4.1 Impact of HG/EA on Timing Error
When comparing the absolute timing error during
RC to that of B1, no improvement in timing error
was noted (p0.14), regardless of the training
condition (t(10)=-1.2, p=0.13) (Figure 2 A).
Figure 2: Comparison of subjects’ A) absolute and B)
relative timing error between the baseline condition and
retention condition following HG and EA robotic training.
However, when analyzing the relative timing
error, where a negative value indicated that the
subjects initiated movement too early, a trend
towards an improvement in timing error was noted
when comparing B1 to RC following HG training (-
412 vs 0.0110 ms; p=0.09), paralleled by a trend
towards a decrease in the variability of the relative
timing error (SD) (1711 vs 126 ms; p=0.08). This
means that subjects learned to initiate movement
later to more successfully hit the targets, and were
more homogenous in doing so. No difference was
noted when comparing B1 to RC following EA
training (-0.97 vs -412 ms; p=0.2) (between
conditions, t(10)=1.3, p=0.11) (Figure 2B).
5 DISCUSSION
These preliminary results suggest that as age
increases, learning can still occur since the subjects’
relative timing error decreased after HG training.
This also supports the results of previous studies on
the elderly’s ability to learn new tasks (Marchal-
Crespo et al., 2010).
Moreover, it appears that a robotic assisted hand
could be an effective approach in improving elders’
timing errors; however, only HG appears to benefit
them. This supports the results of our previous
study, which was conducted on young healthy
individuals (Milot et al., 2010); here, less-skilled
subjects did not benefit from EA in the timing-based
task (k value 0.1). It is plausible that for this sub-
group of subjects, EA training was too challenging
since the motor system was overwhelmed with too
much information, preventing any improvement in
performance. This could be the case in this current
study, since the seniors’ mean k value is 0.07 (range:
0.02; 0.1), falling into the less-skilled sub-group
category.
This current study is part of an ongoing project,
so more subjects are needed in order to validate the
preliminary results and to assess the long-term
benefits of HG and EA on improving movement
timing. If HG and EA trainings are proven to
effectively do so, they could potentially help
improve the movement timings of neurologically
impaired individuals like chronic stroke survivors.
REFERENCES
EMKEN, J. L. et al. 2005. Robot-enhanced motor
learning: accelerating internal model formation during
locomotion by transient dynamic amplification. IEEE
Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng, 13, 33-9.
LUTTGEN, J. et al. 2013. The influence of robotic
guidance on different types of motor timing. J Mot
Behav, 45, 249-58.
MARCHAL-CRESPO, L. et al. 2010. The effect of haptic
guidance, aging, and initial skill level on motor
learning of a steering task. Exp Brain Res, 201, 209-
20.
MARCHAL-CRESPO, L. et al. 2013. The effect of haptic
guidance and visual feedback on learning a complex
tennis task. Exp Brain Res, 231, 277-91.
MILOT, M. H. et al. 2010. Comparison of error-
amplification and haptic-guidance training techniques
for learning of a timing-based motor task by healthy
individuals. Exp Brain Res, 201, 119-31.
PATTON, J. L. et al. 2004. Robot-assisted adaptive
training: custom force fields for teaching movement
patterns. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng, 51, 636-46.
SEIDLER, R. D. et al. 2010. Motor control and aging:
links to age-related brain structural, functional, and
biochemical effects. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 34, 721-
33.
-20
-15
-10
-5
0
5
10
15
Baseline
RE
Retention
RE
Baseline
EA
Retention
EA
Timing error (ms)
B)
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
Timing error
Baseline
RE
Retention
RE
Baseline
EA
Retention
EA
A)