Table 1: The examples of iteration times for convergence.
from
to
Orthography Phonology Semantics
Category 1547.1 1965.3 48.4
Unitary
302.3 171.8 228.1
Orthography
78.2 359.8 378.20
Phonology
966.4 136.1 294.1
Semantics
577.9 288.5 74.4
ognize the shape of a dog exposured in one’s retina
as ’dog’. The category condition might be considered
that subjects and patients could recognize this visual
image of a dog as an animal. We could interpret the
unitary condition as that subjects and patients would
have been recognized a ’dog’ per se. In this way, we
could interpret the three conditions we adopted in this
paper as the human models of recognition described
here. The category condition could be considered as
the one which utilized the loop between the output
and the cleanup layers the best among three condi-
tions. Thus, it seems that the category conditionmight
be the model of category judgement. In addition to
this, we observed one of the category specific disor-
ders in the destruction experiment which destroyed
the mutual connections between the output and the
cleanup layers. This results should not be considered
as accidental artifacts of the computer simulations.
Attractor networks could be applied to the triangle
models of word reading as well. Although the origi-
nal triangle model (Sidenberg and McClelland, 1989)
has self recurrent arrows, each arrow could not be re-
garded as an attractor network. This study tried to
interpret that the triangle model would be consisted
of all 15 attractor networks in total.
5 CONCLUSIONS
In spite of the simplicity, the attractor neural network
could describe several symptoms of neuropsychol-
ogy. This is one of major advantages of this model.
The possibility to explain the double–dissociation be-
tween animate and inanimate objects should be dis-
cussed further in separate papers. However, there
still are possibilities for this model to account for the
double–dissociation between animate and inanimate
objects. The difference between intra– and inter– cor-
relations shown in fig. 1 might cause the category
specificity, because one category has higher inner–
category correlations than that of the other category.
In this study, we adopted non–dichotomous memory
representations whose correlation matrix of micro-
features such as Fig.1 and Fig.6. These representa-
tions can be considered as one of the major advan-
tages of the model.This kind of object representations
might emerge as one aspect of category specific mem-
ory disorders.
REFERENCES
Caramazza, A. and Shelton, J. (1998). Domain spe-
cific knowledge system in the brain: The animate–
inanimate distinction. Journal of Cognitive Neuro-
science, 10(1):1–34.
Devlin, J., Gonnerman, L., Andersen, E., and Seidenberg,
M. (1998). Category-specific semantic deficits in fo-
cal and widespred brain damage: A computational ac-
count. journal of cognitive neuroscience, 10(1):77–
94.
Farah, M. J. and McClelland, J. L. (1991). A computational
model of semantic memory impairment: Modality
specificity and emergent category specificity. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: General, 120(4):339–
357.
Plaut, D. C. (2001). A connectionist approach to word
reading and acquired dyslexia: Extension to sequen-
tial processing. In Chirstiansen, M. H. and Char-
ter, N., editors, Connectionist Psycholinguistics, chap-
ter 8, pages 244–278. Ablex Publishing, Westport, CT.
Plaut, D. C., McClelland, J. L., and Seidenberg, M. S.
(1995). Reading exception words adn pseudowords:
Are two routes really necessary? In Levy, J. P.,
Bairaktaris, D., Bullinaria, J. A., and Cairns, P., edi-
tors, Connectionist Models of Memory and Language,
pages 145–159. University College London Press,
London.
Plaut, D. C., McClelland, J. L., Seidenberg, M. S., and
Patterson, K. (1996). Understanding normal and
impaired word reading: Computational principles
in quasi-regular domains. Psychological Review,
103:56–115.
Sidenberg, M. and McClelland, J. (1989). A distributed, de-
velopmental model of word recognition and naming.
Psychological Review, 96:523–568.
Tyler, L., Moss, H., Durrant-Peatfield, M., and Levy, J.
(2000). Conceptual structure and the structure of
concepts: A distributed account of category-specific
deficits. Brain and Language, 75:195–231.
Warrington, E. and McCarthy, R. (1983). Category specific
access dysphasia. Brain, 106:859–878.
Warrington, E. and McCarthy, R. (1994). Multiple mean-
ing systems in the brain: A case for visual semantics.
Neuropsychologica, 32:1465–1473.
Warrington, E. and Shallice, T. (1984a). Category-specific
semantic impairment. Brain, 107:829–854.
Warrington, E. K. (1981). Neuropsychological studies of
verbal semantic systems. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B,
295:411–423.
Warrington, E. K. and McCarthy, R. (1987). Categories of
knowledge further fracitonations and an attempted in-
tegration. Brain, 110:1273–1296.
Warrington, E. K. and Shallice, T. (1984b). Category spe-
cific semantic impairment. Brain, 107:829–854.
AttractorNeuralNetworksforSimulatingDyslexicPatients'Behavior
587