Based on Table 4.3, it is known that hypothesis 1
is supported because the test results generate a p-
value of 0.001 (≤ 0.05). The average subject that
receives information with ++-- order experiences an
increase in conviction level regarding the budget
raise higher than the subjects receiving --++
information order. That result shows that the recency
effect is happening because the last conviction level
is more affected by the last information received.
Therefore, H1 proposed in this research is supported
by data.
Hypothesis 2: A subject that receives information
with the inappropriate frame (information with
positive-negative order is framed positively, and
information with positive-negative order is framed
negatively) will improve their belief level of budget
raised higher than the subject that receives
information with the appropriate frame (information
with positive-negative order is framed negatively,
and information with negative-positive order is
framed positively). Theoretically, group 1 and group
4 responses (the subjects that received information
with an inappropriate frame) in doing belief level
revision regarding the budget raised will be higher
than group 2 and group 3 responses (the subjects that
received information with an appropriate frame).
Based on Table 4.3, it is known that hypothesis 2
is accepted because the mean response of group 1
was 55.9259, significantly higher than group 3,
whose mean response was 54.8148 (p = 0.036). It is
known that group 1 comprises subjects that received
++-- information with positive framing, while group
3 comprises subjects that received ++-- with
negative framing. This means that the subject that
receives information with an inappropriate frame
(positive negative information order is framed
positively) will increase their belief level regarding
budget raised higher than subjects that receive
information with an appropriate frame (positive
negative information order is framed negatively).
The mean response of group 2 was 44.0741,
significantly higher than group 4, whose mean
response was 47.7778 (p = 0.039). It is known that
group 2 comprises subjects that received --++
information order with positive framing, while group
4 comprised subjects that received --++ an
information order with negative framing. This means
that subjects that receive information with an
inappropriate frame (negative positive information is
framed negatively) will increase their belief level
regarding budget raised higher than the subjects
receiving information with an appropriate frame
(negative-positive information order is framed
positively). This shows that information framing can
affect the recency effect that may show up due to
obtaining diverse information. (The subject’s last
belief level is more affected by the last information
obtained.) Hence, the proposed H2 in this research is
supported by data.
5 CONCLUSIONS
Based on the result and analysis, we conclude that
the existence of the recency effect is real. The
attention of the subject when the information is
provided in Step-by-Step mode is more weighed on
the last information than the first information. The
recency effect causes bias in budget decisions.
However, this bias can be mitigated by framing the
information with an inappropriate manner. If the
order of information is ++--, then the framing of
information should be positive (appropriate frame).
If the order of information is --++, then the framing
of information should be negative (inappropriate
frame).
The next research can use group participants, not
only individuals, because in reality the budgeting
process in a firm is a participative process that
combines several related opinions. The next research
may also include variables in the form of anchor
strength to further validate the use of framing in
mitigating the recency effects, both in low or high
anchor condition.
REFERENCES
Alvia, L., & Sulistiawan, D. (2012). The Impact of
Cognitive Style to Recency Effect in Stock
Investment: An Experimental Study. Available at
SSRN 2201544.
Asare, S. K., & Messier Jr, W. F. (1991). A review of
audit research using the belief-adjustment model
Auditing (pp. 75-92): Springer.
Ashton, A. H., & Ashton, R. H. (1988). Sequential belief
revision in auditing. Accounting Review, 623-641.
Bamber, E. M., Ramsay, R. J., & Tubbs, R. M. (1997). An
examination of the descriptive validity of the belief-
adjustment model and alternative attitudes to evidence
in auditing. Accounting, Organizations and Society,
22(3), 249-268.
Biyanto, F. (2001). Hubungan pembingkaian informasi
anggaran, tanggung jawab, dan pengalaman terhadap
pilihan keputusan pada investasi berisiko. Universitas
Gadjah Mada.
Brewer, P. C. (2008). Redefining management accounting:
Promoting the four pillars of our profession. Strategic
finance, 27-35.