2 CONCEPT NETWORKS
The relational structure of concepts can be thought
as network-like node-link-node representations of the
relations between concepts. For different types of rep-
resentations there are different ways of establishing
the relations and different rules in regard to linking
the concepts, but the skeletal structure of the concept
network is always a network of nodes (i.e. concepts)
connected by links. The concept networks studied
here are done by physics teacher students for purposes
of representing how they think concepts can be intro-
duced in teaching, so that each step is justified either
on basis of experiments or model, which are both cen-
tral procedures connected to the construction and use
of knowledge. It is then natural to assume that these
procedures of the experiments and modelling play an
important role in conferring the structure of the con-
cept networks. In the operationalizing experiment
the concept is operationalized i.e. made measurable
through the pre-existing concepts. The new concept
or law is constructed sequentially, starting from the al-
ready existing ones, which provide the basis for an ex-
periment’s design and interpretation. In its most ide-
alized form the new concept (or law) C is formed on
the basis of two pre-existing concepts A and B so that
the operationalization creates C on the basis of the re-
lations A → C and B → C, but which also requires
that A and B can be related as A → B. There is then
a triangular mutual dependence A → B → C ← A.The
modelling procedures, which in the simplest cases are
often deductive procedures, produce very similar pat-
terns (Koponen and Pehkonen, 2010; Koponen and
Nousiainen, 2012). It is interesting to note cognitively
oriented studies of knowledge formation suggest that
procedures of knowledge construction and processing
may be simple ones, reducible to basic patterns, even
in those cases where the resulting structures are com-
plex. In that,triangular patterns have been recognized
as an essential feature not only in the case of func-
tional knowledge but also in information acquisition
as well as information processing (Kemp et al., 2007;
Kemp and Tenenbaum, 2008; Duong et al., 2009).
The procedures of constructing experiments and
models – connecting concepts to previously intro-
duced concepts – then provide the context or the “af-
filiations” of concepts. Concept maps where these
procedures are used to connect concepts represent
then not only the relatedness of concepts, but they
also represent how concepts are introduced in teach-
ing so that knowledge learned earlier is the basis upon
which new knowledge is built. This means that, in a
sense, these networks also represent the ”flux of in-
formation” which students have planned to take place
in their teaching. In well-planned teaching there
should naturally be a regular flux of information (for
evenly paced learning of new knowledge), but no
unnecessary abrupt changes in that flux (otherwise
the demand to assimilate new knowledge would vary
much); moreover, uncontrollable reductions in the
flow should be hindered to prevent the impression that
learned knowledge would not be needed in further
learning (Koponen and Nousiainen, 2012).
3 THE METHOD OF ANALYSIS
The properties of ordering and information flux in
the concept maps are explored by using the quantities
based on theory of directed ordered networks (DOGs)
(Karrer and Newman, 2009; Goni et al., 2010). Be-
cause we are interested in the connectedness and in-
formation fluxes in the maps, we use the follow-
ing quantities (detailed mathematical definitions are
given in Table 1):
1. The degree k
i
of the node, which is the number
of the incoming and outgoing links k
in
and k
out
,
respectively. The average degree is denoted by D;
2. The clustering coefficient C
i
, which is the ratio
of triangles to all the triply connected neighbours
around a given concept;
3. Flux into the nodes (Flux-I) Φ
i
, which gives the
total number of links terminating at the givennode
k from all levels j < k;
4. Flux around the nodes (Flux-A) Ψ
k
, which gives
the total number of links bypassing the given node
k from all levels j < k.
In the present case, fluxes Φ and Ψ directly describe
the ”information” flowing from the previously in-
troduced nodes to ones introduced later (Karrer and
Newman, 2009). The most important aspect of the
concept maps made by teacher students is their or-
dering and appreciably large clustering with C ≈ 0.2.
Both features follow from the procedures that are used
to connect concepts. It is of interest to develop a sim-
ple model, which captures these features. In addition
to these features the model should also reproduce the
steady node-by-node information flows Ψ and Φ.
4 THE MODEL
The cases studied and modelled here consist of 8 stu-
dent maps, all of which are rather rich in their struc-
ture. The number of the concepts was limited to n=34
most central concepts (in electromagnetism). Details
KEOD2012-InternationalConferenceonKnowledgeEngineeringandOntologyDevelopment
144