The Effect of Music on the Level of Mental
Concentration and its Temporal Change
Fumiya Mori
1
, Fatemeh Azadi Naghsh
2
and Taro Tezuka
3
1
Department of Children and Families, City of Takasaki, 370-8501 Takasaki, Japan
2
Graduate School of Library, Information and Media Studies, University of Tsukuba, 305-0821 Tsukuba, Japan
3
Faculty of Library, Information and Media Science, University of Tsukuba, 305-0821 Tsukuba, Japan
Keywords:
Concentration, Attention, Music, Temporal Pattern.
Abstract:
Concentration is one of the most important factor in determining the efficiency of learning. There has not
been, however, much systematic research on controlling the level of concentration. We therefore examined the
effect of an external factor, namely playing music, on the performance on a task that requires much attention.
We compared three conditions: music that the subject likes, music that the subject is not familiar with, and
silence. The result showed that listening to music that the subject likes do increase the performance level.
Also, we discovered that there exist different temporal patterns in the change of performance. The result also
indicated a relationship between the temporal pattern in concentration and the external factor.
1 INTRODUCTION
The efficiency of learning is highly dependent on the
mental state that the person is in. When the person is
highly concentrated, he can understand and memorize
many complicated concepts in a short period of time,
whereas when he is not concentrated, he cannot learn
as effectively.
Although the change in the level of concentration
is in part a spontaneous process, it is nevertheless af-
fected by external factors as well. The effect is ev-
ident when one considers how productive he can be
when the deadline is approaching. Surprisingly, there
has not been much work in changing the environmen-
tal factors to create higher performance in learning.
Various books describing techniques to enhance
concentration have been published, but most of them
are solely based on personal experiences rather than
objective experiments. For example, many students
listen to music while they study at home. They seem
to have learned from their experience that music in-
creases their level of concentration. There has not
been, however, much work on evaluating if music
could be used to control the level of concentration.
In contrast to education, there is an extensive
amount of research in the field of sports science,
where all efforts are put to win a competition (Katch
and Katch 1999). Physical and mental quantities of
athletes are measured and analyzed to reach highest
performance. The approach here is more objective
and quantitative. If such approach was successfully
applied to education the effect would be tremendous.
One of the ways to measure the effect of music on
learning is to measure how concentrated the subject
is. In this paper, we describe a system aimed at mea-
suring the level of concentration based on the perfor-
mance of the subject. We compare three conditions,
namely playing music that the subject likes, playing
music that the subject is not familiar with, and silence.
The overall performance of the subject and the tempo-
ral change in the performance level will be compared
and analyzed, to objectively evaluate if music have
positive or negative effect on concentration.
The rest of the paper is organized as follows. In
Section 2 we describe our model of concentration, and
factors that would affect it. Section 3 is on implemen-
tation of our system. It is followed by Section 4 where
we illustrates the design and the result of the experi-
ment. Section 5 is on related work. Finally, Section 6
concludes the paper.
2 MODEL
For a person to learn something new, he/she has to be
attentive to the content of learning. When a certain
amount of contents are to be learned, sustaining at-
34
Mori F., Naghsh F. and Tezuka T..
The Effect of Music on the Level of Mental Concentration and its Temporal Change.
DOI: 10.5220/0004791100340042
In Proceedings of the 6th International Conference on Computer Supported Education (CSEDU-2014), pages 34-42
ISBN: 978-989-758-020-8
Copyright
c
2014 SCITEPRESS (Science and Technology Publications, Lda.)
tention for a certain amount of time is a requirement.
In this paper, we define concentration as a sus-
tained high level of attention to the task. In order to
measure it, we therefore employ a task that requires
attention. Figure 1 illustrates the overall construction
of our model. Arrows indicate causal relationships.
Concentration is determined by internal parameters,
which is affected by external factors. By changing
the external factors, we assume it possible to change
the level of concentration (or attention).
Figure 1: External factors and internal parameters.
In this paper, we mainly focus on how music and
time affect the performance level on a task that re-
quires concentration. We discuss the model in more
detail in the following subsections.
2.1 Attention and Learning
In cognitive science, much work has been done to ex-
plain how attention affects different aspects of learn-
ing. The main position is even stated that there is no
learning without attention (Schmidt 1995). A num-
ber of researchers have argued that different types of
learning (e.g., explicit and implicit) depend on atten-
tion (Tomlin and Villa 1994).
Studies have inferred that attentional mechanisms
is essential for all learning even for simple percep-
tual task (Ahissar and Hochstein 2002) and that more
complex learning requires more attention (Schmidt
1995). One established principle of visual attention
is that the harder a task is, the more attentional re-
sources are used to perform the task and the smaller
amount of attention is allocated to peripheral process-
ing because of limited attention capacity (Huang and
Watanabe 2012).
2.2 The Effect of Emotion on Attention
Some researchers have discussed that excitement may
enhance attention and facilitate flexibility. On the
other hand, it has been pointed out that positive mood
may reduce the subject’s performance (Schwarz and
Clore 1983, Schwarz and Clore 1988, Rank and Frese
2008). When a subject experiences positive affective
states, he assumes he is performing sufficiently well
thus he withdraws effort. Also, in Cerin et al.s model
predicts that, in general, an affective profile charac-
terized by mild to moderate intensity levels of threat-
related affects (e.g. fear and apprehension) and af-
fects conducive to or associated with approach behav-
ior and task-focused attention (e.g. interest, excite-
ment and enjoyment) will be perceived as facilitating
performance (Cerin et al. 2000).
We however consider that the balance between ex-
citement and calmness is important. If a person is
overexcited, it often happens that he is distracted by
any small stimuli and cannot concentrate on the task.
On the other hand, if one’s mind is too calm or re-
laxed, he would not feel like doing anything. The
level most appropriate for a task lies somewhere in
between excited and relaxed. We assume that listen-
ing to music that one likes has a modulating effect,
with excitatory and inhibitory factors, helps achieve
the best performance. Some music excites and other
music calms. We assume that people know from ex-
perience which music is best for them to modulate
music to sustain his/her level of attention. We test
this hypothesis through experiments described in this
paper.
2.3 Temporal Change in the Level of
Attention
Time has various effects on cognitive performance
(Grondin 2008). For example, it has been pointed
out that attention is limited in time (Nobre and Coull
2010). Humans cannot sustain a high level of concen-
tration for a long period of time. Learning is no ex-
ception. In practice, it is important to know how the
level of attention changes, how it can be recovered, in
order to make humans more productive.
It is also known that there are various rhythms
in mental processes, from short ones to long ones
(Buzsaki 2006). The level of concentration does not
monotonically decreases either. It may have some
rhythm, or it may rise as the end of the task ap-
proaches. It may follow different patterns other than
rhythms, for example constant decrease in perfor-
mance. By measuring the temporal change in per-
formance, we try to uncover the temporal change of
concentration. In this paper, we try to uncover such
different patterns through experiments.
TheEffectofMusicontheLevelofMentalConcentrationanditsTemporalChange
35
3 IMPLEMENTATION
We implemented a system aimed at controlling the
level of concentration. In this section, we first de-
scribe a way to measure it, then describe the actual
implementation.
3.1 Measurement of Mental
Concentration
Our system measures the level of concentration using
the amount of time required to perform a task that re-
quires attention. It is based on a “conjunction search”
task, where the subject is presented with signs that
have combinations of features, such as shape, color,
and orientation (Bergen and Julesz 1983). Figure 2
shows an example of the image presented to the sub-
ject when using our system.
Figure 2: Conjunction search task.
The subject is presented with 100 signs, aligned
into 10 rows and 10 columns, shown on a computer
screen. Signs are in two types, namely T and L, and
in two colors, blue and orange. The subject is asked
to find a sign that is different from the rest in two as-
pects. For example, about half of the signs are blue T,
and about the half are orange L, but there is one ex-
ception, either orange T or blue L. Once the subject
finds it, he presses a key, and then the next trial starts.
The exceptional sign is presented at different location
each trial.
Usually, it is easy for a subject to find an excep-
tional sign when there is only one feature involved
(Duncan and Humphreys 1989). It is an unconscious
process, and the exceptional sign “pops out”. In other
words, the subject notices the exception without pay-
ing much mental effort.
On the other hand, one that is exceptional because
of a combination of features, it requires much more
time to find it. Usually, it requires conscious process
for searching it. It means that consciousness is in-
volved in the combinatorial search process. Consider-
ing the usual assumption that consciousness is closely
related to attention, the task requires much attention,
or concentration. By measuring the time required to
find the exception, we can quantitatively evaluate the
level of concentration. This type of test is widely used
in psychology to measure the level of attention put by
the subject.
In our system, the subject can click a sign by mov-
ing a pointer using the mouse. If the clicked sing is
not the exceptional one, it is recorded as a mistake. By
a preliminary experiment, we checked that time re-
quired for moving the pointer to the exceptional sign
is negligible compared to time necessary for finding
the sign.
3.2 Environmental Factors
As a control factor to affect the internal parameters,
we chose to focus on music. Music affects emotion,
making a person feel happy or sad. It is natural to
think that it may affect internal parameters mentioned
above. It has already been pointed out that it im-
proves performance on spatial tasks (Schellenberg et
al. 2007). Our experiments are to see if this is also
true for tasks that involve concentration, and see if
it can be quantitatively and systematically measured
with an aid of a computer program.
In our experiments, we compared 3 conditions
listed below.
1. Silence.
2. Music that the subject likes.
3. Music that the subject is unfamiliar with.
We chose to compare between music that are liked
by the subject and that the subject is unfamiliar with.
We assume that when the subject is listening to the
music that he likes, performing the task becomes
more enjoyable.
We asked the subjects to name a song that he/she
likes, and used it in the experiment. For unfamiliar
music, subjects were provided with music liked by
other subjects, after checking that he/she is actually
unfamiliar with it.
Using music that the subject likes is to determine
the effect of enjoyment on concentration. The goal
of our experiment is to determine whether the effect
comes directly from the music itself, or is strongly
influenced by the subject’s liking to it.
CSEDU2014-6thInternationalConferenceonComputerSupportedEducation
36
3.3 Software
For implementing our system, we used PsychToolbox
(Brainard 1997, Kleiner, Brainard and Pelli 2007),
which is a set of functions run on Matlab, aimed at
vision research. PsychToolbox contains various func-
tions that could be used for creating psychological
tests. For implementation, we used Octave, a Matlab
compatible software.
4 EXPERIMENTS
We performed experiments on 12 subjects. All of
them were undergraduateand graduate students, rang-
ing from a freshman to 1st year in master’s course.
Using our system, we measured the number of tri-
als each subject could perform during a set amount
of time (15 minutes), under different conditions. Sub-
jects were asked to perform trials as many times as
possible. In other words, they were asked to find ex-
ceptional signs as fast as possible.
The types of music that were played were mostly
pop music, with lyrics. There were up-tempo ones
and slow-tempo ones, depending on the preference of
each subject. When we could, we chose unfamiliar
music from those that were liked by other subjects.
This was to avoid the effect that comes from the types
of music, for example the positive effect on concen-
tration that may arise from listening to up-tempo mu-
sic.
4.1 Comparison among Subjects
The numbers of trials that the subjects could perform
under three conditions mentioned in the previous sec-
tion is indicated in Table 1. The three conditions were
silence, playing music that the subject likes, and play-
ing music that the subject is unfamiliar with.
It shows scores for each condition, which are the
numbers of trials that the subject succeeded within the
time limit of 15 minutes. If the subject could complete
each trial in a shorter time, he gets a higher score.
To avoid the effect from the subject getting used
to the task and performing better in the latter part of
the experiment, we arranged the conditions in differ-
ent orders. The order of conditions the experiment
was carried out for each subject is indicated by the
second column of Table 1, using order IDs. The order
is explained using Table 2.
The result shows that the average score were high-
est when the subject was listening to the music that
he likes. The second highest was when he listened to
the music that he is unfamiliar with, and the lowest
Table 1: Scores of subjects under different conditions.
Subject Order Silence Like Unfam.
A I 98 89 113
B I 86 84 84
C II 119 119 96
D II 54 61 69
E III 71 72 62
F III 106 132 109
G IV 88 96 93
H IV 109 105 105
I V 116 131 138
J V 115 110 112
K VI 54 51 43
L VI 40 56 53
Total 1056 1106 1077
Avg 88 92.17 89.75
Table 2: Order of testing.
Order Silence Like Unfam.
I 1st 2nd 3rd
II 3rd 2nd 1st
III 2nd 3rd 1st
IV 2nd 1st 3rd
V 1st 3rd 2nd
IV 3rd 1st 2nd
was for the silence condition. It indicates that perfor-
mance can be improved using music.
Table 3 shows the number of mistakes made by the
subjects when carrying out the test. It shows that the
number of mistakes was least when the subject was
listening to the music he likes. Figure 3 illustrates the
same information as a graph.
Table 3: The number of mistakes made by subjects.
Subject Silence Like Unfam. Total
A 0 1 1 2
B 1 0 0 1
C 0 0 0 0
D 2 0 2 4
E 0 2 2 4
F 0 0 0 0
G 0 0 0 0
H 1 0 0 1
I 0 0 0 0
J 3 1 1 5
K 0 1 1 2
L 2 0 0 2
Total 9 5 7 21
TheEffectofMusicontheLevelofMentalConcentrationanditsTemporalChange
37
Figure 3: The number of mistakes made by subjects.
4.2 Temporal Change in Performance
In the experiment, each subject was asked to perform
the task for 15 minutes, for each condition. After per-
forming a task under one condition, the subject takes
5 minutes break.
Since the task is rather simple, so it is assumed
that the subjects get tired with the task, or bored,
which would lower the performance. Unless a high
level of concentration is maintained, the performance
level of the subject is unlikely to be constant.
Figure 4-9 shows one example of the change in
the performance level as a subject did a sequence of
trials. The x-axis indicates the trial ID and the y-axis
indicates the time it took for the subject to complete
that trial, i.e. to find the exceptional sign and click it.
The sequence of the times taken for finishing tri-
als was considered as a function of sample points in
time. We applied second order polynomial fitting to
this function. The polynomials are also shown in the
figures. They show the gradual changes of the perfor-
mance levels.
Based on an observation, we grouped the result
into three types. Type 1 is the case when the perfor-
mance level does not change much. In this case, the
polynomial is nearly constant. Type 2 is that the per-
formance worsens as the time passes. In this case,
the polynomial is a nearly linear increasing function.
Type 3 is when the performance is best near the be-
ginning and near the end, and worse in the middle.
In this case, the polynomial is a convex function with
its maximum in the middle part of the trial sequence.
This is summarized in Table 4. In the table, a
2
is the
coefficient of the quadratic term and a
1
is the coeffi-
cient for the linear term, for the second order polyno-
mial fitting. Note that the performance is better when
the y-value is lower.
The frequent appearance of Type 3 (high in the
middle) was interesting, possibly indicating the rise in
the performance level when the deadline is approach-
ing.
In general, the overall performance level (the
score the subject obtained) was highest for Type 1 and
lowest for Type 3. Also, Type 1 was most frequent
when the subject was listening to the music that he
liked. On the other hand, Type 3 was most frequent
when the subject was in the silence condition.
The result indicates that listening to preferred mu-
sic condition raises the overall performance because
it reduces occasions where the subject cannot find the
exceptional sign and takes unusually longer time to
finish the trial.
Table 4: Types in the temporal change of performance.
Type Polynomial fitting a
2
a
1
Type 1 Roughly constant a
2
0 a
1
0
Type 2 Increasing a
2
0 a
1
> 0
Type 3 High middle, low ends a
2
> 0
Figure 4: Temporal change under the silence condition
(Subject C, Type 2).
Figure 5: Temporal change under the liking music condition
(Subject C, Type 1).
The regression curve may seem to be dependent
on large values that intermittently occurs. However,
since one of the aims of our experiment was to see the
lack of attention, the fact that the subject took a long
time to find the exceptional sign does carry informa-
tion. We did not consider them as outliers.
CSEDU2014-6thInternationalConferenceonComputerSupportedEducation
38
Figure 6: Temporal change under the unfamiliar music con-
dition (Subject C, Type 2).
Figure 7: Temporal change under the silence condition
(Subject F, Type 3).
4.3 Discussion
Our result that music affects the level of concentra-
tion have various application in the daily practicality
of learning. When doing self-study, selecting appro-
priate music would help raise the performance. Even
in a classroom, when it is not a lecture-style class but
is a practice-style, it might help students by allowing
them to listen to music while solving problems.
Teachers may even advice students to try out dif-
ferent types of music while studying, since our result
Figure 8: Temporal change under the liking music condition
(Subject F, Type 1).
Figure 9: Temporal change under the unfamiliar music con-
dition (Subject F, Type 3).
indicates that familiarity to music may affect the tem-
poral change in the level of concentration.
Also, the method proposed in this paper to mea-
sure the level of concentration may be used for other
purposes too, for example to know how long a subject
can stay concentrated in learning.
5 RELATED WORK
In this section, we discuss related work from different
aspects.
5.1 Factors That May Affect Mental
Concentration
Onyper et al. tested a group of subject to solve puzzles
or memorize items, while chewing gum (Onyper et al.
2011). They were compared with another group that
did not chew gum during the test. The result indicated
that chewing gum has positive effect on the subjects’
performance. They state that chewing gum may wake
you up and increase the level of concentration.
Nittono et al. found out that when the subject
is presented with a cute (“kawaii”, in recent termi-
nology) picture preceding a task, his performance in-
creased (Nittono et al. 2012). Although they hypoth-
esized that a cute picture makes the subject focus on
the details of it and increase his performance, it could
also be resulting from the increased level of concen-
tration due to excitement or enjoyment.
Our work is different from existing work in that
it focused on music among many possible factors. It
is also different in that it intended to capture the tem-
poral change in the level of concentration in a short
time span, i.e. in the order of seconds, which is much
shorter than usually considered.
TheEffectofMusicontheLevelofMentalConcentrationanditsTemporalChange
39
5.2 Cognitive Performance While
Listening to Music
Rauscher et al. reported the superior spatial abilities
for participants who listened to a recording of music
composed by Mozart compared to those who sat in si-
lence or listened to relaxation instructions (Rauscher,
Shaw and Ky 1993). Because the performance was
better on the spatial tasks after listening to Mozart,
this result became known as the Mozart effect (Schel-
lenberg 2005). Reviewing studies that examined ef-
fects of listening to music on cognitive performance
can be divided to two general group: performance af-
ter listening to music and performance while listen-
ing to music or background music. Despite the dif-
ference, it is pointed out that music influences a wide
range of behaviors including cognitive performance
(Schellenberg 2012). In these studies, however, it
was not checked whether the subjects actually liked
Mozart. In comparison, we made a distinction be-
tween music liked by the subject and the one that the
subject is not familiar with. Our result showed that
while music itself raises concentration, the one that is
liked works even better.
Shih et al. compared how music with, and with-
out, lyrics affects human attention (Shih, Huang and
Chiang 2012). Background music with, and with-
out lyrics, was tested for effects on listener concen-
tration in attention testing using a RCT (randomized
controlled trial) study. The findings revealed that, if
background music is played in the work environment,
music without lyrics is preferable because songs with
lyrics are likely to have significant negative effects on
the concentration and attention of worker.
Patston and Tippett examined the overlap be-
tween music and language processing in the brain and
whether these processes are functionally independent
in expert musicians (Patston and Tippett 2011). A lan-
guage comprehension task and a visuospatial search
task were performed under three conditions: music-
correct, music-incorrect, and silence for expert musi-
cians and non-musicians. The performance of musi-
cians was negatively affected by the presence of back-
ground music compared to silence when performing a
language comprehension task. In contrast, the perfor-
mance of non-musicianswas not affectedon either the
language task by the presence of music played either
correctly or incorrectly.
Cassidy and MacDonald studied the effects of HA
(music with high arousal potential and negative af-
fect), LA (music with low arousal potential and pos-
itive affect), and everyday noise, on the cognitive
task performance of introverts and extraverts (Cas-
sidy and MacDonald 2007). Performance was de-
creased across all cognitive tasks in the presence of
background sound compared to silence. HA and LA
music produced differential distraction effects, with
performance of all tasks being poorer in the presence
of HA compared to LA and silence, in the presence of
noise than silence across all tasks, and in the presence
of noise than LA in three of the four tasks.
Furnham and Strbac examined whether back-
ground noise would be as distracting as music (Furn-
ham and Strbac 2002). In the presence of silence,
background garage music and office noise, subjects
with introvert and extravert personalities carried out a
reading comprehension task, a prose recall task and a
mental arithmetic task. Results found a significant in-
teraction between personality and background sound
on comprehension task only, although a trend for this
effect was clearly present on the other two tasks. Par-
ticipants performed best in silence, background mu-
sic was second best for performance, and background
noise was lowest results.
The existing work mainly focused on making
comparison among music, silence and noise, whereas
in our work music liked by the subject is compared
with unfamiliar one. In this sense, it is more related
to the emotion of the subject. Also, the existing work
has not discussed much on how listening to music
may affect the temporal change in the level of con-
centration. We designed and carried out experiments
to check this effect.
5.3 Music for Enhancing Learning
Researchers from psychology as well as sociology
have attempted to explain the importance of mu-
sic for intellectual development by focusing on a
variety of cognitive ability. Singer (Singer 2008)
and Barker (Barker 2008) reported that music in-
creased the chance students remembered what they
had learned, by assisting the recall of information.
Binkiewicz discussed the idea that songs are pow-
erful pedagogical tools that enliven a classroom and
enhance student learning in an enjoyable manner
(Binkiewicz 2006).
When music is utilized learning, positive results
occurred in achievement. Music showed positive im-
pacts on achievement as Southgate and Roscigno as-
sessed three patterns of music participation: in school,
outside of school, and parental involvement in the
form of concert attendance and possible effects on
math and reading performance for both elementary
and high school students (Southgate and Roscigno
2009). Their study captured the significant influ-
ence of music involvement for both math and reading
achievement.
CSEDU2014-6thInternationalConferenceonComputerSupportedEducation
40
Paquette and Rieg described the benefits of in-
corporating musical experiences into daily instruc-
tion and argued that integrating experiences with mu-
sic in the childhood classroom supports English lan-
guage learners’ literacy development (Paquette and
Rieg 2008). Sims examined the effects of high versus
low teacher affect and active versus passive student
activities during music listening on preschool chil-
dren’s attention (Sims 1986). Data obtained through
observation indicated that children were most atten-
tive during music listening activities when the teacher
exhibited high magnitude nonverbal affect, and when
they were given a hand-movement activity in which
to participate.
Our paper focused on the effect of music on the
level of concentration, which is related to perfor-
mance in general, rather than specific tasks in learn-
ing. By focusing on a simple task rather than com-
plicated ones, we believe that we could quantify more
fundamental parameter that affect the level of perfor-
mance.
6 CONCLUSION
In order to increase the concentration level and raise
the performance in learning, we implemented a sys-
tem for measuring it, and examined the effect of an
external factor, namely playing music that the subject
likes. The result showed that playing music does have
positive effects on the level of concentration, which
would contribute to the performance level.
In future work, we would like to carry out exper-
iments using more subjects, to make our result more
statistically reliable. We would also like to look at
the temporal patterns of concentration in more detail.
We would like to see if there is actually rhythms for
concentration, as mentioned by Buzsaki for different
mental processes (Buzsaki 2006). We would like to
explore this, for example using frequency analysis.
We also plan to carry out more controlled testing, us-
ing a larger number of subjects, to validate our hy-
pothesis.
We also plan to explore modulation of the excite-
ment level using music, and see if the concentration
can be improved. When the subject is too relaxed,
we make him listen to excitatory music, and while
the subject is overexcited, we make him listen to in-
hibitory music. We would like to see if the concen-
tration level can effectively controlled that way. We
expect the result to provide a fundamental basis for
creating the environment most suited for learning.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This work was supported in part by JSPS KAKENHI
Grant Numbers 21700121, 25280110, and 25540159.
REFERENCES
Ahissar, M. and Hochstein, S. (2002) “The role of attention
in learning simple visual tasks”, Perceptual learning:
253-272.
Barker, C. (2008). “Teacher, producer create CD for math
success”, New York Amsterdam News 99, 16-32.
Bergen, J. R. and Julesz, B. (1983). “Parallel versus se-
rial processing in rapid pattern discrimination”, Na-
ture 303: 696-698.
Binkiewicz, D. M. (2006) “Tunes of the times: Historical
songs as pedagogy for recent US history”, The History
Teacher 39, no. 4: 515-520.
Brainard, D. H. (1997). “The Psychophysics Toolbox”, Spa-
tial Vision 10: 443-446.
Buzsaki, G. (2006). Rhythms of the Brain. Oxford Univer-
sity Press.
Cassidy, G. and MacDonald, R. A. R. (2007). “The effect of
background music and background noise on the task
performance of introverts and extraverts”, Psychology
of Music 35, no. 3: 517-537.
Cerin, E., Szabo, A., Hunt, N. and Williams, C. (2000).
“Temporal patterning of competitive emotions: A crit-
ical review”, Journal of Sports Sciences 18, no. 8:
605-626.
Duncan, J. and Humphreys, G. W. (1989). “Visual search
and stimulus similarity”, Psychological Review 96,
no. 3: 433-458.
Furnham, A. and Strbac, L. (2002). “Music is as distract-
ing as noise: the differential distraction of background
music and noise on the cognitive test performance of
introverts and extraverts”, Ergonomics 45, no. 3: 203-
217.
Grondin, S. (Eds) (2008). Psychology of Time. ElsevierSci-
ence.
Huang, T. R. and Watanabe, T. (2012) “Task attention facil-
itates learning of task-irrelevant stimuli”, PloS ONE
7, no. 4: e35946.
McArdle, W. D., Katch, F. I., and Katch, V. L. (1999). Es-
sentials of Exercise Physiology (2nd edition). Lipin-
cott, Williams and Wilkins.
Kleiner, M., Brainard, D. and Pelli, D. (2007). “What’s new
in Psychtoolbox-3?”, Perception 36 ECVP Abstract
Supplement.
Nobre, A. C. and Coull, J. T. (Eds). (2010). Attention and
Time, Oxford University Press.
Nittono, H., Fukushima, M., Yano, A. and Moriya, H.
(2012). “The Power of Kawaii: Viewing Cute Images
Promotes a Careful Behavior and Narrows Attentional
Focus”, PLoS ONE 7, no. 9.
Onyper, S. V., Carr, T. L., Farrar, J. S. and Floyd, B. R.
(2011). “Cognitive advantages of chewing gum. Now
you see them, now you don’t”, Appatite 57: 321-328.
TheEffectofMusicontheLevelofMentalConcentrationanditsTemporalChange
41
Paquette, K. R., and Rieg, S. A. (2008). “Using music to
support the literacy development of young English
language learners.“ Early Childhood Education Jour-
nal 36, no. 3: 227-232.
Patston, L. L. M, and Tippett, L. J. (2011). “The effect of
background music on cognitive performance in musi-
cians and nonmusicians”, Music Perception 29, no. 2:
173-183.
Rank, J. and Frese, M. (2010). “The impact of emotions,
moods and other affect related variables on creativ-
ity innovation and initiative” in Ashkanasy, N.M. and
Cooper, C.L. (Eds), Research Companion to Emotion
in Organization.
Rauscher, F. H., Shaw, G. L. and Ky, K. N. (1993). “Music
and spatial task performance”, Nature 365, no. 6447:
611.
Schellenberg, E. G. (2005). “Music and cognitive abilities”,
Current Directions in Psychological Science 14, no. 6:
317-320.
Schellenberg, E. G, Nakata, T., Hunter, P. G. and Tamoto,
S. (2007). “Exposure to music and cognitive perfor-
mance: Tests of children and adults”, Psychology of
Music 35, no. 1: 5-19.
Schellenberg, E. G. (2012). “Cognitive performance after
listening to music: a review of the Mozart effect”, in
MacDonald, R., Gunter, K. and Mitchell, L. (Eds.),
Music, Health, and Wellbeing: 324-338.
Schmidt, R. (1995) “Consciousness and foreign language
learning: A tutorial on the role of attention and aware-
ness in learning”, Attention and awareness in foreign
language learning: 1-63.
Schwarz, N. and Clore, G. L. (1983). “Mood, misattribu-
tion, and judgments of well-being: Informative and
directive functions of affective states”, Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology 45, no. 3: 513.
Schwarz, N., and Clore, G. L. (1988) “How do I feel about
it? Informative function of affective states”, in Fiedler,
K. and Forgas, J. (Eds.), Affect, Cognition, and Social
Behavior, Hogrefe International: 44-62.
Shih, Y., Huang, R. and Chiang, H. (2012) “Background
music: Effects on attention performance”, Work: A
Journal of Prevention, Assessment and Rehabilitation
42, no. 4: 573-578.
Sims, W. L. (1986). “The effect of high versus low teacher
affect and passive versus active student activity dur-
ing music listening on preschool children’s atten-
tion, piece preference, time spent listening, and piece
recognition.” Journal of Research in Music Education
34, no. 3: 173-191.
Singer, M. J. (2008). Accessing the Musical Intelligence
in Early Childhood Education”, Australian Journal of
Early Childhood 33, no. 2: 49-56.
Southgate, D. E. and Roscigno, V. J. (2009) “The Impact of
Music on Childhood and Adolescent Achievement”,
Social science quarterly 90, no. 1: 4-21.
Tomlin, R. S. and Villa, V. (1994) Attention in cognitive
science and second language acquisition”, Studies in
second language acquisition 16, no. 2: 183-203.
CSEDU2014-6thInternationalConferenceonComputerSupportedEducation
42