Social Cognition in Silica - A ‘Theory of Mind’ for Socially Aware Artificial Minds
Michael Harré
2014
Abstract
Each of us has an incredibly large repertoire of behaviours from which to select from at any given time, and as our behavioural complexity grows so too does the possibility that we will misunderstand each other’s actions. However, we have evolved a cognitive mechanism that allows us to understand another person’s psychological space: their motivations, constraints, plans, goals and emotional state and it is called our ‘Theory of Mind’. This capability allows us to understand the choices another person might make on the basis that the other person has their own ‘internal world’ that influences their choices in the same way as our own internal world influences our choices. Arguably, this is one of the most significant cognitive developments in human evolutionary history, along with our ability for long term adaptation to familiar situations and our ability to reason dynamically in completely novel situations. So the question arises: Can we implement the rudimentary foundations of a human-like Theory of Mind in an artificial mind such that it can dynamically adapt to the likely decisions of another mind (artificial or biological) by holding an internal representation of that other mind? This article argues that this is possible and that we already have much of the necessary theoretical foundations in order to begin the development process.
References
- Anderson, S. P., Goeree, J. K., and Holt, C. A. (2004). Noisy directional learning and the logit equilibrium. The Scandinavian Journal of Economics, 106(3):581- 602.
- Baron-Cohen, S. E., Tager-Flusberg, H. E., and Cohen, D. J. (2000). Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience . Oxford University Press.
- Bhatt, M. and Camerer, C. F. (2005). Self-referential thinking and equilibrium as states of mind in games: fmri evidence. Games and Economic Behavior, 52(2):424- 459.
- Bogacz, R., Brown, E., Moehlis, J., Holmes, P., and Cohen, J. D. (2006). The physics of optimal decision making: a formal analysis of models of performance in twoalternative forced-choice tasks. Psychological review, 113(4):700.
- Coricelli, G. and Nagel, R. (2009). Neural correlates of depth of strategic reasoning in medial prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(23):9163-9168.
- Dunbar, R. (1992). Neocortex size as a constraint on group size in primates. Journal of Human Evolution, 22(6):469-493.
- Dunbar, R. I. and Shultz, S. (2007). Evolution in the social brain. science, 317(5843):1344-1347.
- Goeree, J. K. and Holt, C. A. (1999). Stochastic game theory: For playing games, not just for doing theory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 96(19):10564-10567.
- Goldman, A. I. (2005). Imitation, mind reading, and simulation. Perspectives on Imitation: Imitation, human development, and culture, 2:79.
- Harré, M. (2013). The neural circuitry of expertise: Perceptual learning and social cognition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7:852.
- Kudo, H. and Dunbar, R. (2001). Neocortex size and social network size in primates. Animal Behaviour, 62(4):711-722.
- Lamm, C., Batson, C. D., and Decety, J. (2007). The neural substrate of human empathy: Effects of perspectivetaking and cognitive appraisal. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 19(1):42-58.
- Lehmann, J. and Dunbar, R. (2009). Network cohesion, group size and neocortex size in female-bonded old world primates. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276(1677):4417-4422.
- Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 58:259-289.
- Powell, J., Lewis, P. A., Roberts, N., García-Fin˜ ana, M., and Dunbar, R. (2012). Orbital prefrontal cortex volume predicts social network size: An imaging study of individual differences in humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279(1736):2157-2162.
- Shultz, S. and Dunbar, R. (2007). The evolution of the social brain: Anthropoid primates contrast with other vertebrates. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 274(1624):2429-2436.
- Tenenbaum, J. B., Kemp, C., Griffiths, T. L., and Goodman, N. D. (2011). How to grow a mind: Statistics, structure, and abstraction. science, 331(6022):1279-1285.
- Wolpert, D. H., Harré, M., Olbrich, E., Bertschinger, N., and Jost, J. (2012). Hysteresis effects of changing the parameters of noncooperative games. Physical Review E, 85(3):036102.
- Yoshida, W., Dolan, R. J., and Friston, K. J. (2008). Game theory of mind. PLoS computational biology, 4(12):e1000254.
- Zacks, J. M. and Michelon, P. (2005). Transformations of visuospatial images. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 4(2):96-118.
Paper Citation
in Harvard Style
Harré M. (2014). Social Cognition in Silica - A ‘Theory of Mind’ for Socially Aware Artificial Minds . In Proceedings of the 6th International Conference on Agents and Artificial Intelligence - Volume 1: ICAART, ISBN 978-989-758-015-4, pages 657-662. DOI: 10.5220/0004917606570662
in Bibtex Style
@conference{icaart14,
author={Michael Harré},
title={Social Cognition in Silica - A ‘Theory of Mind’ for Socially Aware Artificial Minds},
booktitle={Proceedings of the 6th International Conference on Agents and Artificial Intelligence - Volume 1: ICAART,},
year={2014},
pages={657-662},
publisher={SciTePress},
organization={INSTICC},
doi={10.5220/0004917606570662},
isbn={978-989-758-015-4},
}
in EndNote Style
TY - CONF
JO - Proceedings of the 6th International Conference on Agents and Artificial Intelligence - Volume 1: ICAART,
TI - Social Cognition in Silica - A ‘Theory of Mind’ for Socially Aware Artificial Minds
SN - 978-989-758-015-4
AU - Harré M.
PY - 2014
SP - 657
EP - 662
DO - 10.5220/0004917606570662