Supporting Zoo Visitors’ Scientific Observations with a Mobile Guide
Yui Tanaka
1
, Ryohei Egusa
1,2
, Etsuji Yamaguchi
1
, Shigenori Inagaki
1
, Fusako Kusunoki
3
,
Hideto Okuyama
4
and Tomoyuki Nogami
1
1
Kobe University, Hyogo, Japan
2
Research Fellow of Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, Tokyo, Japan
3
Tama Art University, Tokyo, Japan
4
Asahikawa City Asahiyama Zoo, Hokkaido, Japan
Keywords: Mobile Systems, Zoo, Science Learning.
Abstract: This study proposes an observation guide to support zoo visitors’ scientific observation of animals in motion
by providing viewpoints through animations. One of the difficulties that visitors experience when observing
animals is that they do not sufficiently understand the functions and behavior of the body parts of the animal
they are seeing. The guide aims to enhance visitors’ understanding by resolving this issue through the use of
animations of the functions and behaviors of parts of animals’ bodies. To evaluate the guide, we had
kindergarteners and elementary school students use its contents on seals while observing their hind flippers,
noses, and claws at the Asahiyama Zoo. Our finding was that the guide was an effective means to enhance
the children’s understanding of the functions and behaviors of these parts of the body.
1 INTRODUCTION
Zoos serve as places for science education (Bell et
al., 2009), as they allow children to observe the
natural movement of animals directly and learn
about the living environments and forms of animals
through observation (Dierking et al., 2002).
However, most children come to the zoo with their
families for leisure, so they often do not seem to
engage in focused observation of the living
environments and forms of animals at the zoo
(Patrick and Tunnicliffe, 2013), suggesting that they
require educational support in this setting.
In a comment on observation among children,
Eberbach and Crowley (2009) explained that
scientific observation is a complex practice that
requires the coordination of disciplinary knowledge
of, for example, the living environments and forms of
animals, and that children, with educational support,
are capable of making a transition from everyday
observations that focus only on obvious
characteristics to scientific observations. Furthermore,
they also noted that, during the shift between
everyday observations and scientific observations,
transitional observations take place that enable
children to connect features to functions and behavior.
Previous studies have utilized mobile devices to
enrich science education outside the classroom
through learning from observation by effectively
creating personalized learning environments (Traxler,
2005). Ohashi et al. (2008) developed a navigation
system that offered audio and video guidance with an
iPod. Furthermore, Jimenez Pazmino et al. (2013)
designed technological supports for docents running
an immersive, embodied-interaction with portable
tablets and large fixed displays.
Following up on these studies, we developed a
system to support observation among children to
help them move to “transitional noticing,” as
Eberbach and Crowley (2009) explained in their
study, which connects features to function and
behavior. This system provides viewpoints for
observation through animations that simulate
animals’ actual movements. We believe that these
animations provide a more focused approach to
observation so that children can easily understand
functions and behaviors, which is something that
still images alone were not able to do in the past.
This study proposes an observation guide that
supports the observation of the function and
behavior of each part of the body of an animal in
motion. The purpose of this study was to ascertain
whether the guide we developed, using content on
seals, was effective in providing viewpoints on the
Tanaka, Y., Egusa, R., Yamaguchi, E., Inagaki, S., Kusunoki, F., Okuyama, H. and Nogami, T.
Supporting Zoo Visitors’ Scientific Observations with a Mobile Guide.
In Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Computer Supported Education (CSEDU 2016) - Volume 2, pages 353-358
ISBN: 978-989-758-179-3
Copyright
c
2016 by SCITEPRESS – Science and Technology Publications, Lda. All rights reserved
353
function and behavior of the parts of the body.
2 SYSTEM OVERVIEW
2.1 Development Environment
HTML, CSS, JavaScript, and PHP5.3 were used to
create the development environment of the server.
The guide is Internet-based.
2.2 Flow of the Observation Guide
Figure 1 is a flowchart indicating the flow of the
guide. The first page is for entering the child’s name,
and is followed by the homepage.
A prediction page (before) and a result page
(after) are linked to each observation item. The child
selects one of the animated choices given by these
pages for each question related to the observation.
The choices that he/she has made before and after
the observation are then saved as a text file after the
last observation page.
Figure 1: Flow of observation guide.
2.3 Pages
2.3.1 Prediction Page (before)
Figure 2 shows a prediction page, which gives four
choices related to each observation item. Two buttons,
“Watch Animation” and “Prediction,” are displayed.
When the child presses the “Watch Animation”
button, the animation for each choice is activated.
Figure 3 shows an example of an animation page for
an observation item. The child can only select one
choice among the predictions. Once the choice is
made, the guide will move on to the observation page.
Figure 2: Prediction page.
Figure 3: Example of an animation page.
2.3.2 Result Page (after)
Figure 4 shows a result page. Here, the child is also
given four choices, with the choice he/she made
during the prediction phase indicated by the “My
Prediction” graphic. On this page, two buttons,
“Watch Animation” and “Result,” are displayed.
Similar to the prediction page, when the child
Figure 4: Result page/choices for hind flippers.
CSEDU 2016 - 8th International Conference on Computer Supported Education
354
presses the “Watch Animation” button, the
animation for each choice is activated. When the
child presses the “Result” button, the choice after the
observation is confirmed and the guide will move on
to the next observation item.
3 RESEARCH METHOD AND
DESIGN
3.1 Overview
The aim of this exercise was to facilitate observation
of the features of seals while also examining their
functions and behaviors. More specifically, the
children were to observe the features and behaviors
of seals’ hind flippers, noses, and claws. There were
four choices for each observation item. Figure 4
shows the choices for the hind flippers: 1.
Swimming using hind flippers; 2. Catching
something with hind flippers; 3. Swimming using
fingers; and 4. Catching something with fingers.
Figure 5 shows the choices for the nose: 1. Seals
always open their noses and breathe above water; 2.
Seals always open their noses and spray water on the
water surface; 3. Seals close their noses underwater
and open them to breathe above water; and 4. Seals
close their noses underwater and open them to spray
water on the water surface. Finally, Figure 6 shows
the choices for the claws: 1. Walking with fore
flippers without using claws; 2. Eating with fore
flippers without using claws; 3. Walking with fore
flippers using sharp claws; and 4. Eating with fore
flippers using sharp claws.
Observation was carried out in the following
steps. First, as shown in Figure 7, the children were
presented with the questions and choices using
images and the guide. They were then asked to
predict the right answers from among the choices as
shown in Figure 8. Finally, the children were asked
to observe the behavior of the seals in the exhibit
while using the guide and to select the answers from
among the choices once again (Figure 9). After these
steps, the right answers based on the observation
were revealed and explained. The above steps were
followed for each observation item, requiring
approximately 15 minutes each time. After they had
completed the observation of all the items, namely
the hind flippers, nose, and claws, the children were
interviewed for approximately 30 minutes (Figure
10).
Figure 5: Choices for nose.
Figure 6: Choices for claws.
Figure 7: Staff members showing questions to the
children.
Figure 8: A child watching an animation.
Supporting Zoo Visitors’ Scientific Observations with a Mobile Guide
355
Figure 9: A child using his tablet during observation.
Figure 10: A child being interviewed.
3.2 Participants
The participants were 16 children chosen from the
public; their mean age was 7.0 years (SD=2.0). One
or two adults accompanied each child during the
observation of the seals, and each child received a
tablet to use for this exercise.
3.3 Zoo
The Asahiyama Zoo where this study was conducted
is a pioneer in animal behavior exhibits and ranks
high in the number of visitors among zoos around
the world (Kosuge, 2006).
3.4 Data Source and Analysis
During this workshop, the verbal and non-verbal
communication and behavior of the children were
recorded using IC recorders and video cameras. For
analysis, we used the interview research method to
determine the children’s assessments of the
animations used during the observation of the hind
flippers, nose, and claws, and, more specifically,
whether the animations were useful and why.
4 RESULTS
4.1 Overall Trend
Table 1 shows the number of children who answered
whether the animations were useful or not useful.
Sixteen children stated that the animations for the
hind flippers as well as the nose were useful. Fifteen
children stated that the animations of the claws were
useful while one stated that they were not.
Table 1: Number of children who answered whether these
animations were useful.
Useful Not useful
Hind flippers 16 0
Nose 16 0
Claws 15 1
Note: N=16
4.2 Episodes
There are four episodes of the interview-based
research. Episodes 1 and 2 show the children’s
statements about the usefulness of the animations on
the Mobile Guide. Episode 3 shows a child’s
statement about the uselessness of one of the
animations. Episode 4 shows a child’s statement
about the improvement of the animation.
4.2.1 Episode 1: Interview of B11 about
Hind Flippers' Animation
Table 2 shows the episode with a five-year-old boy
(B11). He says the animations of hind flippers are
useful. He gives the reason, “When I forgot what I
Table 2: Episode 1: Interview of B11 about hind flippers'
animation.
01I11v: Was the animation of hind flippers useful for
observation?
02B11n: Nodding.
03I11v: Why?
04B11v: When I forgot what I had observed, I could
confirm it with the tablet immediately.
05I11v: When you confirmed it, did you need the
animation or just this still result page?
06B11v: I could do it just by the result page.
07I11v: How about the animation? Do you think the
animation was not needed?
08B11v: I needed the animation. As the picture on the
result page did not move, I could not understand how
the seals use their hind flippers.
Note on transcription numbers: Consecutive numbers, B11: a
5-year-old boy
I11: an interviewer
v: verbal behavior, n:
non-verbal behavior.
CSEDU 2016 - 8th International Conference on Computer Supported Education
356
had observed, I could confirm it with the tablet
immediately” (04B11v). To clarify the advantages of
the animation, Interviewer 11 asks, “When you
confirmed it, did you need the animation or just this
still result page?” (05I11v). The child answers, “I
needed the animation. As the picture on the result
page did not move, I could not understand how seals
use their hind flippers” (08B11v). Therefore, by
using the animation, B11 can confirm how seals use
their hind flippers.
4.2.2 Episode 2: Interview of G2 about the
Nose's Animation
Table 3 shows the episode with a six-year-old girl
(G2). She says the animation of the nose is useful,
“Because I could understand how seals move”
(04G2v). Interviewer 2 asks, “When you observed
the seals, did you check the same motion?” (05I2v).
She answers, “Yes” (06G2v). Therefore, G2 gets the
viewpoint for observation of nose’s function and
behavior from the animation. Further, she can
observe them.
Table 3: Episode 2: Interview of G2 about the nose's
animation.
01I2v: Was the animation of nose useful for observation?
02G2v: It was useful.
03I2v: Why?
04G2v: Because I could understand how seals
move.
05I2v: When you observed the seals, did you check the
same motion?
06G2v: Yes.
Note on transcription numbers: Consecutive numbers
G2: a 6-year-old girl
I2: an interviewer
v: verbal
behavior.
4.2.3 Episode 3: Interview of B1 about the
Nose's and Claws' Animation
Table 4 shows the episode with a seven-year-old boy
(B1). He says the animation of the nose is useful, but
the animation of the claws is not useful. B1 explains
that the animation of the nose is needed, “Because,
for example, the air, then you can’t see the air”
(04B1v). However, he says the animation of claws is
not needed, “Because, seal, seal, can eat, ah, you can
see the food and the hunt” (08B1v).
Therefore, B1 thinks that the animation of the
nose is useful because in the animation he can notice
the air, which we cannot see otherwise. He can
understand and observe how seals breathe on the
water. On the other hand, he thinks that the
animation of the claws is not useful because he can
see the food and the hunt. This shows that the
animation of the claws is not as useful for him as
that of the nose.
Table 4: Episode 3: Interview of B1 about the nose's and
claws' animation.
01I1v: Was the animation of nose needed or not?
02B1v: Needed.
03I1v: Why?
04B1v: Because, for example, the air, then you can’t
see the air.
05I1v: Was this animation needed or not?
06B1v: No need.
07I1v: Why?
08B1v: Because, seal, seal, can eat, ah, you can see the
food and the hunt.
Note on transcription numbers: Consecutive numbers
B1: a 7-year-old boy
I1: an interviewer
v: verbal
behavior.
4.2.4 Episode 4: Interview of G12 about the
Nose's Animation
Table 5 shows the episode with an eleven-year-old
girl (G12). She says that the animation of the nose
should be improved. She explains, “For beginners, it
looks misleading. In fact, seals open and close their
noses when they are on the water” (02G12v).
Interviewer 12 asks, “You think the animation is
good but it should be improved, right?” (07I12v).
She answers, “Yes” (08G12v). Therefore, G12 says
that the animation is useful but it should be
improved to represent the viewpoint of the nose’s
function and behavior more realistically.
Table 5: Episode 4: Interview of G12 about the nose's
animation.
01I12v: Was the animation of nose useful for
observation?
02G1v: For beginners, it seems misleading. In fact,
seals open and close their nose when on the water.
03I12v: Seals also open and close their nose, so you
think the animation should be improved, right?
04I12v: But, do you think this animation can show that
seals close their nose under the water and open it on the
water?
05G12v: Yes.
06I12v: You think the animation is good but it should
be improved, right?
07G12v: Yes.
Note on transcription numbers: Consecutive numbers
G12: an 11-year-old girl
I12: an interviewer
v:
verbal behavior.
Supporting Zoo Visitors’ Scientific Observations with a Mobile Guide
357
5 CONCLUSION AND FUTURE
WORKS
This study aimed to develop and assess an
observation guide. We discovered that the vast
majority of the children found this system useful
during the observation and that the system helped
the children understand the movements of the animal
sufficiently to engage in prediction and observation.
Our conclusion, therefore, is that this system was an
effective tool for observing the functions and
behaviors of parts of an animal’s body. To identify
the evidence that the system actually supports
children’s observation, we will examine how the
children use the system by video research for future
works.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This research was supported by JSPS KAKENHI
Grant Number 24240100 and 15K12382.
REFERENCES
Bell, P., Lewenstein, B., Shouse, A. W., & Feder, M. A.
(Eds.), 2009. Learning science in informal
environments: People, place, and pursuit. National
Academies Press. Washington, DC.
Dierking, L. D., Burtnyk, K., Buchner, K. S., & Falk, J. H.,
2002. Visitor learning in zoos and aquariums: A
literature review. American Zoo and Aquarium
Association. Annapolis, MD.
Eberbach, C., & Crowley, K., 2005. From everyday to
scientific observation: How children learn to observe the
biologist's world, Review of Educational Research,
79(1), 39–68.
Jimenez Pazmino, P. F., Lopez Silva, B., Slattery, B., &
Lyons, L, 2013. Teachable mo[bil]ment: Capitalizing on
teachable moments with mobile technology in zoos. CHI
’13 Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in
Computing Systems, ACM, 643–648.
http://doi.org/10.1145/2468356.2468470.
Kosuge, M., 2006. The revolution of the Asahiyama zoo:
Revival project of a dream come true (in Japanese).
Kakukawashoten. Tokyo.
Ohashi, Y., Ogawa, H., & Arisawa, M., 2008. Making
new learning environment in zoo by adopting mobile
devices. Proceedings of the 10th International
Conference on Human Computer Interaction with
Mobile Devices and Services, ACM, 489–490.
http://doi.org/10.1145/1409240.1409323.
Patrick, P. G., & Tunnicliffe, S. D., 2013. Zoo talk. Ne
Springer. Netherlands.
Traxler, J., 2005. Defining mobile learning. In P. Isaias, C.
Borg, & P. Bonanno (Eds.), IADIS International
Conference Mobile Learning 2005, 261-266. Lisbon,
Portugal: IADIS Press.
CSEDU 2016 - 8th International Conference on Computer Supported Education
358