Paquette and Rieg described the benefits of in-
corporating musical experiences into daily instruc-
tion and argued that integrating experiences with mu-
sic in the childhood classroom supports English lan-
guage learners’ literacy development (Paquette and
Rieg 2008). Sims examined the effects of high versus
low teacher affect and active versus passive student
activities during music listening on preschool chil-
dren’s attention (Sims 1986). Data obtained through
observation indicated that children were most atten-
tive during music listening activities when the teacher
exhibited high magnitude nonverbal affect, and when
they were given a hand-movement activity in which
to participate.
Our paper focused on the effect of music on the
level of concentration, which is related to perfor-
mance in general, rather than specific tasks in learn-
ing. By focusing on a simple task rather than com-
plicated ones, we believe that we could quantify more
fundamental parameter that affect the level of perfor-
mance.
6 CONCLUSION
In order to increase the concentration level and raise
the performance in learning, we implemented a sys-
tem for measuring it, and examined the effect of an
external factor, namely playing music that the subject
likes. The result showed that playing music does have
positive effects on the level of concentration, which
would contribute to the performance level.
In future work, we would like to carry out exper-
iments using more subjects, to make our result more
statistically reliable. We would also like to look at
the temporal patterns of concentration in more detail.
We would like to see if there is actually rhythms for
concentration, as mentioned by Buzsaki for different
mental processes (Buzsaki 2006). We would like to
explore this, for example using frequency analysis.
We also plan to carry out more controlled testing, us-
ing a larger number of subjects, to validate our hy-
pothesis.
We also plan to explore modulation of the excite-
ment level using music, and see if the concentration
can be improved. When the subject is too relaxed,
we make him listen to excitatory music, and while
the subject is overexcited, we make him listen to in-
hibitory music. We would like to see if the concen-
tration level can effectively controlled that way. We
expect the result to provide a fundamental basis for
creating the environment most suited for learning.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This work was supported in part by JSPS KAKENHI
Grant Numbers 21700121, 25280110, and 25540159.
REFERENCES
Ahissar, M. and Hochstein, S. (2002) “The role of attention
in learning simple visual tasks”, Perceptual learning:
253-272.
Barker, C. (2008). “Teacher, producer create CD for math
success”, New York Amsterdam News 99, 16-32.
Bergen, J. R. and Julesz, B. (1983). “Parallel versus se-
rial processing in rapid pattern discrimination”, Na-
ture 303: 696-698.
Binkiewicz, D. M. (2006) “Tunes of the times: Historical
songs as pedagogy for recent US history”, The History
Teacher 39, no. 4: 515-520.
Brainard, D. H. (1997). “The Psychophysics Toolbox”, Spa-
tial Vision 10: 443-446.
Buzsaki, G. (2006). Rhythms of the Brain. Oxford Univer-
sity Press.
Cassidy, G. and MacDonald, R. A. R. (2007). “The effect of
background music and background noise on the task
performance of introverts and extraverts”, Psychology
of Music 35, no. 3: 517-537.
Cerin, E., Szabo, A., Hunt, N. and Williams, C. (2000).
“Temporal patterning of competitive emotions: A crit-
ical review”, Journal of Sports Sciences 18, no. 8:
605-626.
Duncan, J. and Humphreys, G. W. (1989). “Visual search
and stimulus similarity”, Psychological Review 96,
no. 3: 433-458.
Furnham, A. and Strbac, L. (2002). “Music is as distract-
ing as noise: the differential distraction of background
music and noise on the cognitive test performance of
introverts and extraverts”, Ergonomics 45, no. 3: 203-
217.
Grondin, S. (Eds) (2008). Psychology of Time. ElsevierSci-
ence.
Huang, T. R. and Watanabe, T. (2012) “Task attention facil-
itates learning of task-irrelevant stimuli”, PloS ONE
7, no. 4: e35946.
McArdle, W. D., Katch, F. I., and Katch, V. L. (1999). Es-
sentials of Exercise Physiology (2nd edition). Lipin-
cott, Williams and Wilkins.
Kleiner, M., Brainard, D. and Pelli, D. (2007). “What’s new
in Psychtoolbox-3?”, Perception 36 ECVP Abstract
Supplement.
Nobre, A. C. and Coull, J. T. (Eds). (2010). Attention and
Time, Oxford University Press.
Nittono, H., Fukushima, M., Yano, A. and Moriya, H.
(2012). “The Power of Kawaii: Viewing Cute Images
Promotes a Careful Behavior and Narrows Attentional
Focus”, PLoS ONE 7, no. 9.
Onyper, S. V., Carr, T. L., Farrar, J. S. and Floyd, B. R.
(2011). “Cognitive advantages of chewing gum. Now
you see them, now you don’t”, Appatite 57: 321-328.
TheEffectofMusicontheLevelofMentalConcentrationanditsTemporalChange
41