correspondence between model stimulus intensity
and real stimulus intensity is lacking. iii) To
simulate adaptation at different spatial positions with
a fixed visual-auditory disparity. In the present
work, the adaptation phase consisted in presenting
a visual and an auditory stimulus in fixed spatial
positions. In future, the network could be trained by
presenting two cross-modal stimuli with assigned
spatial difference but variable locations (as in
experimental studies). iv) To mimic aftereffect
generalization on even wider range of frequencies
(i.e., four-octave range as reported by (Frissen et al
2005)). This would require to augment the number
of neurons in the auditory layer. v) To explore the
involvement of other potential factors (e.g. attentive
factors) in producing different aftereffect
generalizations. Focusing selective attention to
either modality (visual or auditory) during the
adaptation phase might impact the overall size of the
aftereffects (Frissen et al., 2003). Selective attention
in the model could be simulated via
facilitatory/inhibitory effects on neuron activation
and/or via increase/decrease of synaptic learning
rate.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This work has been supported by the 2007-2013
Emilia-Romagna Regional Operational Programme
of the European Regional Development Fund.
REFERENCES
Bertelson, P. and Radeau, M. (1981). Cross-modal bias
and perceptual fusion with auditory-visual spatial
discordance. Perception & Psychophysics, 29(6),
pp.578-584.
Bertelson, P. A., G. (1998). Automatic visual bias of
perceived auditory location. Psychonomic Bullettin &
Review, 5(3), pp.482-489.
Cuppini, C., Magosso, E., Rowland, B., Stein, B. and
Ursino, M. (2012). Hebbian mechanisms help explain
development of multisensory integration in the
superior colliculus: a neural network model.
Biological Cybernetics, 106(11-12), pp.691-713.
Cuppini, C., Stein, B. E., Rowland, B. A., Magosso, E.
and Ursino, M. (2011). A computational study of
multisensory maturation in the superior colliculus
(SC). Experimental Brain Research, 213(2-3), pp.341-
349.
Ernst, M. O. and Bulthoff, H. H. (2004). Merging the
senses into a robust percept. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 8(4), pp.162-169.
Frissen, I., Vroomen, J., De Gelder, B. and Bertelson, P.
(2003). The aftereffects of ventriloquism: are they
sound-frequency specific? Acta Psychologica, 113(3),
pp.315-327.
Frissen, I., Vroomen, J., De Gelder, B. and Bertelson, P.
(2005). The aftereffects of ventriloquism:
generalization across sound-frequencies. Acta
Psychologica, 118(1-2), pp.93-100.
Fukushima, K. (1980). Neocognitron: a self organizing
neural network model for a mechanism of pattern
recognition unaffected by shift in position. Biological
Cybernetics, 36(4), pp.193-202.
Haykin, S. (1994). Neural Networks: A Comprehensive
Foundation, New York, Macmillan College
Publishing Company.
Kohonen, T. (1982). Self-Organized Formation of
Topologically Correct Feature Maps. Biological
Cybernetics, 43, pp.59-69.
Kohonen, T. (1995). Sel-Organizing Maps, Berlin,
Springer-Verlag.
Kohonen, T. and Hari, R. (1999). Where the abstract
feature maps of the brain might come from. Trends in
Neurosciences, 22(3), pp.135-139.
Lewald, J. (2002). Rapid adaptation to auditory-visual
spatial disparity. Learning & Memory, 9(5), pp.268-
278.
Magosso, E. (2010). Integrating information from vision
and touch: a neural network modeling study. IEEE
Transactions on Information Technology in
Biomedicine, 14(3), pp.598-612.
Magosso, E., Cuppini, C., Serino, A., Di Pellegrino, G.
and Ursino, M. (2008). A theoretical study of
multisensory integration in the superior colliculus by a
neural network model. Neural Networks, 21(6),
pp.817-829.
Magosso, E., Cuppini, C. and Ursino, M. (2012). A neural
network model of ventriloquism effect and aftereffect.
PloS one, 7(8), e42503, pp.1-19.
Magosso, E., Serino, A., Di Pellegrino, G. and Ursino, M.
(2010a). Crossmodal links between vision and touch
in spatial attention: a computational modelling study.
Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience, ID
304941, pp.1-13.
Magosso, E., Ursino, M., Di Pellegrino, G., Ladavas, E.
and Serino, A. (2010b). Neural bases of peri-hand
space plasticity through tool-use: insights from a
combined computational-experimental approach.
Neuro-psychologia, 48(3), pp.812-830.
Magosso, E., Zavaglia, M., Serino, A., Di Pellegrino, G.
and Ursino, M. (2010c). Visuotactile representation of
peripersonal space: a neural network study. Neural
Computation, 22(1), pp.190-243.
Miyake, S. and Fukushima, K. (1984). A neural network
model for the mechanism of feature-extraction. A self-
organizing network with feedback inhibition.
Biological Cybernetics, 50(5), pp.377-384.
Rajan, R., Aitkin, L. M. and Irvine, D. R. (1990).
Azimuthal sensitivity of neurons in primary auditory
cortex of cats. II. Organization along frequency-band
strips. Journal of Neurophysiology, 64(3), pp.888-902.
IJCCI2013-InternationalJointConferenceonComputationalIntelligence
368