celeration parameter, which derive from the gait fre-
quency and the slightly strolling walking style of the
subject. Overall, DE curves show promising stability
and at the same time reactivity to behavior modifica-
tions and seem to adequately describe the qualitative
commitment of people towards objects like public dis-
plays.
7 CONCLUSIONS
AND OUTLOOK
In this paper, we have presented an approach towards
a higher level interpretative description of behavior
to express engagement and commitment of via detec-
tion of behavior changes. Such an approach can never
claim to be able to predict the exact focus of atten-
tion of a person but can only try to provide a model
which approximates reality through iterative refine-
ment. The more we accomplish a detailed description
of behavior and context, the better we will perform in
interpreting human behavior. Yet, the proposed meth-
ods may provide a first step towards a behavior-based
attention estimation.
REFERENCES
Bitgood, S. (2006). Not another step! economy of move-
ment and pedestrian choice point behavior in shopping
malls. Environment And Behavior, 38(3):394–405.
Blank, M., Gorelick, L., Shechtman, E., Irani, M., and
Basri, R. (2005). Actions as space-time shapes. In
Computer Vision, 2005. ICCV 2005. Tenth IEEE Inter-
national Conference on, volume 2, pages 1395–1402
Vol. 2.
Bobick, A. and Davis, J. (2001). The recognition of human
movement using temporal templates. Pattern Analy-
sis and Machine Intelligence, IEEE Transactions on,
23(3):257–267.
Bongers, K. C., Dijksterhuis, A., and Spears, R. (2009).
Self-esteem regulation after success and failure to at-
tain unconsciously activated goals. Journal of Exper-
imental Social Psychology, 45(3):468 – 477.
Candamo, J., Shreve, M., Goldgof, D., Sapper, D., and Kas-
turi, R. (2010). Understanding transit scenes: A sur-
vey on human behavior-recognition algorithms. Intel-
ligent Transportation Systems, IEEE Transactions on,
11(1):206–224.
Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., and Heward, W. L. (2007).
Applied behavior analysis. Pearson/Merrill-Prentice
Hall, Upper Saddle River and N.J, 2 edition.
Dijksterhuis, A. and Aarts, H. (2010). Goals, Attention,
and (Un)Consciousness. Annual Review of Psychol-
ogy, 61(1).
Guevara, J. E. and Umemuro, H. (2010). Unobtrusive esti-
mation of psychological states based on human move-
ment observation. e-Minds, 2(6).
Hietanen, J. (2002). Social attention orienting integrates vi-
sual information from head and body orientation. Psy-
chological Research, 66(3):174–179.
Hoffman, J. E. and Subramaniam, B. (1995). The role of vi-
sual attention in saccadic eye movements. Perception
& psychophysics, 57(6):787–795.
Hunt, A. R. and Kingstone, A. (2003). Covert and overt
voluntary attention: linked or independent? Brain
research. Cognitive brain research, 18(1):102–105.
Ji, X. and Liu, H. (2010). Advances in view-invariant hu-
man motion analysis: A review. Systems, Man, and
Cybernetics, Part C: Applications and Reviews, IEEE
Transactions on, 40(1):13–24.
Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Prentice-Hall,
Englewood Cliffs, N.J.
Knudsen, E. I. (2007). Fundamental components of atten-
tion. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 30(1):57–78.
Moore, T. and Fallah, M. (2001). Control of eye move-
ments and spatial attention. Proceedings of the Na-
tional Academy of Sciences of the United States of
America, 98(3):1273–1276.
M
¨
uller, H. J. and Rabbitt, P. M. (1989). Reflexive and vol-
untary orienting of visual attention: time course of
activation and resistance to interruption. Journal of
experimental psychology. Human perception and per-
formance, 15(2):315–330.
Perry, R. J. (2000). The neurology of saccades and covert
shifts in spatial attention: An event-related fMRI
study. Brain, 123(11):2273–2288.
Peursum, P., Venkatesh, S., and West, G. (2007). Tracking-
as-recognition for articulated full-body human motion
analysis. In Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition,
2007. CVPR ’07. IEEE Conference on, pages 1–8.
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Jour-
nal of Experimental Psychology, 32(1):3–25.
Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., Dascola, I., and Umilt
´
a, C.
(1987). Reorienting attention across the horizontal
and vertical meridians: evidence in favor of a premo-
tor theory of attention. Neuropsychologia, 25(1A):31–
40.
Shi, Y., Bobick, A., and Essa, I. (2006). Learning tem-
poral sequence model from partially labeled data. In
Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition, 2006 IEEE
Computer Society Conference on, volume 2, pages
1631–1638.
Smith, K. C., Ba, S. O., Odobez, J.-M., and Gatica-Perez, D.
(2006). Tracking attention for multiple people: Wan-
dering visual focus of attention estimation. Idiap-RR
Idiap-RR-40-2006, IDIAP. Submitted for publication.
Wickens, C. D. and McCarley, J. S. (2008). Applied atten-
tion theory. CRC Press, Boca Raton.
Yakiyama, Y., Thepvilojanapong, N., Iwai, M., Mihirogi,
O., Umeda, K., and Tobe, Y. (2009). Observing real-
world attention by a laser scanner. IPSJ Online Trans-
actions, 2:93–106.
PhyCS2014-InternationalConferenceonPhysiologicalComputingSystems
90