In conclusion, we can notice that, in ad-
dition to the two main results of this study
−“disynchrony accounts for misunderstanding” and
“synchronisation and disynchronisation are very
quick phenomenons”− another result is the model it-
self. It proposes a link between synchrony and inter-
subjectivity by the use of dynamical system coupling:
synchrony and dynamical coupling emerge together
when agents mutually understand each other; as a
consequence synchrony account for good interaction.
We believe, this model is a start to answer the is-
sues of what is the part of dynamical coupling be-
tween agents involved in verbal interaction? What
is the part of emerging dynamics in the communica-
tion of meanings and intentions? And moreover, how
these two parts can co-exist and feed each other?
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This work has been partially financed by the Euro-
pean Project NoE SSPNet (Social Signal Processing
Network). Nothing could have been done without the
Leto/Prometheus NN simulator, lent by the Philippe
Gaussier’s team (ETIS lab, Cergy-Pontoise, France).
REFERENCES
Auvray, M., Lenay, C., and Stewart, J. (2009). Perceptual
interactions in a minimalist virtual environment. New
ideas in psychology, 27:32–47.
Bernieri, F. J. (1988). Coordinated movement and rapport
in teacher-student interactions. Journal of Nonverbal
Behavior, 12(2):120–138.
Chammat, M., Foucher, A., Nadel, J., and Dubal, S. (2010).
Reading sadness beyond human faces. Brain Re-
search, In Press, Accepted Manuscript:–.
Condon, W. S. (1976). An analysis of behavioral organisa-
tion. Sign Language Studies, 13:285–318.
Condon, W. S. and Ogston, W. D. (1966). Sound film
analysis of normal and pathological behavior patterns.
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 143:338–
347.
Dubal, S., Jouvent, A. F. R., and Nadel, J. (2010). Human
brain spots emotion in non humanoid robots. Social
Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, in press:–.
Ducan, S. (1972). Some signals and rules for taking speak-
ing turns in conversations. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 23(2):283–292.
Dumas, G., Nadel, J., Soussignan, R., Martinerie, J., and
Garnero, L. (2010). Inter-brain synchonization during
social interaction. PLoS One, 5(8):e12166.
Gaussier, P. and Cocquerez, J. (1992). Neural networks for
complex scene recognition : simulation of a visual
system with several cortical areas. In IJCNN Balti-
more, pages 233–259.
Gaussier, P. and Zrehen, S. (1994). Avoiding the world
model trap: An acting robot does not need to be so
smart! Journal of Robotics and Computer-Integrated
Manufacturing, 11(4):279–286.
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. L., and Rapson, R. L. (1993).
Emotional contagion. Current Directions in Psycho-
logical Sciences, 2:96–99.
Kendon, A. (1990). Conducting Interaction: Patterns of Be-
havior in Focused Encounters. Cambridge University
Press, Cambridge, UK.
LaFrance, M. (1979). Nonverbal synchrony and rapport:
Analysis by the cross-lag panel technique. Social Psy-
chology Quarterly, 42(1):66–70.
Matsumoto, D. and Willingham, B. (2009). Spontaneous
facial expressions of emotion in congenitally and non-
congenitally blind individuals. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 96(1):1–10.
Mertan, B., Nadel, J., and Leveau, H. (1993). New perspec-
tive in early communicative development, chapter The
effect of adult presence on communicative behaviour
among toddlers. Routledge, London, UK.
Murray, L. and Trevarthen, C. (1985). Emotional regula-
tion of interactions vetween two-month-olds and their
mothers. Social perception in infants, pages 101–125.
Nadel, J. (2002). Imitation and imitation recognition: their
functional role in preverbal infants and nonverbal
children with autism, pages 42–62. UK: Cambridge
University Press.
Nadel, J. and Tremblay-Leveau, H. (1999). Early social
cognition, chapter Early perception of social contin-
gencies and interpersonal intentionality: dyadic and
triadic paradigms, pages 189–212. Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Paolo, E. A. D., Rohde, M., and Iizuka, H. (2008). Sensi-
tivity to social contingency or stability of interaction?
modelling the dynamics of perceptual crossing. New
ideas in psychology, 26:278–294.
Pelachaud, C. (2009). Modelling multimodal expression of
emotion in a virtual agent. Philosophical Transactions
of Royal Society. Biological Science, 364:3539–3548.
Pikovsky, A., Rosenblum, M., and Kurths, J. (2001). Syn-
chronization: A Universal Concept in Nonlinear Sci-
ences. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.
Poggi, I. and Pelachaud, C. (2000). Emotional meaning
and expression in animated faces. Lecture Notes in
Computer Science, pages 182–195.
Prepin, K. and Gaussier, P. (2010). How an agent can de-
tect and use synchrony parameter of its own interac-
tion with a human? In et al., A. E., editor, COST
Action2102, Int. Traing School 2009, Active Listening
and Synchrony. LNCS 5967, pages 50–65. Springer-
Verlag, Berlin Heidelberg.
Prepin, K. and Revel, A. (2007). Human-machine interac-
tion as a model of machine-machine interaction: how
to make machines interact as humans do. Advanced
Robotics, 21(15):1709–1723.
Scherer, K. and Delplanque, S. (2009). Emotions, signal
processing, and behaviour. In Chemosensory Percep-
tion Symposium, Geneva. Firmenich.
Tronick, E., Als, H., Adamson, L., Wise, S., and Brazel-
ton, T. (1978). The infants’ response to entrapment
between contradictory messages in face-to-face inter-
actions. Journal of the American Academy of Child
Psychiatry (Psychiatrics)., 17:1–13.
Yngve, V. H. (1970). On getting a word in edgewise. In So-
ciety, C. L., editor, Papers from the 6th regional meet-
ing, pages 567–578.
ICAART 2011 - 3rd International Conference on Agents and Artificial Intelligence
34