The Good, the Bad and the Ugly: Affect and its Role for Renewable
Energy Acceptance
Barbara S. Zaunbrecher, Katrin Arning and Martina Ziefle
Chair for Communication Science, Human-Computer Interaction Center, RWTH Aachen University,
Campus Boulevard 57, 52074 Aachen, Germany
Keywords:
Emotions, Technology Acceptance, Energy Transition, Semantic Differential.
Abstract:
To foster a socially accepted energy transition, it is essential to gain insights into motives for acceptance or re-
jection of technologies related to renewable energies. The study aims to shed light on the emotional evaluation
of renewable energy technologies and in how far the affective responses are correlated with the acceptance
of those technologies. An empirical study is conducted in which a semantic differential is used to assess
emotional evaluation of wind power, solar power (PV) and biomass for electricity production. Furthermore,
general acceptance of the technologies was assessed. It was found that not only did the technologies differ in
terms of emotional responses they evoked, but that those responses also varied significantly between groups of
high and low levels of acceptance concerning the respective energy technology. By analyzing spontaneous as-
sociations with the three energy sources, possible reasons for the affective evaluations were identified, which
can provide essential topics for the communication about those technologies. Overall, the three renewable
energy technologies revealed different emotional evaluations which might considerably impact the overall ac-
ceptance. It is argued that knowledge about such affective perceptions is useful to tailor energy technology
development in early phases and to steer public information and communication strategies.
1 INTRODUCTION
By decentralizing energy production infrastructure in
the course of the energy transition, it is moving closer
to residential areas. This often causes opposition in
the public, as people fear, e.g., health risks (Crich-
ton et al., 2014), spoiled landscapes (Johansson and
Laike, 2007; Upreti and van der Horst, 2004), and, as
a consequence, reduced property values (Elliott and
Wadley, 2002). Other reasons to oppose are related to
the project planning, for example, a lack of trust in de-
velopers, planners, and policy makers, alongside with
perceived unfairness of the decision making process
(R
¨
osch and Kaltschmitt, 1999; Gross, 2007; Zoellner
et al., 2008). It has been shown that opposition leads
to negative consequences for the planned infrastruc-
ture projects, but that early and systematic participa-
tion of the public can help to decrease these negative
effects (D
¨
utschke et al., 2017). In order to support
this participation with targeted information and com-
munication concepts, it is essential to understand how
the public forms attitudes towards energy infrastruc-
ture to be able to address the issues raised. From a
technical point of view, an energy source is often eval-
uated along objective criteria, such as costs (Benitez
et al., 2008; Klaiß et al., 1995) or CO2 footprint (Sims
et al., 2003). From a social science point of view, the
public attitudes towards renewable energy technolo-
gies can additionally be influenced by subjective fac-
tors, such as affect (Huijts et al., 2012) and emotional
connotations of the energy source and its infrastruc-
ture, such as the perception of risks and uncertainty
(Frewer et al., 2002).
Affect is defined as “[t]he specific quality of
“goodness” or “badness” (i) experienced as a feeling
state (with or without consciousness) and (ii) demar-
cating a positive or negative quality of a stimulus.
(Slovic et al., 2007, p. 1333). Affect has a powerful
influence on risk perception and decision making by
providing mental “short cuts” which people rely on
for their assessment (Slovic et al., 2005). The impor-
tance of affect is underlined by findings which suggest
affective evaluations can even “overrule” cognitions:
“When cognitions and affect point in the same direc-
tion (e.g., are both positive or both negative), they
equally contribute to attitude, but when they contra-
dict, then feelings tend to dominate over cognitions
in the formation of attitudes. This shows the impor-
tance of including both cognitions and affects as an-
tecedents for attitudes. (Huijts et al., 2012, p. 528).
Zaunbrecher, B., Arning, K. and Ziefle, M.
The Good, the Bad and the Ugly: Affect and its Role for Renewable Energy Acceptance.
DOI: 10.5220/0006795003250336
In Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Smart Cities and Green ICT Systems (SMARTGREENS 2018), pages 325-336
ISBN: 978-989-758-292-9
Copyright
c
2019 by SCITEPRESS – Science and Technology Publications, Lda. All rights reserved
325
Despite its potential influence on attitudes and deci-
sion making, affect has, to the best of our knowledge,
not yet been profoundly studied in the context of re-
newable energy technologies. This applies in par-
ticular to a quantified measurement of affective re-
sponses to renewable energies and an attempt to an-
alyze the relation between affective responses and ac-
ceptance. The paper aims at uncovering both, affec-
tive responses to renewable energies on the one hand,
and the relation between those responses and accep-
tance. To this end, first, studies on affect in the context
of energy will be reviewed and the remaining research
gap will be identified. The method of the study is then
pointed out, followed by detailed results on affect and
general acceptance of the energy technologies. In the
discussion, the results are critically analyzed with re-
gard to their contribution to acceptance research and
policy implications. Finally, the methodological ap-
proach is discussed.
2 ACCEPTANCE AND AFFECT IN
THE CONTEXT OF ENERGY
SUPPLY
Affect and emotional connotations of energy sources
play a particularly interesting role in the context of
the energy transition. First of all, their relevance in
the context of renewable energies is given by the com-
plexity of the topic of energy supply. Affect and un-
derlying emotional connotations have been shown to
play a key role in decision making, especially when
the context is complex and uncertain (Slovic et al.,
2007). It is therefore likely that affect will also play a
role for forming attitudes about renewable energies.
The complexity here arises, for example, due to a
generally positive image in the public (sustainabil-
ity), but, at the same time, possible negative effects
on the environment (“green on green conflicts”, (War-
ren et al., 2005)), leading to conflicting viewpoints.
This makes it difficult for laypersons to come to an
informed decision (Frewer et al., 2002).
Second, it has been shown that communicative
measures are desperately needed to integrate the pub-
lic in renewable energy planning, as this can help to
overcome opposition, and, as a result of this opposi-
tion, overcome delays of projects and dissatisfaction
in the public (D
¨
utschke et al., 2017). It is argued that
there is a need to increase the public understanding
by involving the public in early phases of technology
development and local infrastructure planning and ad-
dressing the public’s specific information and com-
munication needs. In order to tailor those commu-
nicative measures, “it is crucial to gain insights into
the mental representations of renewable energy sys-
tems and their specific affective evaluation. (S
¨
utterlin
and Siegrist, 2017, p. 362). Exploring affect and men-
tal representations can, for example, help to uncover
seemingly paradox attitudes among laypersons (Leis-
erowitz, 2006).
For these reasons, the study of affect in the con-
text of energy and climate change has gained impor-
tance in recent years. A number of studies focused
on the influence of affect on attitudes towards nu-
clear energy, in order to understand reasons for op-
position and support. In a study by Finucane et al., it
was investigated how risks and benefits of nuclear en-
ergy were perceived when affect as influential factor
is taken into account (Finucane et al., 2000). By pro-
viding biased information to the participants, it was
observed how an altered overall impression changed
assessment of risks and benefits of the technology and
it was concluded that affect moderates both, perceived
risks and benefits. While Leiserowitz’ and Finucane’s
studies provided a more conceptual approach to the
affect heuristic, other studies pursued more content-
driven approaches, in order to uncover the motives
behind attitudes towards different energy sources. In
Keller et al., the influence of affect, assessed through
spontaneous associations, on acceptance of nuclear
power plants was analyzed, also taking user factors
(gender) into account (Keller et al., 2012). There was
a relationship between the affective representation of
nuclear power plants and the acceptance of a new
generation of nuclear power plants: significant differ-
ences were found between the associations of those
who opposed to nuclear power plants and those who
were more accepting towards them. Beyond the af-
fective evaluation, it was shown that also the content
of the associations is worth investigating to come to a
thorough understanding of acceptance.
Comparative approaches, in which more than one
energy source is investigated with regard to affect,
have also been undertaken. S
¨
utterlin and Siegrist in-
vestigated attitudes towards hydroelectric power, so-
lar power, wind power, and, as a contrast, nuclear
power, but only elicited affective imagery for solar
power (S
¨
utterlin and Siegrist, 2017). For the latter,
they found positive affective connotations. They also
found that affect influences abstract acceptance more
than concrete acceptance. In the study by Truelove
et al., nuclear energy was compared to wind energy,
natural gas and coal with regard to implicit associa-
tions and policy support (Truelove et al., 2014). It
was found that not only negative, but also positive,
implicit associations persisted. They had, however,
only moderate impact on policy support. Truelove et
SMARTGREENS 2018 - 7th International Conference on Smart Cities and Green ICT Systems
326
al. also pointed out that the reference against which
an energy source is compared should be carefully cho-
sen (in this study, nuclear energy was for example
evaluated more positive than coal, but not preferred
over natural gas). Most similar to our approach, Renn
(1982) compared “the emotional component of an at-
titude” (p. 251) towards nuclear energy, coal and PV
on a semantic differential scale (Renn, 1982). In the
majority of dimensions, though not in all, PV was
evaluated most positively.
Finally, it has been shown that the influence of the
affect heuristic is not limited to decision making of
laypersons, but can also be found for experts. Chas-
sot et al. investigated how implicit cognition is re-
lated to the investment behavior of energy investors
(Chassot et al., 2015), and, also for this target group, a
link between positive associations and decision mak-
ing could be observed. In the context of the energy
transition, they highlight the necessity to look beyond
rational arguments: “Implicit associations may hin-
der investment in new energy technologies in ways
that are not obvious to the observer, and sometimes
inaccessible to the decision-maker himself.” (p. 291).
It is important to note that in none of the studies,
associations were evaluated for technical accuracy or
logic. Rather, “[the] intention was merely to measure
the structure of attitudes, to find common types of rea-
soning in this matter and to investigate the processes
involved in making up one’s mind (...)” (Renn, 1982,
p. 243). Thus, although the mental representations
might not correspond to technical realities, they are,
nevertheless, important and decisive, because they re-
flect laypersons’ realities and perceptions, which, in
turn, influence the decision for or against a specific
energy supply solution. In this sense, they are no less
“valid” than technical facts when it comes to decision
making for laypersons as they can even overrule tech-
nical “truths”. This calls for an open-minded commu-
nication with the public from a planning and policy
perspective, in which laypeople’s concerns are taken
seriously.
Even though the studies used different methods,
focused on different technologies and different tar-
get variables, they provide strong evidence for the as-
sumption that the affect heuristic plays a major role
for the acceptance of different types of energy and in
the energy transition in general. So far, a comparison
between different renewable energies with regard to
affect and attitude is still lacking, although especially
in this context, public perceptions are important to un-
derstand in order to better understand acceptance and
opposition and react in apporpriate ways.
3 EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE
In order to illustrate the interplay between affect and
attitudes towards renewable energies, an empirical
study was designed in which three renewable energy
sources (wind, solar power (PV), biomass) were ana-
lyzed with regard to affective attitudes (implicit) and
acceptance (explicit). In addition, a word association
task served as means to closer examine underlying
motifs, reflecting mental concepts and cognitions as
part of the affective evaluation.
3.1 Methodological Approach
Data were collected using a quantitative approach for
which an online questionnaire was designed. It was
distributed via mail and social networks to an inter-
ested German audience. Participation was not grati-
fied and was voluntary. Participants were ensured that
their personal data and all of the results are treated
confidentially and in accordance with the high pri-
vacy standards of the research institution. The ques-
tionnaire consisted of two major parts: the first sec-
tion assessed demographic information and user char-
acteristics. Regarding the demographic information,
age, gender, education level, occupation, area of liv-
ing, and other energy infrastructure in the vicinity
were asked for. The user characteristics examined in
the study were general technical attitude (5 items, as
used and tested in (Himmel et al., 2014)) and envi-
ronmental attitude, (combined from attitude towards
renewables (adapted from (Siegrist, 1996)) and envi-
ronmental awareness (selected items from NEP scale
(Dunlap et al., 2000))
1
. Attitude towards technologies
in general and environmental attitude have been found
to influence acceptance of technology in general and
of renewable energy infrastructure in particular (Za-
unbrecher et al., 2014). Both attitudinal traits were
therefore hypothesized to also influence emotive re-
sponses to energy infrastructure and thus needed to be
controlled for. Cronbach’s Alpha for general attitude
towards technology was measured at 0.86, and for
attitude towards the environment at 0.79, the scales
were thus considered reliable.
The second section of the questionnaire assessed
attitude towards wind energy, PV and biomass. For
all questions, 6-point Likert scales were used as an-
swering format to measure agreement (1= do not
agree at all, 6= fully agree). Participants were asked
for their (subjective) knowledge about wind energy,
PV and biomass (“I feel well informed about” and
“I know how a (windpower plant/PV plant/biomass
1
Detailed questionnaire available from the authors by re-
quest.
The Good, the Bad and the Ugly: Affect and its Role for Renewable Energy Acceptance
327
plant) works”). These questions were used to exam-
ine influence of knowledge on emotive responses on
the semantic differential scale. In addition, partici-
pants were asked ve questions on each renewable
energy technology to determine general acceptance of
the technologies (Items see Table 2).
The emotional assessment of wind energy,
biomass and PV was measured using a semantic dif-
ferential scale (Osgood, 1952) for each of the three
technologies . It is described as especially useful
to measure “meanings that are based on emotions”
(Bergler, 1975, p. 69). The semantic differential con-
sisted of 13 adjective-pairs which had previously been
tested by ve independent participants for their suit-
ability in the context of energy-related infrastructure
(for dimensions see Figure 1). Furthermore, par-
ticipants were asked to note down spontaneous as-
sociations with the three energy technologies to en-
rich the quantitative with qualitative data and un-
cover acceptance-relevant aspects which had not been
treated in the questionnaire so far. For statistical eval-
uation of the results, SPSS (Version 21) was used.
The significance level was set at 5%. Missing per-
centages adding up to 100% are due to missing an-
swers from some participants.
3.2 Sample
329 of originally 389 participants were included in
the sample for analysis (excluded participants had
not completed any of the questions on demographics
or user characteristics). 55.3% were female, 42.9%
male. The mean age was 33.7 years (SD= 15.7),
with respondents up to 30 years of age accounting
for around two thirds of the sample. The educa-
tional level was high, 57.1% of the respondents held
a university degree and further 27.1% school degrees
that qualify for university entrance (“Abitur”). 10.0%
had completed vocational training, and 3.3% had ob-
tained a basic school leaving certificate. 58.4% lived
in the city center, while around 27.7% lived in sub-
urbs, 12.5% lived in a village. To control for ex-
posure to (energy) infrastructure in their daily lives,
participants were asked which (energy) infrastruc-
ture was within view of their homes (multiple an-
swers possible). 13.4% reported to have transmis-
sion lines in view of their homes, 8.2% wind power
plants, 1.5% biomass plants, 21.3% PV installations.
Further 26.4% had a mobile phone base station in
view of their home and 54.4% indicated that none
of the above could be seen from their home. Re-
garding user diversity, participants showed an over-
all openness towards technology (general attitude to-
wards technologies: M=4.7, (Maximum: 6), SD=0.9)
and a high environmental awareness (M=4.8 (Maxi-
mum: 6), SD=0.7).
3.3 Results
First, the knowledge about and the general acceptance
of renewable energies will be reported. Afterwards,
results from the semantic differential as well as qual-
itative results are presented.
Participants reported overall average perceived
knowledge with mean values between 2.8 and 4.1
(Maximum: 6) (Table 1). They reported highest
scores for knowledge about and functionality of wind
power plants (M=3.6 and M=4.1, respectively) and
lowest perceived knowledge for biomass plants. Val-
ues for the feeling of being informed and perceived
knowledge of function correlated highly (PV: r=0.77,
p 0.01, Wind: r=0.68, p 0.01, Biomass: r=0.75,
p 0.01) and the two respective items are thus com-
bined (mean value) for subsequent analyses to one
factor (“knowledge”).
Table 1: Perceived knowledge about renewable energy in-
frastructure (N=326-328, Scale: Min: 1, Max: 6).
Feel informed Know about
functionality
PV 3.3 (SD=1.4) 3.6 (SD=1.5)
Wind 3.6 (SD=1.2) 4.1 (SD=1.3)
Biomass 2.8 (SD=1.2) 3.0 (SD=1.4)
Regarding general acceptance of the technologies
(Table 2), PV was overall most favored. Participants
were least unhappy if it would be built nearby, found it
least dangerous, accepted it most within view of their
house, expected least health risks and welcomed it
most for energy supply of their hometown. Biomass,
on the other hand, was least favored by the partici-
pants. People were most unhappy at the thought of a
biomass plant nearby or in view of their house, found
it most dangerous and thus also expected the most
health risks and support was weakest for their home
town to be supplied by this form of renewable energy.
Wind energy was rated worse than PV, but better than
biomass on all dimensions of acceptance.
While these first questions asked for specific at-
titudes towards different renewable energy sources,
the semantic differential required participants to indi-
cate their intuitive rating of renewable energies with
regard to different dimensions. Results are depicted
in Figure 1. The semantic differential shows distinct
profiles for each renewable energy source. Biomass
was consistently rated worse than PV and wind en-
ergy. Ratings for biomass were especially negative (
3.5) for the dimensions “ugly-beautiful”, “disturbing-
SMARTGREENS 2018 - 7th International Conference on Smart Cities and Green ICT Systems
328
Table 2: Acceptance of renewable energy infrastructure.
PV Wind energy Biomass
N M SD N M SD N M SD
I would be unhappy if X was built nearby. 313 2.4 1.4 314 2.9 1.3 302 3.2 1.3
I find X dangerous. 311 1.9 0.9 312 2.2 1.0 298 2.5 1.0
I would accept X in view of my house. 314 4.7 1.3 315 4.2 1.4 305 3.4 1.2
I fear that X could cause health risks. 312 1.8 1.0 313 2.1 1.2 300 2.5 1.0
I would support it if my town
decided to use X for energy supply
314 5.0 1.1 315 4.9 1.2 300 4.1 1.2
Figure 1: Semantic Differential for renewable energy in-
frastructure (N=292-315).
pleasant”, “alien-familiar”, “repulsive-attractive” and
“dirty-clean”. Biomass received the most positive rat-
ing for usefulness. For wind energy and PV, the pic-
ture was less distinct than for biomass. In some di-
mensions, PV was rated more positive than wind en-
ergy, in others, the opposite occurred. While wind
energy was rated more mature than PV, PV was rated
more beautiful. There were only minor differences in
the rating of their usefulness with high overall values
( 5.0). PV was rated more pleasant, modern and
fascinating than wind energy. Regarding complex-
ity and familiarity, wind energy was rated to be sim-
pler and more familiar. PV reached higher values for
attractiveness, peacefulness, and was perceived more
positive and safer. Regarding cleanliness, both energy
sources were rated almost equally positive ( 5.0).
In a next step, to investigate the relation between
ratings on the semantic differential and acceptance,
the participants were split according to the levels of
general acceptance towards the respective technolo-
gies to compare semantic differential evaluations be-
tween the two groups. For each participant, a “general
acceptance” score was calculated for each technology
using the mean score of the acceptance items (Table 2,
negatively worded items were recoded), taking M=3.5
as cut-off value between the “high acceptance” (HA)
and the “low acceptance” (LA) group. This was done
with reference to the 6-point Likert scale, for which
3.5 presented the midpoint of the scale and the tip-
ping point between agreement and rejection. Partic-
ipants who had not answered all questions related to
acceptance of the respective energy sources did not
receive a “general acceptance” score and were there-
fore excluded from the group analysis.
For all energy sources, the HA groups scored
higher than the LA groups on all dimensions of the
semantic differential (Figure 2 to 4). This indicates
that affective ratings are related to the explicitly stated
acceptance, supporting the assumption that the af-
fect heuristic holds true for the context of the renew-
able energy sources. Depending on the specific en-
ergy source, the differences between the groups, as
well as groups sizes and characteristics varied (Ta-
ble 3, Appendix). For biomass, differences between
the LA group (n=73) and the HA group (n=236)
were largest for the dimensions “positive-negative”,
“safety” and “threatening-peaceful” (Figure 2). The
two groups differed significantly in terms of gender
distribution, attitude towards technologies and knowl-
edge about biomass: In the HA group, there were
more male participants and group members had a
more positive attitude towards technologies as well
as higher reported knowledge about biomass. For
PV, the differences between the groups (LA group:
n=16, HA group: n= 299) were the larger than for
biomass, with the largest differences occurring on
the same dimensions (“positive-negative”, “safety”
and “‘threatening-peaceful”) (Figure 3). The groups
according to PV-acceptance differed significantly in
gender distribution, attitude towards technology and
knowledge about PV: The HA group had a higher
share of male participants, a more positive attitude
towards technology and reported better knowledge
about PV. For wind energy, the two groups (LA group:
n=41, HA group: n= 275) produced extreme differ-
ences with respect to the rating on the semantic differ-
ential similar to the PV groups (Figure 4). The largest
differences occurred on the dimensions “positive-
negative”, “threatening-peaceful”, and “disturbing-
pleasant”. The two groups differed significantly in
age, educational level and environmental attitude:
The Good, the Bad and the Ugly: Affect and its Role for Renewable Energy Acceptance
329
Figure 2: Semantic differential for biomass for high accep-
tance and low acceptance groups.
Figure 3: Semantic differential for PV for high acceptance
and low acceptance groups.
Figure 4: Semantic differential for wind power for high ac-
ceptance and low acceptance groups.
The HA group was younger, better educated and had
a more ecologically oriented attitude.
Summarizing results for the three different renew-
able energy sources, feelings of danger or threat were
most distinguishing between the HA and LA groups
for all energy sources. For wind energy, the visual ap-
pearance of wind turbines (evoking feelings of being
“pleasant” or “disturbing”) was an additional distin-
guishing factor. These findings show the underlying
affective evaluations which can help to explain ex-
plicit acceptance ratings.
In addition to rating renewable sources on se-
mantic differential scales, participants were asked for
their spontaneous associations with PV, wind energy
and biomass. Associations and remarks were cate-
gorized, summarizing those associations which were
similar to each other in one category (e.g.: “un-
known”, “not enough information”, “no knowledge
about it” and similar associations were summarized
in “lack of knowledge”). Association categories with
four or more mentions are depicted in Figures 5 to 7
2
.
For PV, 251 participants produced 458 associa-
tions (Figure 5). The two most dominant associa-
tions were “sun”, referring to the source of the en-
ergy, followed by the typical location in which PV
panels are found (“roofs”). Further frequent asso-
ciations included references to the type of energy
(heat/electricity), as well as general favorable eval-
uations of PV as a type of renewable energy (e.g.,
“clean”, “useful”, “modern”), with the exception of
“expensive” and “ugly”. Participants also frequently
referred to the dependence on the weather, as well as
unfavorable climatic conditions in Germany for PV as
a general problem. In contrast to biomass and wind
power, PV was seen as a renewable energy “for ev-
eryone”, referring to the possibility of installing it at
one’s own home. In general, PV was seen as a rather
unobtrusive technology, with the downsides of price
and weather-dependency.
In the case of wind energy, 263 participants pro-
duced 535 associations (Figure 6). From the results,
the bigger impact of wind power plants on their envi-
ronment, in contrast to PV, is clearly visible: although
considered to be “clean”, the “size”,“noise emissions”
and “shadows” of wind power plants are frequently
cited. Besides, there is evidence that the “green on
green” conflict (Warren et al., 2005) concerning pos-
sible harm to animals (birds, bats, etc.) as well as
“landscape impact”, is deeply anchored in peoples’
minds. In contrast to PV, however, there were also
very positive statements, in which wind power plants
were described as “majestic” or even “elegant”. This
ties in with the findings from the semantic differential
results, revealing that HA and LA groups produced
extreme results using almost the entire width of the
scale, which reflects extremely opposing viewpoints.
For biomass, 232 participants produced 356 asso-
ciations (Figure 7). The most striking result is that
among the 22 most frequently mentioned categories–
only two (!) are clearly positive (“ecological” and
“useful”). “Odor/stench” was the most dominant as-
sociation, which is coined negatively. This is in strong
2
Full list of associations available upon request from the
authors.
SMARTGREENS 2018 - 7th International Conference on Smart Cities and Green ICT Systems
330
contrast to wind power and PV, for which “clean” was
among the most frequent associations. Further neg-
ative associations contained references to ecological
(“monocultures”) as well as ethical concerns (“waste
of food”). Besides, the difference to PV and wind
power was that no specific infrastructure was among
the most frequent associations for biomass, in contrast
to, e.g., “wind turbines” or “solar panels”.
Summarizing, the results from the direct accep-
tance measurement (using items), the semantic dif-
ferential and the word association task uncovered a
differentiated acceptance-pattern for biomass, wind
power and PV. According to the results, PV was most
accepted, while wind power was seen more controver-
sial, and biomass was least accepted. While the items
to measure acceptance already brought this pattern
to light, the semantic differential helped to system-
atically compare the three renewable energies along
affective dimensions. Based on the theory of the af-
fect heuristic, the semantic differential and the spon-
taneous word associations helped to uncover the hid-
den drivers of these attitudes: while PV was valued
for its unobtrusiveness, wind power was seen more
critical because of its high impact on landscapes and
fauna. Biomass was frequently associated with neg-
ative concepts (dirt, manure, stench), thus receiving
an overall more negative evaluation. Besides, the re-
lation between affective ratings and acceptance was
shown.
4 DISCUSSION
In this research, the public acceptance of three dif-
ferent renewable energy sources, wind power, solar
power and biomass, was empirically studied. The
overall aim was to gain insights into motives for ac-
ceptance or rejection of technologies related to renew-
able energies. Previous studies (Joffe, 2003; Renn,
1982; Slovic et al., 2005; Slovic et al., 2007) have
shown that –apart from “objective” or “factual” ar-
guments, such as costs, known sustainability effects,
or CO2 footprints– acceptance of the public also re-
lies on implicit affective evaluations. In order to shed
light on these hidden emotional evaluations of renew-
able energy technologies and to understand in how far
the affective responses are related to the acceptance
of those technologies, mental models and associations
of participants were investigated and analyzed in re-
lation to acceptance groups. The study revealed that,
overall, the technologies differed distinctly in terms
of emotional responses they evoked. In addition, the
affective evaluations varied significantly between LA
and HA groups, supporting the theory that affect in-
fluences attitudes. The analysis of the spontaneous
associations revealed possible reasons for attitudes to-
wards the energy sources, which might contribute to
future information strategies for the respective tech-
nologies directed at laypersons. Following, key find-
ings are summarized as well as their policy implica-
tions, followed by a discussion of the effectiveness
of the methodology. The section closes by outlining
some limitations and future research duties.
4.1 Key Findings: Perceptions of
Energy Technologies
The results support the hypothesis that there exists a
link between the affect heuristic and perceptions of
renewable energies: the energy sources with positive
ratings on the semantic differential and in word as-
sociation tasks were perceived more positive regard-
ing risks and acceptability than the one with nega-
tive ratings and negative associations. Biomass re-
ceived, overall, the most negative evaluation, PV the
most positive. This is reflected in the acceptance-
judgments: Biomass was seen as bearing the largest
risk (most dangerous, most fear of health risks), PV
as being least dangerous and bearing the least health
risks, as well as being supported most for communal
energy supply. Wind power, for which results were
in the middle of the answering-scales on most affec-
tive dimensions, showed the lowest distances between
the risk and benefit items. This corresponds to results
by Alhakami and Slovic, who found on the one hand
that “items with intense positive or intense negative
evaluations had the largest distance between risk and
benefit judgments.
3
” and that “items in the middle of
the evaluation scale were associated with smaller dis-
tances.” (Alhakami and Slovic, 1994, p. 1095).
From the results of the semantic differential and
the acceptance groups, it can be deduced that PV rep-
resents the overall most “agreeable” renewable en-
ergy. It is thus not only more positive evaluated than
fossil sources (Renn, 1982), but also than the renew-
ables it was compared against. From the associa-
tions, the reasons for this can be assumed: it does nei-
ther have an impact on landscape, nature, nor humans
through emissions of any kind. The group separation
according to acceptance showed large differences on
the semantic differential scales for PV, however, the
LA-group comprised of only 16 participants, as the
overall acceptance of PV was high. The very selec-
tive group could therefore account for the extreme
views. Wind power, according to the associations,
provoked equally strong feelings in the two groups,
3
“Item” is to be understood here as “artefact under
study”, rather than “question”.
The Good, the Bad and the Ugly: Affect and its Role for Renewable Energy Acceptance
331
Figure 5: Associations with the term “photovoltaics”, total: 460 associations.
Figure 6: Associations with the term “wind energy”, total: 535 associations.
Figure 7: Associated with the term “biomass”, total: 357 associations.
SMARTGREENS 2018 - 7th International Conference on Smart Cities and Green ICT Systems
332
on the positive as well as on the negative end of the
scale. From the group comparison and the associa-
tions it can be inferred that it is the most debated of
the energy sources, this will be especially true for peo-
ple with concerns for the welfare of animals and those
sensitive towards landscape change. Biomass repre-
sents the renewable energy source on the most neg-
ative end of the scale from the perspective of social
acceptability. Besides scoring comparably negative
on the affect-scales (semantic differential), the word
associations were mostly unfavorable. Besides odor
emissions, negative associations contained references
to ecological (monocultures) as well as ethical con-
cerns (waste of food). These barriers are in line with
other studies on the acceptance of biomass (R
¨
osch
and Kaltschmitt, 1999; Kortsch et al., 2015). Ac-
cording to the associations, biomass did not provoke
many positive associations, which could explain the
overall hesitant attitude towards it, in contrast to other
studies, in which positive aspects such as job secu-
rity were frequently mentioned (Kortsch et al., 2015).
Concerning the impact of user diversity, no clear pat-
tern emerged in which one demographic variable or
user factor was similarly influential in all technology
contexts. It thus seems that next to a differentiated
acceptance pattern on the level of content, also the
influence of user characteristics is different for each
technology. These results should, however, be treated
with care and need further evidence, as some of the
groups observed were only small.
4.2 Methodological Discussion
As in this study, qualitative and quantitative mea-
sures were used to uncover users’ attitudes and af-
fective drivers of energy technology acceptance, the
methodology needs a thorough discussion regarding
its strengths and reliability. The application of the
semantic differential had the advantage that a di-
rect, quantitative comparison between the three en-
ergy sources was possible in terms of affect, as they
were rated on the same scales. In combination with
free associations, the method was useful for a compar-
ative view on affect in the domain of energy supply.
Other studies have compared affect related to wind
and solar energy with coal, nuclear and energy from
gas (e.g., (Lee, 2015)), while we focused specifically
on a comparison between renewables only. Method-
ologically, this approach was chosen because even re-
newables, although generally having a positive image,
have considerable drawbacks, which might have been
diminished when comparing them to fossil fuels. In
contrast to quantitative approaches, in which attitudes
are measured and used to make predictions follow-
ing a theoretical model, the use of qualitative mea-
sures and the findings might leave, though insightful,
a fuzzy and somewhat elusive impression behind in
terms of arbitrariness and randomness of the findings.
One might argue that these qualitative and indirect
measures to uncover affective evaluations might miss
the “numerical” rigor in comparison to quantitative
analyses which use inferential statistics to validate
findings. Even if we cannot fully rule out this justi-
fiable argumentation, only by such “unregulated” and
“free” methodology it is possible to uncover the hid-
den drivers that underlie affective evaluations which
impact acceptance and decision making.
Thus, the combination of qualitative and quantita-
tive methods is mandatory to detect affective accep-
tance drivers, at least in early phases of acceptance
modeling. In the next step, a cross validation with a
more detailed quantitative model is needed. Regard-
ing policy, appealing to the “conscious level of deci-
sion making”, e.g. by presenting technical facts on
new renewable energy projects, may not be the most
effective way of promoting renewable energy infras-
tructure. This also highlights the need for an interdis-
ciplinary approach to such projects which takes the
human factor into account rather than base decisions
on technical parameters only.
4.3 Implications for Policy and
Communication
Considering the implications of the results for public
communication and policy, different topics are rele-
vant for the communication about the different energy
sources. For wind, the impact on landscape and nature
was frequently associated, indicating that perceived
fit within the landscape and visual intrusion are topics
which need to be addressed. This is in line with other
acceptance studies on wind energy, some of which
have even found that perceived negative landscape
impact can “spill over” to evaluations of efficiency
(Waldo, 2012). Biomass was not well-known among
the participants, indicating a need for more informa-
tion on this energy source. This is supported by the
lack of a specific, symbolic infrastructure as part of
the mental model of biomass of laypersons (in con-
trast to, e.g., wind turbines for wind power or solar
panels for PV). It can be assumed that a widespread
diffusion of biomass plants will be difficult to achieve
on the basis of the current level of knowledge of the
public, as participants spontaneously had mostly neg-
ative associations concerning various aspects (eco-
logical, ethical and health concerns). PV, on the
other hand, seemed most agreeable, some participants
even demanded that it should be “obligatory for every
The Good, the Bad and the Ugly: Affect and its Role for Renewable Energy Acceptance
333
house owner”. The concerns regarding dependence
on weather conditions could be addressed by better
informing the public about storage possibilities. From
a social acceptance point of view, results indicate that
PV is likely to provoke the least opposition. Apart
from the implications of the findings on public infor-
mation strategies, a final note is directed at a metaper-
spective of the utility of the mental models prevailing
in the public. Naturally, mental models and affective
evaluations of energy sources of the uninformed pub-
lic might be “illogical” or even “incorrect” from ex-
perts’ points of view. Not considering these mental
models seems ill-advised from a social science point
of view. If experts take those affective evaluations as
a starting point to integrate the public, giving them the
chance to participate in an open and transparent dis-
course, this might help to overcome affective barriers
and to shape public knowledge systematically. In this
case, uncovering implicit mental models and address-
ing them deliberately in an open discourse between
experts, planners, and citizens, represents a kind of
discourse instrument and an educative tool to negoti-
ate accepted renewable and sustainable energy transi-
tion processes.
4.4 Limitations
Regarding the influence of the affect heuristic on ac-
ceptability of renewable energy sources, it should be
further analyzed how stable the mental models of the
participants are, for example by giving biased infor-
mation. In the case of biomass, for which a lack of
specific information was identified, the influence of
biased information could be different than for wind
power or PV. Additionally, the extent of the influ-
ence of affect on acceptance was not quantified in
this study. Comparative research on the three en-
ergy sources showed tendencies for affect having a
different influence on acceptance depending on the
energy source (Zoellner et al., 2008). Furthermore,
user factors and their influence on affect were not an-
alyzed in detail in this study, however, S
¨
utterlin and
Siegrist found that affective images for solar power
are influenced by gender, so it is likely that this is
also the case when comparing across different en-
ergy sources (S
¨
utterlin and Siegrist, 2017). They also
suggest a cultural influence on affect. Future studies
should therefore include more countries, concentrat-
ing on user diversity and the question which individ-
ual factors might impact affective responses towards
energy technologies (e.g., extent of risk tolerance, in-
terest in innovation, extent of ecological awareness).
In addition, out sample was quite young and –in com-
parison to the German population highly educated.
As education levels, social norms and cultural tradi-
tions also impact the environmental awareness and the
consciousness about societal welfare, a more detailed
look into user factors is needed (Kollmuss and Agye-
man, 2002). Finally, electricity production is only one
part of the grid which needs to be adapted to the en-
ergy transition. Storage facilities for electricity also
play an important role for securing sustainable elec-
tricity supply, also here, mental models can help to
uncover possible barriers to acceptance (Zaunbrecher
et al., 2016).
5 CONCLUSIONS
The study showed that emotional responses to renew-
able energies can, next to cognitive responses, ex-
plain attitudes and uncover hidden motivations. In-
vestigating those affective responses can thus help to
improve understanding for attitudes of laypersons to-
wards energy infrastructure. As research has shown
that an integrative approach which values public opin-
ions and participation can influence the success of
such projects, it is advisable to integrate public men-
tal models and perceptions into early phases of energy
infrastructure planning and communication strategies.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
Special thanks to Jonas Hemsen for research support.
This work was funded by the Excellence initiative of
the German federal and state governments and the
Strategy fund of RWTH Aachen University (Projects
UFO and KESS).
REFERENCES
Alhakami, A. S. and Slovic, P. (1994). A psychological
study of the inverse relationship between perceived
risk and perceived benefit. Risk Analysis, 14(6):1085–
1096.
Benitez, L. E., Benitez, P. C., and Van Kooten, G. C. (2008).
The economics of wind power with energy storage.
Energy Economics, 30(4):1973–1989.
Bergler, R. (1975). Das Eindrucksdifferential: Theorie und
Technik. Huber.
Chassot, S., Kl
¨
ockner, C. A., and W
¨
ustenhagen, R. (2015).
Can implicit cognition predict the behavior of profes-
sional energy investors? An explorative application of
the implicit association test (IAT). Journal of Applied
Research in Memory and Cognition, 4(3):285–293.
SMARTGREENS 2018 - 7th International Conference on Smart Cities and Green ICT Systems
334
Crichton, F., Dodd, G., Schmid, G., Gamble, G., and
Petrie, K. J. (2014). Can expectations produce symp-
toms from infrasound associated with wind turbines?
Health Psychology, 33(4):360.
Dunlap, R. E., Van Liere, K. D., Mertig, A. G., and Jones,
R. E. (2000). New trends in measuring environmental
attitudes: Measuring endorsement of the new ecolog-
ical paradigm: a revised NEP scale. Journal of Social
Issues, 56(3):425–442.
D
¨
utschke, E., Schneider, U., and Wesche, J. (2017).
Knowledge, use and effectiveness of social accep-
tance measures for wind projects. Zeitschrift f
¨
ur En-
ergiewirtschaft, 41(4):299–310.
Elliott, P. and Wadley, D. (2002). The impact of transmis-
sion lines on property values: Coming to terms with
stigma. Property Management, 20(2):137–152.
Finucane, M. L., Alhakami, A., Slovic, P., and Johnson,
S. M. (2000). The affect heuristic in judgments of
risks and benefits. Journal of Behavioral Decision
Making, 13(1):1–17.
Frewer, L. J., Miles, S., Brennan, M., Kuznesof, S., Ness,
M., and Ritson, C. (2002). Public preferences for in-
formed choice under conditions of risk uncertainty.
Public Understanding of Science, 11(4):363–372.
Gross, C. (2007). Community perspectives of wind energy
in australia: The application of a justice and commu-
nity fairness framework to increase social acceptance.
Energy Policy, 35(5):2727–2736.
Himmel, S., Zaunbrecher, B. S., Wilkowska, W., and
Ziefle, M. (2014). The youth of today designing
the smart city of tomorrow. In International Con-
ference on Human-Computer Interaction, pages 389–
400. Springer.
Huijts, N. M., Molin, E. J., and Steg, L. (2012). Psycholog-
ical factors influencing sustainable energy technology
acceptance: A review-based comprehensive frame-
work. Renewable and Sustainable Energy Reviews,
16(1):525–531.
Joffe, H. (2003). Risk: From perception to social rep-
resentation. British Journal of Social Psychology,
42(1):55–73.
Johansson, M. and Laike, T. (2007). Intention to respond
to local wind turbines: The role of attitudes and visual
perception. Wind Energy, 10(5):435–451.
Keller, C., Visschers, V., and Siegrist, M. (2012). Affective
imagery and acceptance of replacing nuclear power
plants. Risk Analysis, 32(3):464–477.
Klaiß, H., K
¨
ohne, R., Nitsch, J., and Sprengel, U. (1995).
Solar thermal power plants for solar countriestechnol-
ogy, economics and market potential. Applied Energy,
52(2):165–183.
Kollmuss, A. and Agyeman, J. (2002). Mind the gap: why
do people act environmentally and what are the bar-
riers to pro-environmental behavior? Environmental
Education Research, 8(3):239–260.
Kortsch, T., Hildebrand, J., and Schweizer-Ries, P. (2015).
Acceptance of biomass plants–results of a longitudi-
nal study in the bioenergy-region Altmark. Renewable
Energy, 83:690–697.
Lee, R. P. (2015). Stability of energy imageries and af-
fect following shocks to the global energy system:
The case of Fukushima. Journal of Risk Research,
18(7):965–988.
Leiserowitz, A. (2006). Climate change risk perception and
policy preferences: The role of affect, imagery, and
values. Climatic Change, 77(1):45–72.
Osgood, C. E. (1952). The nature and measurement of
meaning. Psychological Bulletin, 49(3):197.
Renn, O. (1982). Nuclear energy and the public: Risk per-
ception, attitudes and behaviour. In Uranium and Nu-
clear Energy: 1981. Proceedings of the Sixth Interna-
tional symposium held by the Uranium Institute, Lon-
don, 2-4 September 1981.
R
¨
osch, C. and Kaltschmitt, M. (1999). Energy from
biomass Do non-technical barriers prevent an in-
creased use? Biomass and Bioenergy, 16(5):347–356.
Siegrist, M. (1996). Questionnaire of ecocentric and
anthropocentric attitudes towards the environment.
Zeitschrift f
¨
ur Sozialpsychologie, 27(4):290–294.
Sims, R. E., Rogner, H.-H., and Gregory, K. (2003). Carbon
emission and mitigation cost comparisons between
fossil fuel, nuclear and renewable energy resources for
electricity generation. Energy Policy, 31(13):1315–
1326.
Slovic, P., Finucane, M. L., Peters, E., and MacGregor,
D. G. (2007). The affect heuristic. European Jour-
nal of Operational Research, 177(3):1333–1352.
Slovic, P., Peters, E., Finucane, M. L., Hawaii, K. P., and
Macgregor, D. G. (2005). Affect, risk, and decision
making. Health Psychology, pages 35–40.
S
¨
utterlin, B. and Siegrist, M. (2017). Public acceptance of
renewable energy technologies from an abstract ver-
sus concrete perspective and the positive imagery of
solar power. Energy Policy, 106:356–366.
Truelove, H. B., Greenberg, M. R., and Powers, C. W.
(2014). Are implicit associations with nuclear energy
related to policy support? Evidence from the brief
implicit association test. Environment and Behavior,
46(7):898–923.
Upreti, B. R. and van der Horst, D. (2004). National re-
newable energy policy and local opposition in the UK:
The failed development of a biomass electricity plant.
Biomass and Bioenergy, 26(1):61–69.
Waldo,
˚
A. (2012). Offshore wind power in Sweden - A
qualitative analysis of attitudes with particular focus
on opponents. Energy Policy, 41:692–702.
Warren, C. R., Lumsden, C., O’Dowd, S., and Birnie, R. V.
(2005). Green on Green: Public perceptions of wind
power in Scotland and Ireland. Journal of Environ-
mental Planning and Management, 48(6):853–875.
Zaunbrecher, B. S., Bexten, T., Wirsum, M., and Ziefle,
M. (2016). What is stored, why, and how? Mental
models, knowledge, and public acceptance of hydro-
gen storage. Energy Procedia, 99:108–119.
Zoellner, J., Schweizer-Ries, P., and Wemheuer, C. (2008).
Public acceptance of renewable energies: Results
from case studies in Germany. Energy Policy,
36(11):4136–4141.
The Good, the Bad and the Ugly: Affect and its Role for Renewable Energy Acceptance
335
APPENDIX
Table 3: Demographics and user characteristics of acceptance groups (WP: wind power plant, BM=biomass plant), *sign.
differences (p 0.05)
Biomass-acceptance groups
HA group (n=73) LA group (n=236)
Gender* female: 52.4% male: 47.6% female: 68.5% male: 30.1%
Age M=33.3 (SD=15.5) M= 35.2 (SD=17.1)
University degree 59.70% 47.90%
Place of residence City center: 57.2%,
Suburbs: 28.8%
Village: 12.3%
City center: 57.5%,
Suburbs: 30.1%
Village: 12.3%
Infrastructure within view WP: 8.9%,
BM: 2.1%,
PV: 19.9%
WP: 5.5%,
BM: 0%,
PV: 27.4%
Environmental attitude M=4.9 (SD=0.7) M=4.8 (SD=0.6)
Attitude towards technology* M=4.8 (SD=0.8) M=4.5 (SD=0.9)
Knowledge biomass* M=3.0 (SD=1.2) M=2.5 (SD=1.3)
Windpower-acceptance groups
HA group (n=275) LA group (n=41)
Gender female: 56.0% male: 42.9% female: 61.0% male: 36.6%
Age* M=32.7 (SD=15.1) M=40.2 (SD=16.8)
University degree* 60.70% 39.00%
Place of residence City center: 58.9%,
Suburbs: 28.4%
Village: 11.3%
City center: 53.7%,
Suburbs: 29.3%
Village: 17.1%
Infrastructure within view WP: 8.7%,
BM: 1.5%,
PV*: 18.9%
WP: 4.9%,
BM: 2.4%,
PV*: 34.1%
Environmental attitude* M=4.9 (SD=0.7) M=4.5 (SD=0.8)
Attitude towards technology M=4.7 (SD=0.9) M=4.7 (SD=0.9)
Knowledge wind power M=3.9 (SD=1.1) M=3.7 (SD=1.2)
PV-acceptance groups
HA group (n=299) LA group (n=16)
Gender* female: 54.8% male: 43.8% female: 81.3% male: 18.8%
Age M=33.9 (SD=15.8) M=31.5 (SD=15.7)
University degree 60.20% 25.00%
Place of residence City center: 57.5%,
Suburbs: 28.8%
Village: 12.4%
City center: 62.5%,
Suburbs: 31.3%
Village: 6.3%
Infrastructure within view WP: 8.4%,
BM: 1.3%,
PV: 21.49%
WP: 6.3%,
BM: 6.3%,
PV: 12.5%
Environmental attitude M=4.8 (SD=0.7) M=4.9 (SD=0.7)
Attitude towards technology* M=4.8 (SD=0.7) M=3.9 (SD=1.0)
Knowledge PV* M=3.5 (SD=1.3) M=2.2 (SD=1.5)
SMARTGREENS 2018 - 7th International Conference on Smart Cities and Green ICT Systems
336