Basic Research on Multisensory Methods for Teaching Onomatopoeia to
the Hearing-impaired
Broadening the Experience of Sound
Miki Namatame
1
, Fusako Kusunoki
2
and Shigenori Inagaki
3
1
Department of Industrial Information, Tsukuba University of Technology, Amakubo, Tsukuba, Ibaraki 305-8520, Japan
2
Department of Information Design, Tama Art University, Yarimizu, Hachioji, Tokyo 192-0394, Japan
3
Faculty of Human Development, Kobe University, Tsurukabuto, Nada-ku, Kobe 657-8501, Japan
Keywords:
Hearing-impaired, Science Lesson, Cicada, Vibration, Onomatopoeia.
Abstract:
It is difficult for hearing-impaired individuals to learn animal sounds and the onomatopoeia that depict them
because they have limited or no access to auditory information. To attempt to solve this problem, the author
designed a science lesson to help hearing-impaired students learn cicada songs. This lesson used multiple
media including text, images, sounds, sound waveforms, onomatopoeia and vibrations to stimulate the senses
of sight, hearing and touch. An experiment was conducted on 26 hearing-impaired students, who were split
into two groups. One of the groups was provided with vibrations as part of the lesson and the other was not.
Pre- and post-tests on the names of cicadas and their songs and a lesson evaluation survey were conducted to
assess the lesson’s effectiveness. Although results showed no significant difference in learning between the
two groups, students who were able to discriminate among the different vibrations reported that they found
the lesson enjoyable and that it was a useful way to learn science.
1 INTRODUCTION
The Japanese language is said to use from three to
five times as many onomatopoeic words as western
languages or Chinese (Yamaguchi, 2003). The one
dictionary of Japanese mimetics lists 4500 entries
(Ono, 2007). Educational research has suggested that
onomatopoeia, that is, words which mimic animate
sounds (giseigo), inanimate sounds (giongo) or are
symbolic of states (gitaigo), of emotions (gijougo)
or of actions (giyougo), are easy for students to un-
derstand and enhance their comprehension and ima-
gination (Miyazaki and Tomimatsu, 2009), that high
iconicity between the sound and the referent enables
listeners to accurately generalize the meaning of the
word and to make an immediate connection with the
object being symbolized, and, that sound symbolic
words referring to states, emotions or actions facili-
tate the learning of verbs as children are acquiring vo-
cabulary(Imai and Kita, 2014). So learners of the Ja-
panese language must master onomatopoeia to make
their Japanese more descriptive and expressive(Yusuf
and Watanabe, 2008).
In this way, onomatopoeia plays an important role
in word acquisition, child development and educa-
tion. It has also been found that, when using the au-
ditory modality in the education of hearing-impaired
children, onomatopoeia teaches vocalization patterns
and how to associate meaning with words, which fa-
cilitates verbalization (Nakamura, 2007). This sug-
gests that onomatopoeic words are important for the
hearing-impaired to learn. Other studies have found
that when deaf individuals understood spoken senten-
ces, not only the brain’s left hemisphere (as in na-
tive speakers with normal hearing) but also the right
brain was extensively activated (Neville, 1998), and
that deaf individuals used phonological representati-
ons in visually presented verbal memory tasks simi-
lar to people with normal hearing (Okada and Mat-
suda, 2015). However, when deaf subjects were as-
ked to judge the appropriateness of the use of sound
symbolic words depicting states, actions or emotions
to describe scenes in a video, the visual and audi-
tory association areas in the brain were not activated
(Arata and Matsuda, 2009). These findings support
the idea that it is difficult for hearing-impaired in-
dividuals, who have difficulty accessing auditory in-
formation, to construct linguistic symbols using the
sound symbolism of onomatopoeic words, in spite
of their synesthetic and sound-symbolic characteris-
22
Namatame, M., Kusunoki, F. and Inagaki, S.
Basic Research on Multisensory Methods for Teaching Onomatopoeia to the Hearing-impaired.
DOI: 10.5220/0006665800220027
In Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on Computer Supported Education (CSEDU 2018), pages 22-27
ISBN: 978-989-758-291-2
Copyright
c
2019 by SCITEPRESS Science and Technology Publications, Lda. All rights reserved
tics (Hinton and Ohala, 2006). Thus, onomatopoeias
(also referred to as sound-symbolic words) are diffi-
cult for these individuals to learn. However, thinking
in another way, even without the ability to hold sound
symbols in memory in the absence of auditory infor-
mation, one can postulate that they may be able to
construct other linguistic symbols if a multisensory
environment is created to provide alternative sensory
information, such as vibrations or visual information.
In particular, hearing impairment has been linked to
a heightened sense of touch (Heidenreich and Le-
win, 2012). Previous research on information acces-
sibility and sensory substitution for the disabled has
shown that haptic information is effective in speech
training (Alves and Freire, 2015). Practical applica-
tions using vibrations to provide alerts are already
available, such as, vibrating alarm clocks, vibrating
devices worn in the hair (Honda, 2016), and applica-
tions that alert the user to sounds indicating danger
(Henriquez and Travieso, 2013). In addition, infor-
mation provided through vibrations enables hearing-
impaired individuals to recognize rhythms and enjoy
music (Kanebako and Namatame, 2016). This makes
the use of vibration promising as an alternative to au-
ditory information. Consequently, the author attemp-
ted to develop lesson which would enable people with
impaired hearing to learn sound symbols. A multisen-
sory lesson was designed to teach onomatopoeias mi-
micking animal sounds, which broadened learner ex-
perience beyond sound by simultaneously presenting
them with haptic and visual information in addition to
auditory information.
2 PURPOSE
The purpose of this study was to find out what com-
ponent elements hearing-impaired learners appreciate
in a multisensory lesson designed to teach onomato-
poeias for animal sounds by broadening the learner
experience of sound and whether vibration as a form
of haptic information can play a complementary role
to auditory information in learning.
3 EXPERIMENTAL METHOD
In this report, we develop the teaching materials pro-
moting understanding for synesthesia about the kind
of the experience like insect (cicada) sound which the
hearing-impaired was not able to learn because acqui-
sition of the hearing information is difficult.
We measure the learning effect by comparing the
cases when the vibration information is added or not
added. The evaluation of the way of receiving and the
teaching materials for the sense of touch (vibration)
will be obtained.
3.1 Design of Teaching Material
Because the body of the cicada is the housing, the best
way to learn the cicada’s song is the touching of a ci-
cada. However, it is difficult to hear the original sound
because the cicada makes a different sound when the
cicada is touched. Therefore we designed the multi-
media teaching materials which appealed to hearing,
the sense of touch, sight to help the learning of the
sound of cicada.
We chose seven kinds of cicadas which are well-
known in Japan. The teaching materials contents are
constituted by the commentary sentence about the ha-
bits of the cicada, the onomatopoeia of the cicada’s
song, the images of cicada, the sound information of
the cicada’s song, the waveform of the cicada’s song,
vibration. In the main display, the commentary sen-
tence of the cicada and onomatopoeia and an image
and the button of the cicada’s song are shown.
Figure 1: Configuration of the learning materials.
Basic Research on Multisensory Methods for Teaching Onomatopoeia to the Hearing-impaired
23
Table 1: Specifications of the cicadas’ sound.
cicada onomatopoeia time volume vibration
ms db m/s2
1 Jiri-jiri 45 95 2.8
2 Achi-achi 40 95 27.0
3 Kana-kana 46 93 9.2
4 chi. chi. chi. 41 91 3.7
5 Tsuku-tusku 42 95 3.3
6 min-min 46 95 3.0
7 Chiiii... 45 100 0.6
The sound waveform of the cicada’s song emerges
to a vice-display when a button is clicked, and the ci-
cada’s song is played back. The vibration links with
the cicada’s song by using a vibration speaker. The ci-
cada’s song is edited so that it becomes 93db(+-2db)
for approximately 40 seconds. In addition, the presen-
tation of the cicada in the learning using the teaching
materials is shown at random every time.
The presentation of the cicada is shown automa-
tically when the learner clicks the start button. The
learner can read the commentary sentence of the ci-
cada and onomatopoeia, and then click the button of
the cicada’s song. The learner get the cicada’s song
and the sound waveform with the vibration.
The constitution of the learning materials is shown
in figure.1, the sound condition of each cicada is
shown in table.1.
3.2 Experiment Procedure
The experiment was conducted from 27th Jun. until
6th Jul. in 2017. We had 26 participants with
hearing-impaired (Average age: 21.1 years old). The
participants were divided into two groups. Group A
was 17 participants using the vibration device, and
Group B was 9 participants not using the vibration
device. The experiment was 30 minutes in total per
person. Experiment procedure is following.
1: Informed consent and experiment explanation
2: 2back task (working memory task)
3: PreTest
4: Learning
5: Post Test
6: Questionnaire evaluation
3.3 Evaluation Method of the Lesson
This lesson used multiple media including text, ima-
ges, sounds, sound waveforms, onomatopoeia and vi-
brations to stimulate the senses of sight, hearing and
touch. The following 8 questions were prepared to
evaluate the lesson.
Q1: Good material
text, images, sounds, sound waveforms,
onomatopoeia, (vibrations)
Q2: Did you hear anything?
Q3: Did you distinguish those vibrations?
Q3’:(or Did you need vibration?)
Q4: The degree of easy intelligibility
Q5: The degree of useful
Q6: The degree of interesting
Q7: The degree of enjoyable
Q8: The degree of educative
4 RESULT
4.1 Learning Ability of Participants
As a baseline information, participants were assigned
the 2 Back-task, which you need to decide if the cur-
rent number is the same as the one presented 2 trials
ago, in order to assess the working-memory. After
a practice, formal experiment was conducted twice.
As the mean score of a percentage of two answers
were calculated, mean score of experimental group
was 53.2% and that of control group was 57.9%. In
order to assess the knowledge of cicada’s song, the
participants connected the 7 cicada’s names and 7 ci-
cada’s song (Onomatopoeia) as one by one, before
and after the learning. As a result, a percentage of
correct answers of pre-test was 3.9 for experimental
group and 3.8 for control group. That of post-test
was 6.8 for experimental group and 6.6 for control
group. That is, almost all the participants became
able to match the cicada’s name and song (Onoma-
topoeia) after the learning. Therefore, we regarded
the working-memory and leaning effect of the parti-
cipants as equal, and compared the subjective evalua-
tion about the lesson between the experimental group
and control group.
4.2 Evaluation About the Lesson
These figures (Figure 2Figure 3) show the percentage
that evaluation about good points of the lesson. The
vertical axis shows a percentage and horizontal axis
shows the alternatives. The alternatives are caption,
photo, onomatopoeia, sound, waveform and vibra-
tion. This question was multiple answers. The each
population were the 17 participants which gave vibra-
tion information, and the 9 participants which didn’t
give vibration information.
With or without vibration, we can read that the
onomatopoeia were evaluated as a factor to constitute
lesson. When there was not vibration, the sound was
estimated as an onomatopoeia at the same level, but
CSEDU 2018 - 10th International Conference on Computer Supported Education
24
Figure 2: Good element (Vib.).
Figure 3: Good element (no Vib.).
the sound was not evaluated too much when there was
vibration.
4.3 Subjective Evaluation About the
Lesson
The likert-scale was marked every one point and was
calculated for getting the average. The point of scale
was following.
+2point: strong-agree
+1point: agree
0point: not either
-1point: disagree
-2point: strong-disagree
The result shows the figure.4.
In order to check the differences between group
A (with vibration) and group B (without vibration),
T-test was analyzed. But there were not significant
differences.
Figure 4: Comparison of the subjective evaluation.
4.4 Relation of the Sense
We made two questions about the sense of hearing
and touching. Q2Did you hear anything?, Q3Did you
distinguish those vibrations? The t-test was exami-
ned to provide significant difference of their feeling
and subjective evaluation. The population was the 17
participants which gave vibration information. They
were separated into positive group and the negative
group. The positive-group was comprised of ”agree”
”strong-agree”, and the negative-group was compri-
sed of ”disagree”,”strong-disagree” and ”not either”.
4.4.1 Q2Did You Hear Anything?
The result that The 11 participants was ”can hear so-
mething” and the 6 participants was ”can’t hear any-
thing”. There was no significant difference between
the subjective evaluation (IntelligibilityUseful, Inte-
restingEnjoyableEducative) and hearing-feeling. The
result shows that it was not different in the evaluation
of the lesson by the degree of the hearing.
4.4.2 Q3Did You Distinguish those Vibrations?
The result that The 11 participants was ”can distin-
guish” and the 6 participants was ”can’t distinguish”.
There were significant differences between the
subjective evaluation about ”Interesting”t(15)=3.05,
p.05)and ”Educative”t(15)=2.49, p.05).
The table.2 shows that an evaluation is higher in
the person who was able to distinguish the vibration.
Basic Research on Multisensory Methods for Teaching Onomatopoeia to the Hearing-impaired
25
Table 2: Distinction of the vibration and evaluation of the
lesson.
distinguish no distinguish
AverageS.D. AverageS.D.
Intelligibility 1.09(0.70) 0.50(0.84)
Useful 1.27(0.47) 0.33(1.21)
Interesting 0.45(0.82) 0.17(0.75)
Enjoyable 1.27(0.65) 0.33(0.52)
Educative 1.00(0.77) -0.17(1.17)
5 DISCUSSION
The purpose of this study was to find out whether a
multisensory lesson to teach sound symbolic onoma-
topoeias for animal sounds by broadening the lear-
ner experience of sound beyond auditory information
could facilitate learner comprehension. The subject of
the lesson was cicada songs. Analyses were based on
participants’ evaluations of the lesson and on tests to
determine if the lesson had an effect on participants’
learning .
Results showed no significant difference in lear-
ning between two groups of participants (26 in total),
one that was provided with vibrations as part of the
lesson and one that was not. Post-lesson test results
showed improvement over the pre-lesson results with
almost all participants ultimately answering all the
questions correctly. However, to properly assess the
lesson’s effectiveness, knowledge retention needed to
be measured by conducting another test at some set
period of time after the lesson.
There were inconsistencies in how participants
were able to feel the vibrations in the lesson, which
suggested that the vibrations were not provided in a
way that everyone could understand them. Lesson
content consisted of an explanation of cicada ecology,
the onomatopoeia for the cicada’s song, a picture of
the cicada, the cicada’s song (auditory information),
its sound waveform and, for one of the groups, a vi-
bration.
Regardless of whether vibrations were included
in their group, most participants rated the onomato-
poeias for the songs as a good element in the les-
son. They thought they were useful for learning sound
symbolic words as they conveyed the sounds in word
form.
For the group in which vibrations were included
as an element in the lesson, fewer of the participants
selected the songs auditory information than the vi-
bration as a good element in the lesson, suggesting
that they may have tried to make more use of vibra-
tions when they were available. On the other hand,
when vibrations were not included, more of those par-
ticipants appreciated the inclusion of the songs audi-
tory information. This indicated that these hearing-
impaired individuals were using their residual hea-
ring.
No statistically significant difference was found in
the subjective evaluations of the lesson between the
groups given and not given the vibrations. Nor was
there any significant difference in evaluation results
due to the degree to which participants reported they
were able to hear sounds.
However, a significant difference was shown due
to differences in how participants were able to feel
the vibrations. Those that were able to discriminate
among them reported they found the lesson fun and
that it could be used for teaching science. Given that
including vibrations in a lesson may make learning
sound symbolic words enjoyable, it may be promi-
sing to put them to use in science lessons for young
children with poor hearing.
6 CONCLUSION AND FUTURE
TOPICS
The elements of the multisensory lesson on onomato-
poeia that broadened learner experience of sound and
were most appreciated by the participants in this study
were the onomatopoeia, the sound waveforms for the
cicada songs, and either the song or its vibration, de-
pending on which group they were assigned to.
The study demonstrated that haptic information
like vibrations is a useful alternative to sound that
complements auditory information for people with
difficulty hearing. While results did not show that the
inclusion of vibrations in the lesson facilitated lear-
ner comprehension of sound symbolic onomatopoeia,
it did show vibration to be a promising element for
inclusion in educational materials for the hearing im-
paired.
In the future, it may be possible to develop more
effective lessons by using better vibration devices that
can vibrate in more distinctive ways. Better devices
are needed that can convey more nuances and higher
registers of sound, not vibrate the way an alarm does.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We thank to the students of the Tsukuba University of
Technology who cooperated on the experiment. We
thank J. Kanebako and M. Kitamura.
This work was supported by JSPS KAKENHI
Grant Number 15K12122.
CSEDU 2018 - 10th International Conference on Computer Supported Education
26
REFERENCES
Alves, R. and Freire, R. (2015). Correction of the funda-
mental voice frequency using tactile and visual feed-
back. In Proceedings of the 17th International ACM
SIGACCESS Conference on Computers & Accessibi-
lity. ACM.
Arata, M. and Matsuda, T. (2009). Semantic processing of
mimetic words in deaf individuals. an fmri study. In
26th O2-3,. Japanese Cognitive Science Society.
Heidenreich, M. and Lewin, R. (2012). Deafness linked to
heightened sense of touch. In Nature Neuroscience.
Springer Nature.
Henriquez, P. and Travieso, C. (2013). Review of automatic
fault diagnosis systems using audio and vibration sig-
nals. In TRANSACTIONS ON SYSTEMS, MAN, AND
CYBERNETICS: SYSTEMS. IEEE.
Hinton, L. and Ohala, J. (2006). Sound Symbolism. Cam-
bridge University Press, USA, 1st edition.
Honda, T. (2016). ontenna http://ontenna.jp/en/. Fujitsu,
Tokyo.
Imai, M. and Kita, S. (2014). The sound symbolism boots-
trapping hypothesis for language acquisition and lan-
guage evolution. In PUBMED. US National Library
of Medicine.
Kanebako, J. and Namatame, M. (2016). Effect of vibration
on listening sound for a person with hearing loss. In
Computers Helping People with Special Needs. Sprin-
ger Nature.
Miyazaki, A. and Tomimatsu, K. (2009). Onomato pla-
nets: physical computing of japanese onomatopoeia.
In Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on
Tangible and Embedded Interaction. ACM.
Nakamura, K. (2007). Use of the auditory modality and
language acquisition. In The Journal Logopedics and
Phoniatrics (in Japanese). The Japan Society of Lo-
gopedics and Phoniatrics.
Neville, H. (1998). Cerebral organization for language in
deaf and hearing subjects. In Proc Natl Acad Sci USA.
National Center for Biotechnology Information.
Okada, R. and Matsuda, T. (2015). The deaf utilize pho-
nological representations in visually presented verbal
memory tasks. In Neuroscience Research. National
Center for Biotechnology Information.
Ono, M. (2007). Nihongo onomatope jiten (Japanese Mi-
metics Dictionary). Shogakkan, Tokyo, 1st edition.
Yamaguchi, N. (2003). Giongo gitaigo jiten (Onomato-
poeia Mimetic words Dictionary). Kodansha, Tokyo,
2nd edition.
Yusuf, M. and Watanabe, C. (2008). Onomatopeta!: De-
veloping a japanese onomatopoeia learning-support
system utilizing native speakers cooperation. In
Proceedings of the 2008 IEEE/WIC/ACM Internati-
onal Conference on Web Intelligence and Intelligent
Agent Technology. IEEE Computer Society Washing-
ton, DC, USA.
Basic Research on Multisensory Methods for Teaching Onomatopoeia to the Hearing-impaired
27