Despite this evidence, the AVS model (and thus
also our rAVS model) only considers geometric repre-
sentations of the RO and the LO. For the AVS model,
however, a range of extensions that integrate func-
tionality were already proposed (Carlson et al., 2006;
Kluth and Schultheis, 2014). Since the rAVS model
is designed to be as similar as possible to the AVS
model, these functional extensions might also be ap-
plicable for the rAVS model.
Implementing the Models in Artificial Systems.
In order to implement these models into artificial sys-
tems, additional steps are necessary. The models were
designed to model spatial language understanding.
So, the models produce an acceptability rating given
a RO, a LO, and a preposition. As part of an artifi-
cial system that interprets spatial language, the mod-
els can be used straightforwardly: Given a spatial ut-
terance and a visual scene, the models can be used
to compute acceptability ratings for all points around
the RO (i.e., a spatial template). The artificial system
then starts the search for the LO at the point with the
highest rating.
To generate spatial language with the help of
these models, one could imagine the following steps:
Compute the acceptability ratings of different spatial
prepositions (e.g., above, below, to the left of, in front
of, ...) and subsequently pick the one with the highest
rating.
In conclusion, we proposed a modified version of
the AVS model: the rAVS model. The rAVS model
accounts for the same empirical data as the AVS
model while integrating additional recent findings re-
garding the direction of the attentional shift that con-
flict with the assumptions of the AVS model.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This research was supported by the Cluster of Ex-
cellence Cognitive Interaction Technology ‘CITEC’
(EXC 277) at Bielefeld University, which is funded
by the German Research Foundation (DFG). The au-
thors would also like to thank two anonymous review-
ers for their useful comments and suggestions.
REFERENCES
Burigo, M. (2008). On the role of informativeness in spa-
tial language comprehension. PhD thesis, School of
Psychology, University of Plymouth.
Burigo, M., Coventry, K. R., Cangelosi, A., and Lynott, D.
(in press). Spatial Language and Converseness. Quar-
terly Journal of Experimental Psychology.
Burigo, M. and Knoeferle, P. (2015). Visual attention
during spatial language comprehension. PloS ONE,
10(1):e0115758.
Burigo, M. and Sacchi, S. (2013). Object orientation affects
spatial language comprehension. Cognitive Science,
37(8):1471–1492.
Canty, A. and Ripley, B. (2015). boot: Bootstrap R (S-Plus)
Functions. R package version 1.3-15.
Carlson, L. A. and Logan, G. D. (2005). Attention and spa-
tial language. In Itti, L., Rees, G., and Tsotsos, J. K.,
editors, Neurobiology of Attention, chapter 54, pages
330–336. Elsevier.
Carlson, L. A., Regier, T., Lopez, W., and Corrigan, B.
(2006). Attention unites form and function in spa-
tial language. Spatial Cognition and Computation,
6(4):295–308.
Carlson-Radvansky, L. A., Covey, E. S., and Lattanzi, K. M.
(1999). What effects on where: Functional influ-
ences on spatial relations. Psychological Science,
10(6):516–521.
Carrasco, M. (2011). Visual attention: The past 25 years.
Vision Research, 51(13):1484–1525.
CGAL, Computational Geometry Algorithms Library.
http://www.cgal.org.
Coventry, K. R. and Garrod, S. C. (2004). Saying, seeing,
and acting: The psychological semantics of spatial
prepositions. Essays in cognitive psychology. Psy-
chology Press, Taylor and Francis, Hove and New
York.
Coventry, K. R., Lynott, D., Cangelosi, A., Monrouxe, L.,
Joyce, D., and Richardson, D. C. (2010). Spatial
language, visual attention, and perceptual simulation.
Brain and Language, 112(3):202–213.
Coventry, K. R., Prat Sala, M., and Richards, L. (2001). The
interplay between geometry and function in the com-
prehension of over, under, above, and below. Journal
of Memory and Language, 44(3):376–398.
Franconeri, S. L., Scimeca, J. M., Roth, J. C., Helseth, S. A.,
and Kahn, L. E. (2012). Flexible visual processing of
spatial relationships. Cognition, 122(2):210–227.
Georgopoulos, A. P., Schwartz, A. B., and Kettner, R. E.
(1986). Neuronal Population Coding of Movement
Direction. Science, 233:1416–1419.
H¨orberg, T. (2008). Influences of form and function on
the acceptability of projective prepositions in swedish.
Spatial Cognition & Computation, 8(3):193–218.
Kim, W., Navarro, D. J., Pitt, M. A., and Myung, I. J.
(2004). An MCMC-based method of comparing con-
nectionist models in cognitive science. Advances in
Neural Information Processing Systems, 16:937–944.
Kluth, T. (2016). A C++ Implementation of the reversed
Attentional Vector Sum (rAVS) model.
Kluth, T. and Schultheis, H. (2014). Attentional distribu-
tion and spatial language. In Freksa, C., Nebel, B.,
Hegarty, M., and Barkowsky, T., editors, Spatial Cog-
nition IX, volume 8684 of Lecture Notes in Computer