will almost likely to develop future depressogenic
thought.
∀γ:TRACE, t, t’:TIME, R1,R2,R3,R4, MIN_MLD_LEVEL,
MIN_SC_LEVEL, MAX_CDT_LEVEL:REAL, X:AGENT
[state(γ, t) |= maladap_bf (X, R1) &
R1 > MIN_MLD LEVEL &
state(γ, t) |= sit_cues(X, R2) & R2 > MIN_SC_LEVEL &
state(γ, t) |= cog_dep_tgt (X,R3) & R3 <
MAX_CDT_LEVEL]
⇒ ∃t’:TIME > t:TIME
[state(γ, t’) |= cog_dep_tgt (X,R4) & R4 > R3]
By checking property VP2, one can verify whether
negative perception (situational cues) and bad
support will influence the rise of depressogenic
thought. It is particularly significant to observe this
property in the model given that bad support and
negative perception is highly correlated towards the
development of depressogenic thought (Crossfield et
al., 2002).
7 CONCLUSIONS
In this paper, the assumed role of negative cognitive
content in depression is explained. Based on this, a
agent-model is presented that describes the temporal
relation between personal characteristics, negative
life events and social support. This model is used in
a small simulation to investigate the effect of
different types of support on different persons that
undergo similar life events. The mathematical
analysis of the model and the verification of
expected behaviour of the modelled agents in the
simulation traces give some evidence for the
appropriateness of the model.
In the future, we would like to extent the model
with the effect of negative thoughts and a bad mood
on the willingness to offer support. Together with
the existing elements of the model, this would allow
for a multi-agent simulation of a larger community,
in which different persons interact with each other
by giving and receiving support. Such analysis
would make it possible to investigate the
consequences of depressive persons in a small
community.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
The preparation of this paper would not have been
possible without the support and ideas of Prof. Dr.
Jan Treur. Both authors would like to thank him for
ideas, and refinement of this paper.
REFERENCES
Abramson, L.Y., Alloy, L.B., Hogan, M.E., Whitehouse,
W.G., Donovan, P., Rose, D.T., Panzarella, C.,
Raniere, D. 1999. Cognitive vulnerability to
depression: Theory and evidence. Journal of Cognitive
Psychotherapy, 13, 5-20.
Alloy, L. B., Abramson, L. Y., Gibb, B. E., Crossfield, A.
G., Pieracci, A. M., Spasojevic, J., Steinberg, J. A.
2004. Developmental antecedents of cognitive
vulnerability to depression: Review of findings from
the cognitive vulnerability to depression project.
Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 18(2), 115-133.
Aziz, A.A., Klein, M.C.A., Treur, J. 2009. An agent model
of temporal dynamics in relapse and recurrence in
depression. In: Ali, M., Chen, S.M., Chien, B.C.,
Hong, T.P. (eds.), IEA-AIE 2009. LNAI, Springer
Verlag, pp. 36-45.
Beck, A.T. 1987. Cognitive models of depression, Journal
of Cognitive Psychotherapy 1, pp. 5–37.
Bosse, T., Jonker, C.M., Meij, L. van der, Treur, J. 2007.
A language and environment for analysis of dynamics
by simulation. International Journal of Artificial
Intelligence Tools, vol. 16, pp. 435-464.
Bosse, T., Jonker, C.M., Meij, L. van der, Sharpanskykh,
A., Treur, J. 2009. Specification and verification of
dynamics in agent models. International Journal of
Cooperative Information Systems, vol.18, pp.167 - 193
Coyne, J.C. 1990. Interpersonal process in depression. In:
G.I. Keitner, Editor, Depression and families: Impact
and treatment, American Psychiatric Press,
Washington, DC , pp. 31–53.
Crossfield, A. G., Alloy, L. B., Gibb, B. E., Abramson, L.
Y. 2002. The development of depressogenic cognitive
styles: The role of negative childhood life events and
parental inferential feedback. Journal of Cognitive
Psychotherapy, 16(4), 487–502.
Dobkin, R.D, Panzarella, C., J. Fernandez, Alloy, L.B.,
Cascardi, M. 2004. Adaptive inferential feedback,
depressogenic inferences, and depressed mood: A
laboratory study of the expanded hopelessness theory
of depression, Cognitive Therapy and Research, pp.
487–509.
Heller, K., Rook, K.S. 1997. Distinguishing the theoretical
functions of social ties: Implications for support
interventions. In: S. Duck, Editor, Handbook of
personal relationships: Theory, research, and
interventions, John Wiley and Sons, Chichester,
England, pp. 649–670.
Panzarella, C., Alloy, L.B., Whitehouse, W.G. 2006.
Expanded hopelessness theory of depression: On the
mechanisms by which social support protects against
depression, Cognitive Therapy and Research 30 (3),
pp. 307–33
Robinson, M. S., Alloy, L. B. 2003. Negative cognitive
styles and stress-reactive rumination interact to
predict depression: A prospective study. Cognitive
Therapy & Research, 27(3), 275-292.
Spasojevic, J., Alloy, L. B. 2001. Rumination as a
common mechanism relating depressive risk factors to
depression. Emotion, 1(1), 25-37.
AN AGENT BASED SIMULATION OF THE DYNAMICS IN COGNITIVE DEPRESSOGENIC THOUGHT
237