Separation of Auditory Evoked Responses to the Right- and Left-Ear
Inputs
S. Kuriki
1
, H. Kurumaya
2
, K. Tanaka
2
and Y. Uchikawa
2
1
Research Institute for Science and Technology, Tokyo Denki University, Inzai, Japan
2
Department of Science and Engineering, Tokyo Denki University, Hatoyama, Japan
1 INTRODUCTION
In the clinical examination of auditory cortical
function, it is desirable to observe the evoked
responses separately to the right- and left-ear inputs
during binaural stimulation. Due to the dominance
of contralateral auditory pathway, it is expected that
the right ear response represents the function of the
left central auditory system and vice versa.
However, the response to the input of the same-ear
side is also included due to the conduction through
ipsilateral pathway. Difficulty exists how to
discriminate the mixed responses in the auditory
cortex into their input channels. From a view point
of basic science, separate observation of the two
ears’ responses to different acoustic stimuli such as
tones and speech sounds may be interesting to study
the hemispheric difference in the auditory function
(Zatorre and Belin, 2001). Here, we developed a
novel method for the separation of evoked responses
based on synchronous and asynchronous averaging
of signals.
2 PRINCIPLE
In the conventional averaging of evoked responses,
recorded signals, which are mixed with biological,
environmental and sensor noises, are averaged with
a trigger time-locked (synchronous) to the stimulus
epochs. This is based on the fact that noise signals
are asynchronous to the stimuli and thus attenuated
by averaging. Transient evoked responses are
usually phasic, being composed of several peaks
changing in polarity, e.g., P1, N1, P2, N2
components, of auditory response. It is expected that
such phasic responses are attenuated in amplitude
after averaging over many epochs if we use such
triggers that are asynchronous to the stimulus epochs.
This attenuation should be maximal when the jitter
of the trigger time extends to the whole response
period that may contain multiple peaks. For the
relevant response to be observed, synchronous
triggers are used besides the ongoing asynchronous
averaging, reducing the noise but not the signal.
We prepared two sets of time series of onset-
triggers in the simulation, where each set had
random onset-to-onset intervals varying in a period
of 500 ms. Figure 1a shows the distribution of the
onset intervals of the first trigger series, where bars
indicate the number of triggers included within time
bins of 50 ms width. The trigger onsets were
distributed uniformly in the 500 ms period, keeping
random intervals. It was confirmed that the second
trigger series prepared also had similar distribution.
(a)
(b)
Figure 1: (a) Distribution of the onset-to-onset intervals of
the triggers used in the simulation and (b) waveforms of
the signals averaged using synchronous and asynchronous
triggers.
As the signal, we used a waveform of auditory
evoked response consisting of several peak
components superposed with white noises. Signal
epochs of 500 ms length were generated repeatedly
at the onset of triggers of the first series. Averaging
Kuriki S., Kurumaya H., Tanaka K. and Uchikawa Y..
Separation of Auditory Evoked Responses to the Right- and Left-Ear Inputs.
Copyright
c
2013 SCITEPRESS (Science and Technology Publications, Lda.)
was performed across 200 signal epochs using the
same trigger set and also using the second set of
triggers that were not time-locked to the signal
epochs. The result (Fig. 1b) showed clear waveform
of the original response for the synchronous
averaging, while no visible response was left for the
asynchronous averaging.
3 EXPERIMENTAL
We have carried out magnetoencephalograpy (MEG)
measurements of dichotic stimulation using a speech
sound /a/ to the right ear a 600-Hz pure tone to the
left ear of 500 ms length. The sound intensity was 70
dB SPL in both ears. Inter-stimulus intervals were
randomized between 250 and 750 ms, where the
right and left sounds were synchronized to two sets
of triggers having random onset times with no
correlation between them. Three male and a female
subjects of 22-25 years old participated in the
measurements. A whole head 122-channel magneto-
meter was used to measure MEG signals.
Averaging was performed across 200 epochs of
the recorded signals using the trigger set
synchronous to the tone epochs and also using the
other trigger set synchronous to the vowel epochs.
The averaged signals were filtered to 2-40 Hz offline.
The responses obtained at 6 channel sensors over the
right and left temporal areas were averaged to
represent the hemisphere response. The waveform of
the response was expressed as the root-mean-
squared magnitude.
4 RESULS AND DISCUSSION
Figure 2 shows the waveforms of the responses
obtained by synchronous averaging. The contra-
lateral vowel response was larger than ipsilateral
tone response in the left hemisphere, as expected.
However, the contralateral tone response was
reduced to a comparable magnitude to the ipsilateral
vowel response in the right hemisphere. The results
suggest that at short-interval stimulation (0.25-0.75
s) the vowel response in the left hemisphere does not
attenuate much, as compared with the tone response
and the responses in the right hemisphere.
MEG sensors in the temporal area can detect
exclusively the auditory cortical response in the right
or left hemisphere due to highly spatially localized
magnetic signals. So far, steady state responses that
are tagged with modulation frequency (Tononi et al.,
1998) have been used to discriminate the responses
to two-channel inputs. In addition to this, the present
method allows us to examine transient responses,
representing higher auditory functions, separately to
the speech and tone sounds in the two hemispheres.
It may be used in dichotic listening test in diagnosis
of auditory processing disorder (Moncrieff, 2006).
Figure 2: Waveforms of separated grand-mean responses
(n = 4) in the right and left hemispheres evoked by vowel
and pure tone stimuli, which were delivered to the right
and left ears, respectively.
REFERENCES
Moncrieff D. W., 2006. Identification of binaural
integration deficits in children with the Competing
Words Subtest: standard score versus interaural
asymmetry. Int J Audiol. 45:546-454.
Tononi G., Srinivasan R., Russel D. P., Edelman G. M.,
1998. Investigating neural correlates of conscious
perception by frequency-tagged neuromagnetic
responses. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 95:3198-3203.
Zatorre R. J., Belin P., 2001. Spectral and temporal
processing in human auditory cortex. Cereb Cortex.
11:946-953.