(node) is supported or backed up by knowledge con-
tained on the network existing before the introduc-
tion of the new node. This, quite evidently, is one
characteristic of well-planned teaching. In the sample
of 70 maps there are, however, several cases which
are nearly chain-like and very poorly connected with
D < 2 and fluxes of order Ψ ≈ 1 and Φ ≈ 1. It should
be noted that not only in poorly connected maps in-
formation flux is low. In a well-connected network,
though one which is poorly ordered or directed, the
fluxes would be also very small and the passage of
information would be nearly hindered. In terms of
the inherent logic of how concepts are introduced, this
type of situation would indicate either: 1) circular rea-
soning, or 2) constant reference backwards.
In clustering and the fluxes there are large node-
by-node variations. The large variability from node to
node indicates that there is a tendency for certain con-
cepts to gather more links than other concept would
do. A suitable quantity characterizing the relative
variation is the dispersion of the variable defined as
δX = σ
X
/X, where X is the variable’s average value
and σ
X
is the standard deviation. Interestingly, the
dispersions for D, C and fluxes Φ and Ψ given in Ta-
ble 2 show that in student maps there is more varia-
tion than in the ”master map”, which means that stu-
dent maps are not equally regular and balanced as the
master map. This of course is related to the fact that in
student maps there are abrupt changes in the informa-
tion fluxes; some concepts become very central and
much effort goes into their introduction. This, on the
other hand, is somewhat awkward for teaching, be-
cause it means that demandingness of learning may
increase in an uncontrollable way. The master map
does not have such abrupt changes; instead, it dis-
plays a rather steady flux of information throughout
the whole concept network. The results suggest that
rich concept maps not only have large values of clus-
tering and fluxes but, in addition, the node-by-node
values do not vary much. This means that all con-
cepts are roughly similar in the degree of importance
for the whole structure. This, of course, is required
from well-planned teaching, where most of the topics
discussed should appear to be of importance for a stu-
dent. Maintaining small node-by-node variability is
relatively demanding, perhaps owing to the fact that it
apparently requires evaluating the functionality of the
structure as a whole instead only of locally.
6 CONCLUSIONS
We have explored physics teacher-student plans for
the teaching of physics (electricity and magnetism),
Table 2: Average degree D, clustering C and fluxes Ψ and
Φ for master map Gm and student maps G1-G5. The dis-
persions δX of the variables X (see text) are also given.
Gm G1 G2 G3 G4 G5
D 4.18 4.24 3.47 3.06 3.18 3.18
δD 0.38 0.60 0.62 0.54 0.66 0.59
C 0.18 0.48 0.25 0.15 0.25 0.16
δC 0.90 0.69 1.28 1.94 1.39 1.91
Φ 2.82 3.31 3.00 2.76 2.97 3.12
δΦ 0.37 0.40 0.48 0.47 0.60 0.46
Ψ 2.32 2.81 2.51 2.26 2.47 2.62
δΨ 0.43 0.46 0.56 0.53 0.70 0.48
carried out in a physics-teacher preparation course.
The plans were represented in the form of concept
maps. The concept maps were made such that each
link between concepts needed to be justified either
through an experimental or modelling procedure, and
they were explained in a written report coming with
the map. The aspects of interest in these plans are the
relatedness of concepts, and how the ordering of the
concept allows the introductionof new concepts in the
maps. We have introduced here a new method, which
can be used to analyse the students’ plans so that their
inherent logic, ordering and the way to use the infor-
mation in the plans is revealed. These aspects, we
believe, are of importance for understanding and eval-
uating students’ view of how concepts are related and
how they can be introduced in teaching physics.
The structural analysis of the concept maps is
based on the identification of the basic knowledge-
ordering patterns. The pattern of most importance is
a triangular pattern, connected to the procedures of
experiments and modelling. However, the analysis of
the plans for teaching show that even in cases where
the maps have a rich set of connections, and when
concepts are well connected, the inherent logic and
the way knowledge is passed from the initial levels
to the final levels may be awkward and that there are
often abrupt changes in the information flux. Inter-
estingly, when all valid connections found in the stu-
dents’ maps are combined and reorganized, the result-
ing map shows a very regular and steady information
flux. This suggests that handling the information con-
tent is very demanding and perhaps one of the most
difficult skills for a teacher student to master. This no-
tion has direct implications for teacher education and
it also calls for methods to monitor this kind of devel-
opment. The method of analysis introduced here is a
step in this relatively unexplored direction.
KNOWLEDGE ORGANIZATION IN CONCEPT MAPS - Teacher Students' Representations of the Relatedness of
Physics Concepts
143