figures at Level 1 and 2. Short deductive chains ap-
pear naturally during such training and computer ap-
plications of lower class than DGS allow to achieve
clarity and precision of the expression of these chains.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
The authors thank to the reviewers for the suggestions
which are taken into account in the final version of
this paper. The authors are very thankful to Albena
Vassileva for the improvement of the text.
REFERENCES
Burger, W. F. & Shaughnessy, J. M. (1986). Characteriz-
ing the Van Hiele levels of development in geome-
try. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education,
17(1), pp 31-48.
De Villiers, M. (2010) Some Reflections on the Van Hiele
theory. Invited plenary presented at the 4th Congress
of teachers of mathematics of the Croatian Mathemat-
ical Society, Zagreb, 30 June – 2 July 2010.
Ginsberg, D. (2015) Multimodal semiotics of mathematics
teaching and learning. Dissertation, Georgetown Uni-
versity. Washington, DC, 2015, pp 9-10
Haapasalo, Lenni (2007) Adapting Mathematics Education
to the Needs of ICT. eJMT, Vol. 1, No 1.
Kadijevich, Dj. (2006). Coordinating the Process and object
features of mathematical knowledge by CAS. Confer-
ence Proceedings of DES-TIME-2006.
Lazarov, B. (2014) Socratic Style Support for Transferabil-
ity of Math Knowledge and Skills. The Korean So-
ciety of Mathematical Education. Proceedings of the
2014 Int’l Conference on Mathematical Education,
Cheongju National University of Education, October
17-18, 2014, pp 75-85.
Lazarov, B. (2015) Project-oriented Education as a Platform
for Transfer of Math KSA. Proceedings of the 7th In-
ternational Conference on Computer Supported Edu-
cation Volume 2, Lisbon, Portugal, 23-25 May, 2015,
pp 325-330
Lozanov, C., Vitanov, T. & Kalcheva, A. (2011) Mathemat-
ics for 5th grade. Sofia, Anubis, p 77, problem 8 (in
Bulgarian)
Rittle-Johnson, B. & Koedinger, K. (2004). Comparing in-
structional strategies for integrating conceptual and
procedural knowledge. In D. Mewborn, P. Sztajn, D.
White, H. Hiegel, R. Bryant and K. Nooney (Eds.),
Proceedings of the Twenty-fourth Annual Meeting of
the North American Chapters of the International
Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education.
ERIC Clearinghouse for Science, Mathematics, and
Environmental Education. OH: Columbus, pp. 969-
978.
Rocard, M. et al. (2007) Science Education Now: A Re-
newed pedagogy for the Future of Europe. High Level
Group on Science Education. EUROPEAN COMMIS-
SION, Directorate-General for Research.
Shabanova, M. & Lazarov, B. (2014) Detecting Math-and-
ICT Competence. Proceedings of the 6th Interna-
tional Conference on Computer Supported Education,
Volume 2, Barcelona, Spain, 1 - 3 April, 2014, pp 153-
158
Van Hiele, P. M. (1984) A child’s thought and geometry.
In D.Fuys, D.Geddes and R.Tischler (Eds), English
translation of selected writings of Dina van Hiele-
Geldof and P.M. van Hiele, pp. 237-241. Brooklyn
College, N.Y. 1984.
Usiskin, Z. (1982) Van Hiele levels and Achievement
in Secondary School Geometry. Final report of the
CDASSG Project. Chicago: Univ. of Chicago.
Yollis, L. (2014) http://yollisclassblog.blogspot.bg/2014/10/
monstrous-fun.html (active in Dec 2015)
APPENDIX
Operationalization of Van Hiele Levels 1-3
Burger & Shaughnessy characterized pupils’ geomet-
rical reasoning at the first three Van Hiele levels as
follows:
Level 1 (Recognition)
(1) Often use irrelevant visual properties to identify
figures, to compare, to classify and to describe.
(2) Usually refer to visual prototypes of figures, and
is easily misled by the orientation of figures.
(3) An inability to think of an infinite variation of a
particular type of figure (e,g. in terms of orientation
and shape).
(4) Inconsistent classifications of figures; for exam-
ple, using non-common or irrelevant properties to sort
figures.
(5) Incomplete descriptions (definitions) of figures by
viewing necessary (often visual) conditions as suffi-
cient conditions.
Level 2 (Analysis)
(1) An explicit comparison of figures in terms of their
underlying properties.
(2) Avoidance of class inclusions between different
classes of figures, eg. squares and rectangles are con-
sidered to be disjoint.
(3) Sorting of figures only in terms of one property,
for example, properties of sides,
while other properties like symmetries, angles and di-
agonals are ignored.
(4) Exhibit an uneconomical use of the properties of
figures to describe (define) them, instead of just using
sufficient properties.
(5) An explicit rejection of definitions supplied by
other people, e.g. a teacher or textbook, in favour of
their own personal definitions.
Computer Supported Evolution Inside Van Hiele Levels 1 and 2
191