could have diminished the effects of peripheral
perception on the direction of the initial fixation.
More research in this field is needed to clarify the
boundaries of peripheral preprocessing in hungry
participants, when food and non-food items are
presented while also controlling for stimulus
characteristics of these two categories.
The results for first fixation durations showed that
first fixations were longer when household items were
shown compared to food items. This also becomes
clear when considering that household items were
presented on plates. It is obviously odd to see
household items (like keys) presented on a white plate.
We found longer first fixation durations on the right
AOI for hungry participants. When assuming that
fixations towards the left in the current set-up reflect
rather strategic processes, fixations towards the right
might be regarded as more prone to user states.
A similar picture emerged for the amount of
fixations on the AOIs: Overall, there were more
fixations towards household items than towards food
items supporting the assumption that household items
presented on a plate are unfamiliar and therefore more
difficult to process. But again, this effect was more
pronounced for hungry subjects. This indicates that
processing of non-food stimuli – the less relevant or
more distracting category when being hungry - is
impaired in hungry participants.
Our results are in accordance to former studies
showing that hunger and the calorie content of food
pictures also modulates the activation of early visual
areas (Frank et al., 2010). The present study
substantially extends these findings by showing that
hunger might also affect effectiveness of visual
search as indicated by longer fixation duration in
hungry participants when food is presented in the
paradigm. As we did not use a separate task with non-
food stimuli only, potential expectation effects
concerning food might have influenced the HH-
categorization too. In addition to that, presenting
household objects like keys on a plate might have
increased the association with food for these objects
as only food is usually presented or served on plates.
Therefore, future research should consider using a
more naturalistic display of household items.
Previous studies already confirmed the increase in
sympathetic activation and decrease in
parasympathetic activation of hunger (Chan et al.,
2007). Given that pupil size is influenced by
sympathetic activation (e.g. emotional arousal, Ehlers
et al., 2016; Partala and Surakka, 2003), it was
hypothesized that pupil dilation can be linked to
hunger. The results of the present study indeed
demonstrate the first time that the pupils of hungry
participants are more dilated than the pupils of
satiated ones. Moreover, the data show that pupil size
and subjective hunger are positively correlated
suggesting that this measure can also serve for
diagnostic purposes. For user sensing, this means that
pupil dilations have to be carefully interpreted with
regard to potentially activating sources. That is,
whether or not this method allows to discriminate
between different sources of bodily arousal such as
mental stress has to be further elucidated in future
research. Besides, taking additional sources of bodily
arousal into account, further studies should also
examine whether user characteristics such as weight,
height or psychological disorders such as eating
disorders influence the results of hunger on
sympathetic activation and attentional processing.
Hence, our study indicates that pupillometry is a
feasible way to quantify bodily arousal as associated
with hunger feelings. This method is therefore an
innovative way to assess physiological processes in
the context of bodily states.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We want to thank Andreas Stegmaier for his help in
data collection.
REFERENCES
Andersson, B., Wallin, G., Hedner, T., Ahlberg, A. C., &
Andersson, O. K. (1988). Acute effects of short-term
fasting on blood pressure, circulating noradrenaline and
efferent sympathetic nerve activity. Acta Medica
Scandinavica, 223(6), 485–490.
Chan, J. L., Mietus, J. E., Raciti, P. M., Goldberger, A. L.,
& Mantzoros, C. S. (2007). Short-term fasting-induced
autonomic activation and changes in catecholamine
levels are not mediated by changes in leptin levels in
healthy humans. Clinical Endocrinology, 66(1), 49–57.
Ehlers, J., Strauch, C., Georgi, J., & Huckauf, A. (2016).
Pupil Size Changes as an Active Information Channel
for Biofeedback Applications. Applied
Psychophysiology and Biofeedback.
Flint, A., Raben, A., Blundell, J. E., & Astrup, A. (2000).
Reproducibility, power and validity of visual analogue
scales in assessment of appetite sensations in single test
meal studies. International Journal of Obesity, 24(1),
38–48.
Frank, S., Laharnar, N., Kullmann, S., Veit, R., Canova, C.,
Hegner, Y. L., et al. (2010). Processing of food pictures:
Influence of hunger, gender and calorie content. Brain
Research, 1350, 159–166.
Giel, K. E., Friederich, H.-C., Teufel, M., Hautzinger, M.,
Enck, P., & Zipfel, S. (2011). Attentional processing of