1991). The visual acuity of subjects in Smith’s
experiment did not decrease much. Therefore, the
distance from an emerging object in our experiment
may not have been a problem and was correctly
viewed by subjects.
Meanwhile, Patterson reported that there should
be a problem in only a near-eye display and that the
accommodation-vergence mismatch likely would
not occur under most stereoscopic display viewing
conditions because of the depth of field (Patterson,
2009). Patterson (2009) and Wang and Ciuffreda
(2006) found that the depth of field was large, and
they stated that the average total depth of focus was
on the order of 1.0 diopter (Wang anda Ciuffreda,
2006). Based on this value, the range of total depth
of field would be from a distance of about 0.1m in
front of a fixed point to about 0.17m behind the
fixed point of 0.5m. For a fixed distance of 1 m, the
total depth of field would be from a distance of
about 0.33m in front of the point to about 1.0 m
behind the point. For a fixed distance of 2 m, the
total depth of field would be from about 1m in front
of the point to an infinite distance behind the fixed
point.
They also reported that the depth of field was
affected in various ways by the pupil diameter and
resolution.
Some researchers found that pupil diameter will
be slightly over 6 mm for a luminance level of
0.03cd/m2 and near to 2 mm for a luminance level
of 300cd/m2. For each millimeter of decrease in
pupil size, the depth of field increases by about 0.12
diopters (Patterson, 2009).
Therefore, the value of accommodation can be in
the range of the depth of field in our experiment.
In the future research, we plan further
investigations concerning the influence of age, pupil
diameter, the illuminance of the experimental
environment, and the luminance of visual targets.
5 CONCLUSIONS
In this experiment, we simultaneously measured
accommodation and convergence of subjects
viewing real object and 3D video clips that showed
exactly the same motion as in real objects. We did
not confirm the existence of discrepancy between
lens accommodation and convergence. Therefore,
we believe it is inconclusive that symptoms such as
eye fatigue and 3D sickness are not caused by this
discrepancy but other factors. We plan to perform
further investigations and studies of other variables.
REFERENCES
M. Lambooij, W. IJsselsteijn, M. Fortuin, and I.
Heynderickx, 2009. Visual Discomfort and Visual
Fatigue of Stereoscopic Displays: A Review, J.
Imaging Sci. Technol. Vol. 53, 03.
M. Miyao, et al., 1996. Visual accommodation and subject
performance during a stereographic object task using
liquid crystal shutters. Ergonomics, Vol.39, 11, 1294-
1309.
K. Ukai, P. A. Howarth, 2008. Visual fatigue caused by
viewing stereoscopic motion images: background,
theories, and observation, Displays 29, 2, 106-116.
H. Hori, T. Shiomi, et al., 2011. Comparison of
accommodation and convergence by simultaneous
measurements during 2D and 3D vision gaze. FORMA
Specieal Issue.
G. Smith, 1991. Relation between Spherical Refractive
Error and Visual Acuity, Optometry Vision Science,
Vol. 68, No.8, 591-598.
R. Patterson, 2009. Human factors of stereo displays: An
Update. Journal of SID, Vol.17, No.12, 987-996.
B. Wang and K. J. Ciuffreda, 2006. Depth of focus of the
human eye: Theory and clinical applications. Surv.
Ophthal. 51, 75.
ComparisonofSimultaneousMeasurementWhileViewingRealObjectsand3DVideoClips
545