visual tasks.
Figure 1: Grand-average ERP signals in an oddball
paradigm with geometric figures (a), angry salesman (b)
and a sentence (c) task with target and standard stimulus.
The P300 waveform looks different in each
electrode site. The signal from C3 is broad and starts
after 300 ms from the event. The waveforms from
Cz and C4 are more complex and have three local
maxima. Interesting is the fact that the P300 is
similar in all performed tasks. The values of
latencies and amplitudes of P300 waveform were
calculated and are presented in Table 1.
Table 1: P300 latencies and amplitudes for grand-average
visual oddball ERPs from target stimulus for three tasks.
Task Electrode
P300
Latency [ms]
P300
Amplitude [μV]
Figures
C3 344 4.54
Cz 391 4.56
C4 352 3.88
Salesman
C3 348 4.79
Cz 391 4.57
C4 348 3.88
Sentence
C3 340 4.80
Cz 391 4.56
C4 355 3.81
The latencies of P300 at peak maximum are in
the range 340 ms – 355 ms at C3 and C4, while at
Cz it is about 390 ms. The amplitudes of P300 are in
the range 4.54 μV – 4.80 μV at C3 and Cz, and
about 3.85 μV at C4. The values are comparable in
all tasks. These results indicate that emotional
picture of angry salesman did not influence the
changes of the processing of sensory information in
studied group of students. Latencies and amplitudes
of P300 waveform was also not affected by reading
illogical word in the sentence task. There were no
changes in latency of amplitude of P300, because the
task was not very complex.
4 CONCLUSIONS
P300 waveform obtained in visual ERP study with
oddball paradigm (target and standard stimulus)
registered at C3 was different from those at C4 and
Cz positions. It was broad and had higher amplitude
than C4 and Cz. The character of signals was
independent on kind of task, what seems that the
emotions brought out were so weak that they didn’t
activate the structures in brain responsible for
processing the emotional information. However this
research requires further studies on more
participants.
REFERENCES
Bayer, M., Sommer, W., Schacht, A., 2010. Reading
emotional words within sentences: The impact of
arousal and valence on event-related potentials,
International Journal of Psychophysiology 78.
Duncan, C. C., Barry R. J., Connolly J. F., Fischer, C.,
Michie, P. T., Naatanen, R., Reinvang I., Van Petten,
C., 2009. Event-related potentials in clinical research:
Guidelines for eliciting, recording aand quantifying
mismatch negativity, P300 and N400, Clinical
Neurophysiology 120.
Groh-Bordin, C., Zimmer, H. D., Ecker, U. K. H., 2006.
Has the butcher on the bus dyed his hair? When color
changes modulate ERP correlates of familiarity and
recollection, NeuroImage 32.
Heinze, H. J., Munte, T. F., Kutas, M., Butler, S. R.,
Naatanen, R., Nuwer, M. R., Goodin, D. S., 1999.
Cognitive event-related potentials. In G. Deuschl and
A. Eisen, Recommendations for the Practice of
Clinical Neurophysiology: Guidelines of the
International Federation of Clinical Physiology (EEG
Suppl. 52), Elsevier Science B.V.
Picton, T. W., Bentin, S., Berg, P., Donchin, E., Hillyard,
S. A., Johnson, R., J. R., Miller, G. A., Ritter, W.,
Ruchkin, D. S., RUGG, M. D., Taylor, M. J., 2000.
Guidelines for using human event-related potentials to
study cognition: Recording standards and publication
criteria, Psychophysiology 37.
Veiga, H., Deslandes, A., Cagy, M., McDowell, K.,
Pompeu, F., Piedade, R., Ribeiro, P, 2004. Visual
event-related potential (P300), Arq Neuropsiquiatr 62.