfenbrenner, 1993), such as personal
characteristics, interest in world-exploration, and
directive belief about their relationship with the
world. Simultaneously, the environment has physical
and social features that initiate the transactions with
the child. The nature of the environmental properties
can either promote or thwart a child’s motivation for
environmental interaction (Tudge, Shanahan and
Valsiner, 1997).
5 DESIGNING ENVIRONMENTS
TO PROVIDE
DEVELOPMENTAL
AFFORDANCES
As discussed in previous sections, we understand that
the relationship between the perceiver and the
environment can be measured through the actions and
experiences of using the affordances which are
naturally motivated by the perceivers’ developmental
tasks. This section will explore the implications of
developmental stages on environmental design to
provide developmental affordances.
We posit three key aspects of designing an
environment that provides developmental
affordances: developmental tasks, developmental
related activities/experiences, and supportive
environmental conditions/features within which the
activities/experiences can occur. Figure 1 depicts the
relationship between the key design aspects.
Figure 1: Three key aspects of designing an environment
that supports developmental affordances (proposed by
authors).
To support our proposition, we provide an
example of developmental tasks and the supporting
environmental features for each developmental stage
during early and middle childhood (table 2).
However, an environment can be defined on a small
or large scale. Hence, this paper provides an example
of properties of a play space in the context of public
space. Public space is often assumed to be the
representation of a place that provides free access for
all ages and affords a variety of developmental
Develop
mental
Tasks
(vary
between
develop
mental
stages)
Activities/
experienc
es
motivated
by
developme
ntal tasks
which take
place
within a
context
Developmental-Affordances - An Approach to Designing Child-friendly Environment
97
activities (Elsley, 2004; Francis et al., 2012; Pacilli et
al., 2013).
Many approaches are discussed in the literature in
order to understand children’s behaviour related to
their development. However, in this paper, we use
developmental theory related to psychosocial by
Erikson because this approach has several advantages
(Newman and Newman, 2012; Ray, 2016). First,
psychosocial theory acknowledges the influence of
capabilities during the earlier stages on later
development. Second, this theory focuses on clear
developmental themes and the context for each
developmental stage, and the implications for failures
and successes that lead to achieving the
developmental tasks. Third, the psychosocial
approach recognises the bidirectional influence of
individuals and their environment on development,
which can be described as transactionalism as it is
adopted in affordance theory.
From table 2, we understand that each developmental
stage has different as well as similar preferences of
environmental features to support activities. Different
developmental stages may also have similar choices
of environmental features, but the use of them can be
flexible to accommodate different intentions
(Shackell et al., 2008). For example, a ladder within
early childhood can be used to support their gross
motor skills, while for middle childhood it can be
used to cater to their risk-taking interests by enabling
them to jump from different heights. The common use
of affordances may also appear across the
developmental stages because basically development
is not a result, but a process (Bronfenbrenner, 1993;
Richardson, 2000). Children will always be
advancing their capabilities, starting from what is
familiar to them and exploring the unfamiliar, as the
conditions needed to challenge and develop their new
skills.
Table 2: Childhood developmental stages and the supportive environmental features (adapted from Moore, 1974; Loebach,
2004; Newman and Newman, 2012; Masiulanis and Cummins, 2017).
Developmental
Stage
Developmental
Tasks
Activity/ Experiences
Supportive Environmental
Features
Early childhood
(3-6 years)
Psychosocial
crisis:
Initiative vs
guilt
- Gender
identification
- Early moral
development
- Peer play
- Climbs with confidence
- Increased speed of run
- Solitary activities
- Physical balance activity (e.g.
rides a tricycle)
- Recognising the spatial concept
(behind, under, in front of)
- Flexible elements (e.g. rocks, logs,
branches)
- Loose objects including leaves and
twigs that support diverse play
- Supporting facility for climbing
(e.g. ladders)
- More structured solitary games that
invite interaction (e.g. hide and
seek, castle with window)
- Facility for gathering and
interaction (low seat and desk) with
same age children
Middle
childhood (6-12
years)
Psychosocial
crisis:
Industry vs
inferiority
- Friendship
- Concrete
operations
- Skill learning
- Self-evaluation
- Purposive social interaction
- Team play
- Educational activity
- Risk-taking physical activity
- Restorative experience for
emotion regulation
- Adventure play properties (both
loose and fixed)
- Safe place and equipment
- Sufficient places and facilities for
group activities (e.g. soccer,
handball)
- Clear rules of place use and spatial
organisation
- Educational related tools (e.g.
reading material, counting tools)
- Adult’s support to gain new
cultural knowledge
- Restorative qualities of place, such
as privacy, relaxing atmosphere
6 CONCLUSIONS
This paper posits that developmental-affordances is a
practical approach to designing and planning child-
friendly environment. This approach will guide
designers and planners to be aware of children’s
developmental needs that drive them to engage with
specific activities within a place. Therefore, designers
and planners can create a meaningful pslace that
supports the positive outcomes of children’s
ANCOSH 2018 - Annual Conference on Social Sciences and Humanities
98
development, as it is the ultimate indicator of the
child-friendly environment.
REFERENCES
Altman, I. and Wohlwill, J. (1978) Children and the
Environment. New York: Plenum Press.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1993) ‘The Ecology of Cognitive
Development: Research Models and Fugitive
Findings’, in Wozniak, R. and Fischer, K. (eds)
Development in Context: Acting and Thinking in
Specific Environments. New York: Taylor & Francis.
Clark, C. and Uzzell, D. L. (2006) ‘The Socio-
Environmental affordances of adolescents’
environments’, Children and their Environments:
Learning, Using and Designing Spaces, (January
2006), pp. 176196.
Elsley, S. (2004) ‘Children ’ s Experience of Public Space’,
Children & Society, 18, pp. 155164. doi:
10.1002/CHI.822.
Francis, J. et al. (2012) ‘Creating sense of community: The
role of public space’, Journal of Environmental
Psychology. Elsevier Ltd, 32(4), pp. 401409.
Gibson, J. J. (1979) The Ecological Approach to Visual
Perception, of Experimental Psychology Human
Perception and. doi: 10.2307/989638.
Greeno, J. G. (1994) ‘Gibson’s affordances.’,
Psychological Review, 101(2), pp. 336342.
Heft, H. (1988) ‘Affordances of children’s environments :
a functional approach to environmental description’,
Children’s Environments Quarterly, 5(3), pp. 2937.
Heft, H. (2013) ‘An ecological approach to psychology.’,
Review of General Psychology, 17(2), pp. 162167.
Kyttä, M. (2003) Children in outdoor contexts: Affordances
and Independent Mobility in the Assessment of
Environmental Child Friendliness. Helsinki University
of Technology.
Loebach, J. (2004) Designing learning environments for
children:
Maier, J. R. A., Fadel, G. M. and Battisto, D. G. (2009) ‘An
affordance-based approach to architectural theory,
design, and practice’, Design Studies. Elsevier Ltd,
30(4), pp. 393414.
Masiulanis, K. and Cummins, E. (2017) How to Grow a
Playspace (Development and Design). Routledge.
Matthews, M. H. (1992) Making sense of place: Children’s
understanding of large scale environments, Journal of
Rural Studies. Harvester Wheatsheaf.
McGlone, N. (2016) ‘Pop-Up kids: exploring children’s
experience of temporary public space’, Australian
Planner. Taylor & Francis, 3682(April), pp. 110.
Mistry, J., Contreras, M. and Dutta, R. (2012) ‘Culture and
Child Development’, in Handbook of Psychology:
developmental psychology, pp. 243263.
Moore, G. and Marans, R. (1997) Advances in
Environment, Behavior, and Design.
Moore, R. C. (1974) ‘Patterns of Activity in Time and
Space: The Ecology of a Neighbourhood Playground’,
in Canter, D. and Lee, T. (eds) Psychology and The
Built Environment. New York: John Wiley & Sons Inc.,
p. 1974.
Moore, R. C. (1986) ‘Questions of quality’, in Childhood’s
Domain (Play and Place in Child Development).
Croom Helm Ltd., pp. 154.
Moore, R. and Young, D. (1978) 'Children and
Neighbourhood Outdoors', in Altman, I. and Wohlwill,
J. (1978) Children and the Environment. New York:
Plenum Press.
Newman, B. M. and Newman, P. R. (2012) Development
through life.
Oerter, R. (1998) ‘Transactionalism’, in Görlitz, D.,
Harloff, H. J., Mey, G., & Valsiner, J. (ed.) Children,
cities, and psychological theories: Developing
relationships. Walter de Gruyter.
Pacilli, M. G. et al. (2013) ‘Children and the public realm:
antecedents and consequences of independent mobility
in a group of 1113-year-old Italian children’,
Children’s Geographies, 11(4), pp. 377393.
Parke, R. (1978) 'Children and Home Environments', in
Altman, I. and Wohlwill, J. (1978) Children and the
Environment. New York: Plenum Press.
Ramezani, S. and Said, I. (2013) ‘Children’s nomination of
friendly places in an urban neighbourhood in Shiraz,
Iran’, Children’s Geographies, 11(1), pp. 727.
Ray, D. C. (2016) A Therapists Guide to Child
Development. Routledge.
Richardson, K. (2000) Developmental Psychology: How
Nature and Nurture Interact. New J: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates. Available at:
http://books.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=aNh4
AgAAQBAJ&pgis=1.
Shackell, A. et al. (2008) Design for Play: A guide to
creating successful play spaces. Available at:
http://www.playengland.org.uk/media/70684/design-
for-play.pdf%0Ahttp://eprints.mdx.ac.uk/5028/.
Tudge, J., Shanahan, M. J. and Valsiner, J. (1997)
Comparisons in Human Development: Understanding
Time and Context, Cambridge studies in social and
emotional development.
Van Vliet, W. (1983) ‘Exploring the Fourth Environment:
an Examination of the Home Range of City and
Suburban Teenagers’, Environment and Behavior,
15(5), pp. 567588.
Vygotsky, L. (1994) ‘The Problem of the Environment’, in
Van Der Veer, R. and Valsiner, J. (eds) The Vygotsky
Reader. Oxford, p. 1994.
Developmental-Affordances - An Approach to Designing Child-friendly Environment
99