α
3
= −i
k
1
− b
1
1+ b
2
1+ k
2
1+ b
2
−1/2
= −ic
1
(1+ c
2
)
−1/2
.
Now α
1
, α
3
and (16) lead to the relations between
the vectors
−→
c = (c
1
,c
2
,0),
−→
b = (b
1
,b
2
,0), and
−→
k =
(k
1
,k
2
,0) and the angle φ
0
summarized in Table 1.
Table 1: All solutions of the composition of two gaze-
changes. Here, + or - means that the value of the quantity in
this column is positive or negative, respectively. the other
choices of + or - results in contradictions.
Here the saccadic gaze rotation problem is different
then the one discussed in Section 3.2 since the target
is moving. We show that the vector parameters of the
smooth pursuit
−→
c are linked to the saccadic eye rota-
tion φ
0
. Since the smooth pursuit rotation angle φ
1
(t)
can be considered known (efference copy or anticipa-
tory mechanisms), only one internal parameter c
1
(t)
is needed to describe the retinal image transformation
during the smooth-pursuit movement of the confor-
mal camera. Thus, the results we present in this sec-
tion should support the fact that smooth pursuit and
saccades are not independent (Erkelens, 2006). We
intend to use the compositions of gaze changes we
presented in this section to model the eye movement
sequences, see (Quaia et al., 2010) for example.
4 CONCLUSIONS
The conformal camera provides a computational
framework that has the unique capability of develop-
ing algorithms for processing visual information dur-
ing the exploratory motion of a camera with anthro-
pomorphic visual sensors that resemble a sequence of
saccades interlaced with fixations. In particular, we
presented algorithmic steps based on the front-end
neural processes of the perisaccadic perception that
are using the oculomotor command of the impending
saccade to shift stimuli receptive fields in cortical ar-
eas to their future postsaccadic locations. This shift
is thought to underlie the scene remapping of the cur-
rent foveal frame to the frame at the upcoming sac-
cade target and may help acquire the visual informa-
tion without repeating, afresh, the whole process at
each fixation and maintain stability of primate percep-
tion is spite of about 200, 000 saccades produced each
day. Because the shift occurs in log-polar coordinates,
it explains perisaccadic spatial distortion. Also, the
conformal camera seems to be able to model smooth-
pursuit eye movements, as our preliminary results are
suggesting. Of course, much more work has to be
done before the system could be tested.
In addition to our model of perisaccadic percep-
tion (Section 3.2), there is one another elaborate com-
putational modeling that assumes that the flashed
stimuli receptive fields in cortical areas dynamically
change position toward the saccade target receptive
field as the result of the gain of feedback of the retino-
topically organized activity hill of the saccade target
in the oculomotor superior colliculus layer (Hamker
et al., 2008). The perceived spatial distortion of stim-
uli is the result of the cortical magnification factor of
the retinotopic mapping when the position of each
stimulus is decoded from activity of the neural en-
semble. What sets apart our modeling from the other
model is the fact that the computational efficiency is
built into the modeling process (computations with
FFT) and it accommodates other types of eye move-
ments. In the case of saccadic eye movement, this is
especially important because the occurrence of three
saccades per second and the time needed for the ocu-
lomotor system to plan and execute each saccade.
REFERENCES
de Brouwer S, Yuksel D, Blohm G, Missal M, and Lefevre
P. (2002). What triggers catch-up saccades during vi-
sual tracking? Journal of Neurophysiology, 87, 1646–
1650.
Duhamel J-R, Colby C. L, and Goldgerg M. E. (1992). The
updating of the representation of visual space in pari-
etal cortex by intended eye movements, Science 255,
90-92.
Erkelens C J. (2006). Coordination of smooth pursuit and
saccades, Vision Research, 46, 163-170.
Gollisch T and Meister M. (2010). Eye Smarter than Scien-
tists Believed: Neural Computations in Circuits of the
Retina, Neuron, 65, 150-164.
Hamker F. H, Zirnsak M, Calow D, and Lappe M.
(2008). The Peri-Saccadic Perception of Objects
and Space, PLoS Computational Biology 4(2):e31.
doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.0040031.
Henle M. (1997). Modern Geometries. The Analytical Ap-
proach, Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River, NJ.
Jones G and Singerman D. (1987). Complex Functions,
Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Klier E. M. and Angelaki D. E. (2008). Spatial Updat-
ing and the Maintenance of Visual Constancy, Neu-
roscience 156, 801-818.
Knapp A. W. (1986). Representation Theory of Semisimple
Groups: An overview Based on Examples, Princeton
University Press, Princeton, NJ.
ICINCO 2011 - 8th International Conference on Informatics in Control, Automation and Robotics
262