Figure 6: EIM Trading Activity over Time.
off. Besides this, overall trading activity was observ-
able, more or less every day. In total, trading occurred
on 30 out of 40 trading days including weekends.
Some traders remembered their user name and their
password and logged in during weekends, which was
not expected. In figure 6, the weekends are marked
with blue boxes. The daily trading average was 110
transactions with human involvement with a mini-
mum number of 2 and a maximum number of 366
transactions. In total, traders caused more than 2.000
transactions and submitted more than 4.000 orders.
Figure 7 shows the number of transactions per
trader. In total, the most active trader triggered
slightly more than 1000 transactions and the least ac-
tive trader did only one transaction.
Figure 7: Transactions per Trader.
The results in figure 6 and 7 indicate that research
questions 1a, 1b and 1c can be answered. Concerning
1a, the trading activity is spread over the whole mar-
ket period. That is exactly what one may expect be-
fore. The expectation, that the trading activity is only
observable at the beginning of the market period, can
be denied.
Research question 1b can be answered with 35
traders. In total, 110 workshop participants were in-
vited to join the market after the innovation workshop.
About one third followed the invitation and traded
in the market. Approximately 10 traders triggered
nearly 100 transactions, whereas 10 traders performed
less than 10. The rest resides somewhere between 100
and 10 transactions. That is also what one can expect
ex ante and is a typical distribution of trading behavior
reported in IM experiments (Luckner, 2008). Secon-
darily, figures 5, 6 and 7, show that traders used the
EIM nearly every day – even on weekends. On 30
out of 40 market days traders used the market, which
is quite more often than what one may expect. This
indicated that the motivation of traders does not de-
crease over time which is a very nice result regarding
research question 1c.
3.1.2 Survey Results for motivational Aspects
After the innovation workshop at EnBW, workshop
participants were asked by a paper based survey about
their opinion using an EIM at EnBW. The overall re-
sponse rate of the 110 participants was at 69 %, which
is representing. To indicate the related research ques-
tions 1d-e, we focus on two questions from the sur-
vey. One question was intended to get an estimate
about how employees approve the usage of an EIM.
They answered via a 5 point Likert scale where 5
represented “very good” whereas 1 represented “not
good”. On average, employees answered with 3.54, a
median of 4.00, a variance of 0.86 and a standard error
of 0.93. This indicates that the employees positively
judge the approach of using an EIM for innovation as-
sessment. This results supported by the high trading
activity presented in the previous section and the over-
all motivation of employees in using an EIM and indi-
cates, that employees approve the usage of EIM (Re-
search Question 1d). Furthermore, another question
asked about the employees’ motivation using an EIM.
Similarly, employees were expected to answer via a
5 point Likert scale where 5 represents “very high”
and 1 indicates “very low”. On average, employees
answered with 2.85 whereas the median was 3.00.
This indicates that the motivation using an EIM for a
longer time is perceived as neutral (research question
1e). Nevertheless, as results in section 3.1.1 indicate
a high motivation of employees and altogether, the re-
sults show evidence for the motivation of employees
in using an EIM.
3.2 Harnessing implicit Information
A very common way to measure the accuracy of IMs
is to compare its results to an observable benchmark.
In case of sport or political events, results of IMs can
be compared to the final values of the sport event
outcome or the final values of the election (Servan-
Schreiber et al., 2004; Berg et al., 2003; Luckner,
2008; Spann and Skiera, 2004; Stathel et al., 2009).
In case of EIM, a similar final value may exist, if
sales figures or project run times are traded. In case
of using EIM without real world events such as in-
novation assessment, IMs are nevertheless applicable
even if no final value is observable (Soukhoroukova,
2007; Chen et al., 2009). In case that no observable
Figure 6: EIM Trading Activity over Time.
off. Besides this, overall trading activity was observ-
able, more or less every day. In total, trading occurred
on 30 out of 40 trading days including weekends.
Some traders remembered their user name and their
password and logged in during weekends, which was
not expected. In figure 6, the weekends are marked
with blue boxes. The daily trading average was 110
transactions with human involvement with a mini-
mum number of 2 and a maximum number of 366
transactions. In total, traders caused more than 2.000
transactions and submitted more than 4.000 orders.
Figure 7 shows the number of transactions per
trader. In total, the most active trader triggered
slightly more than 1000 transactions and the least ac-
tive trader did only one transaction.
Figure 6: EIM Trading Activity over Time.
off. Besides this, overall trading activity was observ-
able, more or less every day. In total, trading occurred
on 30 out of 40 trading days including weekends.
Some traders remembered their user name and their
password and logged in during weekends, which was
not expected. In figure 6, the weekends are marked
with blue boxes. The daily trading average was 110
transactions with human involvement with a mini-
mum number of 2 and a maximum number of 366
transactions. In total, traders caused more than 2.000
transactions and submitted more than 4.000 orders.
Figure 7 shows the number of transactions per
trader. In total, the most active trader triggered
slightly more than 1000 transactions and the least ac-
tive trader did only one transaction.
Figure 7: Transactions per Trader.
The results in figure 6 and 7 indicate that research
questions 1a, 1b and 1c can be answered. Concerning
1a, the trading activity is spread over the whole mar-
ket period. That is exactly what one may expect be-
fore. The expectation, that the trading activity is only
observable at the beginning of the market period, can
be denied.
Research question 1b can be answered with 35
traders. In total, 110 workshop participants were in-
vited to join the market after the innovation workshop.
About one third followed the invitation and traded
in the market. Approximately 10 traders triggered
nearly 100 transactions, whereas 10 traders performed
less than 10. The rest resides somewhere between 100
and 10 transactions. That is also what one can expect
ex ante and is a typical distribution of trading behavior
reported in IM experiments (Luckner, 2008). Secon-
darily, figures 5, 6 and 7, show that traders used the
EIM nearly every day – even on weekends. On 30
out of 40 market days traders used the market, which
is quite more often than what one may expect. This
indicated that the motivation of traders does not de-
crease over time which is a very nice result regarding
research question 1c.
3.1.2 Survey Results for motivational Aspects
After the innovation workshop at EnBW, workshop
participants were asked by a paper based survey about
their opinion using an EIM at EnBW. The overall re-
sponse rate of the 110 participants was at 69 %, which
is representing. To indicate the related research ques-
tions 1d-e, we focus on two questions from the sur-
vey. One question was intended to get an estimate
about how employees approve the usage of an EIM.
They answered via a 5 point Likert scale where 5
represented “very good” whereas 1 represented “not
good”. On average, employees answered with 3.54, a
median of 4.00, a variance of 0.86 and a standard error
of 0.93. This indicates that the employees positively
judge the approach of using an EIM for innovation as-
sessment. This results supported by the high trading
activity presented in the previous section and the over-
all motivation of employees in using an EIM and indi-
cates, that employees approve the usage of EIM (Re-
search Question 1d). Furthermore, another question
asked about the employees’ motivation using an EIM.
Similarly, employees were expected to answer via a
5 point Likert scale where 5 represents “very high”
and 1 indicates “very low”. On average, employees
answered with 2.85 whereas the median was 3.00.
This indicates that the motivation using an EIM for a
longer time is perceived as neutral (research question
1e). Nevertheless, as results in section 3.1.1 indicate
a high motivation of employees and altogether, the re-
sults show evidence for the motivation of employees
in using an EIM.
3.2 Harnessing implicit Information
A very common way to measure the accuracy of IMs
is to compare its results to an observable benchmark.
In case of sport or political events, results of IMs can
be compared to the final values of the sport event
outcome or the final values of the election (Servan-
Schreiber et al., 2004; Berg et al., 2003; Luckner,
2008; Spann and Skiera, 2004; Stathel et al., 2009).
In case of EIM, a similar final value may exist, if
sales figures or project run times are traded. In case
of using EIM without real world events such as in-
novation assessment, IMs are nevertheless applicable
even if no final value is observable (Soukhoroukova,
2007; Chen et al., 2009). In case that no observable
Figure 7: Transactions per Trader.
The results in figure 6 and 7 indicate that research
questions 1a, 1b and 1c can be answered. Concerning
1a, the trading activity is spread over the whole mar-
ket period. That is exactly what one may expect be-
fore. The expectation, that the trading activity is only
observable at the beginning of the market period, can
be denied.
Research question 1b can be answered with 35
traders. In total, 110 workshop participants were in-
vited to join the market after the innovation workshop.
About one third followed the invitation and traded
in the market. Approximately 10 traders triggered
nearly 100 transactions, whereas 10 traders performed
less than 10. The rest resides somewhere between 100
and 10 transactions. That is also what one can expect
ex ante and is a typical distribution of trading behavior
reported in IM experiments (Luckner, 2008). Secon-
darily, figures 5, 6 and 7, show that traders used the
EIM nearly every day – even on weekends. On 30
out of 40 market days traders used the market, which
is quite more often than what one may expect. This
indicated that the motivation of traders does not de-
crease over time which is a very nice result regarding
research question 1c.
3.1.2 Survey Results for Motivational Aspects
After the innovation workshop at EnBW, workshop
participants were asked by a paper based survey about
their opinion using an EIM at EnBW. The overall re-
sponse rate of the 110 participants was at 69 %, which
is representing. To indicate the related research ques-
tions 1d-e, we focus on two questions from the sur-
vey. One question was intended to get an estimate
about how employees approve the usage of an EIM.
They answered via a 5 point Likert scale where 5
represented “very good” whereas 1 represented “not
good”. On average, employees answered with 3.54, a
median of 4.00, a variance of 0.86 and a standard error
of 0.93. This indicates that the employees positively
judge the approach of using an EIM for innovation as-
sessment. This results supported by the high trading
activity presented in the previous section and the over-
all motivation of employees in using an EIM and indi-
cates, that employees approve the usage of EIM (Re-
search Question 1d). Furthermore, another question
asked about the employees’ motivation using an EIM.
Similarly, employees were expected to answer via a
5 point Likert scale where 5 represents “very high”
and 1 indicates “very low”. On average, employees
answered with 2.85 whereas the median was 3.00.
This indicates that the motivation using an EIM for a
longer time is perceived as neutral (research question
1e). Nevertheless, as results in section 3.1.1 indicate
a high motivation of employees and altogether, the re-
sults show evidence for the motivation of employees
in using an EIM.
3.2 Harnessing Implicit Information
A very common way to measure the accuracy of IMs
is to compare its results to an observable benchmark.
In case of sport or political events, results of IMs can
be compared to the final values of the sport event
outcome or the final values of the election (Servan-
Schreiber et al., 2004; Berg et al., 2003; Luckner,
2008; Spann and Skiera, 2004; Stathel et al., 2009).
In case of EIM, a similar final value may exist, if
sales figures or project run times are traded. In case
of using EIM without real world events such as in-
novation assessment, IMs are nevertheless applicable
even if no final value is observable (Soukhoroukova,
INNOVATION ASSESSMENT VIA ENTERPRISE INFORMATION MARKETS
211