Ethnic Identity and Ethnic Prejudice in Children:
A Comparative Study of the Three Cities in Indonesia
Dyan Evita Santi
1
, Suryanto
2
and M. G. Bagus Ani Putra
2
1
Faculty of Psychology, University of 17 August 1945 Surabaya
2,3
Faculty of Psychology, Universitas Airlangga
Keywords: Ethnic prejudice, Ethnic identity, Children.
Abstract: This study aims to see the relationship between ethnic identity and children’s ethnic prejudice against
Chinese in 3 cities Bangkalan, Yogyakarta and Medan. The population in this study comprised children
aged 11-14 years. The data was collected in Islamic schools and public schools from a total of 173 children
in Bangkalan, 132 in Jogjakarta and 148 in Medan. The results concluded that Bangkalan and Jogjakarta
showed similar results in that there was a negative relationship between ethnic identity and a child’s ethnic
prejudice. This means that the higher the ethnic identity associated with the child's understanding of who
he/she is, the stronger the emotional ties and beliefs, and the stronger the commitment to the group, the
higher the child's ethnic prejudice and vice versa. While Medan showed different results in that there is no
relationship between ethnic identity and a child’s ethnic prejudice against Chinese.
1 INTRODUCTION
Almost all societies today are multicultural.
Indonesia as the one of the multicultural countries
divided into two major groups namely indigenous
ethnic groups such as Javanese, Sundanese, Batak,
Minang and immigrant ethnic groups such as Indian,
Arabic, European (representing Portuguese and
Dutch) as well as ethnic Chinese. Especially, the
Chinese, after the independence of Indonesian, are
classified as one of the tribes within the national
scope of Indonesia, in accordance with Article 2 of
Law No. 12/2006 regarding the Citizenship of the
Republic of Indonesia. According to Lubis (1995),
Indonesia as a multicultural country, could
potentially lead to disharmony and inter-ethnic
conflict. This will lead to disintegration and conflict
latency, so it could threaten the unity of the nation.
One of the most frequent ethnic conflicts in
Indonesia over the years has particularly occurred
with the Chinese in relation to indigenous and non-
indigenous dictatorships. In the Dutch colonial
period, the indigenous was used as the Malay term
for Inlanders, one of the Dutch Indies population
groups originating from indigenous tribes of the
archipelago. Therefore, Indonesians of Chinese,
Indian, Arab, European or mixed descent are often
classified as non-indigenous even though several
generations have been born in Indonesia. However,
the grouping in practice was often racist, because
there was discrimination. Therefore, when Indonesia
was led by President BJ Habibie, Presidential
Instruction No. 26/1998 was issued which abolished
the use of the term indigenous and non-indigenous.
In the era of President Susilo Bambang Yudhoyono
Presidential Decree no. 12/2014 was issued on the
change of the Chinese term to Tionghoa. It was
because the Chinese term was considered to have a
psychosocial-discriminatory effect on the social
relations of Chinese descent. President Yudhoyono
assesses that discriminatory views and treatment of a
particular person, group, community and/or race, in
principle violate the values and principles of human
rights protection.
One of the problems in a multicultural life was
prejudice. Prejudice can be a time bomb that can
erupt at any time, if the diversity was not understood
wisely. Psychological studies have also revealed that
differences in ethnic, religious, political and
socioeconomic status can lead to prejudice.
Prejudice is actually something natural in human
life. Allport (1954) argued that prejudice is
unavoidable in human life. Specifically, this study
focuses on children's ethnic prejudice. For decades
researchers have tried to understand children's
Santi, D., Suryanto, . and Putra, M.
Ethnic Identity and Ethnic Prejudice in Children: A Comparative Study of the Three Cities in Indonesia.
DOI: 10.5220/0008587302070215
In Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on Psychology in Health, Educational, Social, and Organizational Settings (ICP-HESOS 2018) - Improving Mental Health and Harmony in
Global Community, pages 207-215
ISBN: 978-989-758-435-0
Copyright
c
2020 by SCITEPRESS Science and Technology Publications, Lda. All rights reserved
207
prejudice. Some researchers assume that prejudice
that children showed was not a representation of the
actual attitudes that they have, but only as a
reflection of the attitudes they imitate from others
(Aboud, 2000). So it is assumed that there is no real
prejudice in children. Yet others point to the
contradictory notion that the real prejudice has
occurred in children.
In children at primary school, their
understanding of racial stereotypes and prejudice
begins to increase; at the age of 10 to 11 years, their
awareness of racial stereotypes is widespread
(McKown & Weinstein, in Kail, 2010). But at this
age the overt prejudice generally begins to decrease.
This is because they begin to learn about the norm of
not being too open in showing favoritism towards
the ingroup beyond any other groups (Rutland, Kail,
2010). However, some studies show that the
decrease of overt prejudice does not mean that the
implicit prejudice also declines (Kail, 2010). This
assumed support by research conducted by Santi
(2014) with the subject of Javanese ethnic students
aged between 12-14 years. The results of this study
indicate that 77% of Javanese children in this study
have prejudice against Chinese, who are moderate to
high and only about 23% are low. It can be
concluded that prejudice has developed so early.
According to Bigler and Liben (in Kail, 2010)
prejudice develops as a part of a child's efforts in
understanding his/her social world, where this
understanding leads them to classify individuals
based on visible prominent aspects (such as race,
gender, age) as well as labels shown by adults
against particular groups.
To understand the factors that cause prejudice
shown by children was very important because there
was anguish that once children or adolescents have
shown prejudice against members of a particular
group, they keep hold of this attitude to all members
of the group. So, these attitudes will carry over to
adulthood even down to the next generations. In
addition, understanding child prejudice is important
because prejudice in adults is very difficult to
change, so that intervention will be more effective at
the beginning (Aboud and Levy, 2000).
Ethnic prejudice is essentially complex attitudes
that must be comprehensively understood.
Motivational aspects view prejudice as a result of
the development of moral reasoning (Killen, 2007),
ingroup identification (Rutland, Killen and Abrams,
2010), as well as threats to one's social identity
(Nesdale, et al., 2005; in Costello and Hodson,
2012). Based on this motivational process, a factor
that assumed the emergence of ethnic prejudice in
childhood was the ethnic identity of a child. In life,
individuals will always identify and define
themselves based on their social group (Turner,
1991, Hogg, 1996 in Myers, 1999) resulting in an
ethnic identity. Ethnic identity is the individual's
understanding of who he/she is, the attachment
between the emotional individual and group, the
confidence in the group, and the strong commitment
to the group and together performing the same
customs (Isajiw, 1999). Ethnic identity has proven to
be an important aspect of adolescent development,
such as dealing with psychological wellbeing
(Umaña-Taylor, Diversi and Fine, 2002), academic
achievement (Smith, et al., 1999), prejudice and
ability to overcome discrimination and racism
(Dubow, et al., 2000).
In this study researchers associate ethnic
prejudice with ethnic identity. Because ethnic
identity is an important part of one's social identity
that will affect how one wants to be known in their
social life. So it is very interesting to be able to
understand how ethnic identity affects ethnic
prejudice especially in children of majority ethnics.
Some studies assumed that ethnic identity has a
positive relationship with ethnic prejudice. This
means that the stronger the ethnic identity, the
higher the ethnic prejudice and vice versa. Ethnic
identity was the individual's understanding of who
he/she is, the emotional attachment between
individuals with their groups, the confidence in the
group, and the strong commitment to the group and
together performing the same customs. In some
articles it is stated that ethnic identity was defined as
an ethnic component in social identity. As defined
by Tajfel (1986) it was a part of individual self-
concept, derived from knowledge of membership in
a certain social group in line with values and
emotional aspects that are significantly attached to
the membership. Ethnic identity can be defined as a
subjective feeling of membership in ethnic groups
that involves self-labeling, belonging, group
preference, positive evaluation of ethnic groups,
ethnic knowledge, and involvement in ethnic group
activities (Phinney, 1990, 1996 in Cokley, 2007).
For decades, social psychologists have tried to
understand how identification of ingroups influenced
both ingroup and outgroup attitudes. This
assumption arises from the perspective of social
identity (Tajfel and Turner, 1979) in which
individuals who strongly identify themselves with
ingroups will tend to show a preference for their
ingroup rather than outgroups, compared to
individuals whose self-identification of ingroup
tends to be weak. This is because ingroups are
ICP-HESOS 2018 - International Conference on Psychology in Health, Educational, Social, and Organizational Settings
208
psychologically central to self-concept (Brown,
2000 in Yogeeswaran et al., 2014). In the context of
national identity, Pehrson et al. in Yogeeswaran,
(2014) found that individuals with a national identity
tend to have greater prejudice against outgroups. But
this depends on how they understand the definition
of the nation (Meeus, et al., 2010 in Yogeeswaran et
al., 2014). In particular, using two approaches that
are cross-sectional and longitudinal, Pehrson et al.
(2009) found that individuals who strongly
identified themselves with their nations expressed
greater prejudice against immigrants and asylum
seekers only if they defined their country in ethnic
terms, but not if they define a nation in the context
of citizenship. In this regard, some researchers argue
that there are two factors that tend to influence the
attitudes of members of the majority to ethnic
groups in the context of pluralistic countries: (a)
how ethnic group members openly express their
cultural identity and (b) how strongly an individual
identifies with their citizenship. These findings
suggest that even if one already has ethnic diversity
principles, expressing ethnic identity especially in
the public sphere can lead to rejection, because it is
assumed to threaten the status quo and the
peculiarities of being American.
On the other side ethnic identity conceptually
can be associated with some psychological aspects
such as psychological wellbeing, either directly or
indirectly related. But empirical evidence for a direct
relationship between ethnic identity and
psychological function is often less than convincing.
Cross (1991) summarizes 45 studies from 1961 to
1984 that examined directly the relationship between
reference group orientation, such as ethnic identity
and personal identity indicators including self-
esteem, anxiety, introversions, and depression. Only
a few studies have reported a very significant
relationship between ethnic identity and self-esteem
(e.g. Stephen and Rosenfield, 1979 in Selllers,
2006), while other studies have reported no
significant relationship between ethnic identity and
self-esteem (e.g. Rosenberg, 1979 in Sellers, 2006).
But some psychological literature confirmed that
ethnic identity has a positive personal disposition.
Identification with larger groups can also provide a
sense of belonging and social support. Thus, ethnic
identity can provide a sense of strength, competence
and self-acceptance to facing a complex social
situation (Outten, et al., 2009; Ruiz, 1990 in Smith
and Sylva, 2011). By definition, high levels of ethnic
identity are generally positively associated with
personal wellbeing. A number of studies have found
a positive relationship between a strong ethnic
identity with indicators in self-esteem and personal
adjustment. For example, a large study (N = 5,423)
in adolescents showed a significant positive
correlation between ethnic identity and various
positive attributes such as problem-solving skills,
mastery of a thing, self-esteem and optimism
(Roberts, et al., 1999, Sylva, 2011). In general, the
results consistently showed a positive relationship
between ethnic identity and various aspects of
personal welfare among ethnic groups (Phinney,
Cantu and Kurtz, 1997 in Smith and Sylva, 2011).
This study used subjects from the dominant
ethnic groups in their respective regions. Based on
the theory of social dominance proposed by
Sidanius, that the stronger the identity of an
individual as a member of a majority ethnic group,
the higher the prejudice to the members of other
ethnic groups. This is supported by studies by Levin,
Sidanius, Rabinowitz and Federico (Wittig, et al.,
2007), which conclude that there was a strong
positive relationship between the strength of one's
ethnic identity and the prejudice of the majority
ethnic group. While in minority ethnic groups,
showing no relation or having a negative
relationship. It can be concluded that the relationship
between ethnic identity and ethnic prejudice shows
varied results; therefore the researcher is interested
in examining the relationship between the two
variables, especially in children of the majority
ethnic group in Bangkalan, Jogjakarta and Medan.
The selection of these three cities was based on the
assumption that they have an interesting relationship
pattern between local ethnic population and Chinese.
Bangkalan was chosen because researchers assumed
that in this region the majority held Islamic customs
and traditions, which were strong enough so that
they were very different from Chinese customs and
traditions. Yogyakarta was chosen because this city
still applied rules that seem discriminate against
Chinese, especially regarding ownership of land
rights. Meanwhile Medan was chosen because it was
considered a multicultural city, which became a
barometer of the existence of inter-ethnic harmony,
so this city had a high acceptance of the existence of
Chinese.
1.1 Hypothesis
Children’s ethnic identity will affect ethnic prejudice
against Chinese in Bangkalan, Jogjakarta and
Medan.
Ethnic Identity and Ethnic Prejudice in Children: A Comparative Study of the Three Cities in Indonesia
209
2 METHODS
The study used a quantitative method with bivariate
Spearman correlation analysis using SPSS version
22.
2.1 Operational Definition
The children’s ethnic prejudice against Chinese is
the tendency of the child to respond with a
disrespectful attitude, having negative evaluation
and emotions towards Chinese.
Ethnic identity is the individual's understanding
of who he/she is, the emotional attachment between
individuals and their groups, the beliefs of being in a
group, and a strong commitment to the group and
together following the same customs.
2.2 Sample
The population in this study was children aged 11-
14 years who live in Bangkalan, Jogjakarta and
Medan. Collecting data was conducted in schools.
The criteria of the school were public schools and
Islamic schools that assumed there weren’t Chinese
students. In total 173 children in Bangkalan, 132 in
Jogjakarta and 148 in Medan.
2.3 Instruments
Ethnic prejudice. The ethnic prejudice scale was
developed from the Pettigrew and Marteens scale
(1995). The original version of this scale has two
dimensions: blatant prejudice and subtle prejudice.
Referring to the adaptation of this scale in some
countries, the authors assumed that Pettigrew and
Meertens’ scale (1995) was unidimensional, so it has
blatant and subtle dimensions. So, in this study
ethnic prejudice was measured using 6 indicators:
perceived economic threat, perceived physical
threat, avoidance of close contact, perceived
problems in adaptation, exaggeration of cultural
differences and the denial of positive emotion. The
scale is a 28-item Likert scale with items answered
on a four-point scale, from strongly agree to strongly
disagree. This scale has a composite reliability =
0.957; CR ≥ 0.70.
Ethnic identity. The scale of ethnic identity was
developed based on the identity theory of Stryker
(1987 in Scottham, Sellers and Nguyen, 2008). This
ethnic identity scale was originally developed by
Sellers et al. (1998 in Byrd, 2011) called
Multidimensional Inventory of Black Identity
(MIBI). This scale has three dimensions of ethnic
identity: Centrality, Regard, and Ideology. The scale
is a 20-item Likert scale with items answered on a
four-point scale from strongly agree to strongly
disagree. This scale has a composite reliability =
0.933; CR ≥ 0.70.
3 RESULTS
The results of the analysis are presented in Table 1
presenting correlation between ethnic identity and
ethnic prejudice. Bangkalan showed a negative
relation between ethnic identity and ethnic prejudice
with r = -.172 (p = .023; p < .05); Yogjakarta
showed similar results with r = -.26 (p = .002; p
<.05). While at Medan the correlation was r2 = -.094
(p = .258; p> .05), because the analysis indicated
was not significant so it can be concluded that in
Medan there is no relationship between ethnic
identity and a child’s ethnic prejudice. In addition to
the results above, this study also compared the
strength of the correlation coefficient between the
samples in Bangkalan and Yogyakarta using
Fisher’s R to Z transformation. Correlation
coefficient in Bangkalan Z = -0.17 was stronger than
Yogyakarta Z = -0.27.
4 DISCUSSION
It is assumed that social conflicts (ethnic, racial and
religious) in Indonesia often involve Chinese as one
of the minority ethnic groups. In this case, Chinese
are often used as scapegoats for the bad situations in
Indonesia. The relationship between Chinese and
majority ethnics in Indonesia seems like the embers
Table 1: The correlation and descriptive data of the variables
City Bivariate Co
r
relation
N
Ethnic Identit
y
Ethnic Pre
j
udice
Mean SD Mean SD
Ban
g
kalan r = -.172 (p = .023 ; p < .05) 173 65.433 8.586 60.659 6.544
Jo
gj
akarta r = -.266 (p = .002 ; p < .05) 132 65.510 6.358 61.690 9.639
Medan r = -.094 (p = .258 ; p > .05) 148 62.040 10.360 69.385 7.620
ICP-HESOS 2018 - International Conference on Psychology in Health, Educational, Social, and Organizational Settings
210
in the chaff even since in the Dutch colonial era. So,
it is very interesting to study ethnic prejudice of
children who come from the ethnic majority to
Chinese in the present time. This study was to
explore the relationship between ethnic identity with
children’s ethnic prejudices against Chinese. First, it
must be stressed that the data presented is
correlational and consequently cannot address the
question of causality.
In line with the concepts, the results of this study
also concluded a similar view. More specifically,
this study examined the relationship between ethnic
identity with children’s ethnic prejudice in
Bangkalan, Yogyakarta and Medan. The results
show slightly different trends but generally show the
same results. In Bangkalan and Yogyakarta they
showed a significant negative relationship between
ethnic identity and ethnic prejudice, while Medan
showed a negative relationship but it was not
significant. In general, this study concluded that
there is a negative relationship between ethnic
identity and ethnic prejudice in children. This means
that the stronger the ethnic identity of a child, the
lower their ethnic prejudice and vice versa. In this
context it can be assumed that a person's ethnic
identity has a major influence on how they perceive
others, their self-esteem, aspirations, motivation and
effort expended in various aspects of their lives
(Smith, et al., 1999 in Salmeri and Pellerone, 2015).
In this case a child with a high ethnic identity will be
able to assess the issues related to ethnicity more
positively as well. Ethnicity in this case is a series of
dynamics derived from history, institutionalized
ideas and practices that: (1) enable people to identify
or be identified as a group of people who are
assumed to have similarities in language, history,
nation or region of origin, customs, life, religion,
name, physical appearance, and/or genealogy or
ancestry; (2) can be a source of meaning, action and
identity; and (3) a conferred sense of belonging,
pride and motivation (Markus, 2008). So those who
have a high ethnic identity will be able to understand
the concept of ethnicity, including the ethnicity of
other groups. Thus, they are able to demonstrate a
positive attitude and appreciate the presence of other
ethnics around them, to minimize the attitudes of
ethnic prejudice. As explained earlier, ethnic identity
can affect both positively and negatively and
prejudice depends on several factors. Some of these
factors are (a) the strength level of ingroup identity,
(b) the threat that outgroups have ingroup identity
(called outgroup threats), and (c) social norms in
ingroups (Killen & Rutland, 2011, Nesdale, Durkin
et al., 2005). The strength of ingroup identity is
important because not all individuals consider
membership of a group as important; therefore, the
importance of identity for a person varies according
to the context (Bennett, 2004, Bennett and Sani,
2008, Steele, Spencer and Aronson, 2002 in Killen
et al., 2015).
In addition, developmental researchers
concluded that the bias against the group was due to
the type of norms applied. If the norm in the group
has a morally wrong perspective, as the group acts in
an unfair manner, then favoritism towards ingroups
will not appear (Killen, et al., 2001 in Killen et al.,
2015). In the literature of children and adults, it also
shows that to inhibit the occurrence of group bias
was often associated with an assessment of group
goals and norms that apply in groups (Abrams, et al.,
2008, Devine, et al., 2002 in Killen et al., 2015). As
another example, if the group norm reflects the
attitude of prejudice, then the group members who
reject this norm may be considered disloyal to the
group but are faithful to the norm of justice or
equality. The study of social and developmental
perspectives based on social identity theory
suggests, when studying the development of
intergroup attitudes, it is important to measure the
meaning of identity, the power of outside threats and
the type of norm (moral, social, psychological)
identified to the group in order to understand the
emergence of prejudice.
So, based on that theory it may be the subject of
this study, especially in Bangkalan and Yogyakarta
where they feel that as the majority ethnicity they do
not feel threatened by the presence of other ethnics,
feeling quite confident that they will able to deal
with any social situation without feeling anxious
with the presence of other groups. So, they do not
need to show negative attitudes toward other ethnic
groups. In addition, the subject of this study may
have taught about social norms that refer to moral
values in general such as mutual respect and fairness
to everyone. This is in line with the opinion of
Crocker and Luhtanen (1990 in Masson and
Verkuyten, 1993), that prejudice will depend on
group norms, so they are not related to how they
connect with ethnic minorities but connect with
traditions that include prejudice values so that leads
to the formation of prejudice. So, the implications of
this research are very important to develop strong
ethnic identity in children through the indicators of
ethnic identity used in this research - centrality,
regard and ideology. Therefore, it is important for
relevant parties such as in family and school
environments as a child’s microsystem to develop a
positive ethnic identity. Because if schools do not
Ethnic Identity and Ethnic Prejudice in Children: A Comparative Study of the Three Cities in Indonesia
211
consistently facilitate the development of a positive
identity for all their students, including ethnic/racial
identity, then there will be negative consequences
(Pellerone, 2012, in Salmeri and Pellerone, 2015).
Researchers chose Yogyakarta because this city
still applies rules that tend to be discriminatory
about the status of land ownership of Chinese
citizens in Yogyakarta. On this rule the Governor of
Yogyakarta, Sri Sultan Hamengkubawono XI was
once sued in court by one of the ethnic Chinese
descendants. In 1975, Paku Alam VIII issued a letter
of instruction to the Regents and Mayors not to
deliver land titles to non-indigenous citizens. The
instruction letter permits the citizens of offspring to
own the land with the right to use the building status
(HGB), not the property rights (SHM). If the land
was previously owned by the indigenous people then
it was bought by the descendants, then the land
ownership status will be transferred to the state. In
the process, a person who has "a face of a
descendant" must give birth certificates, marriage
certificates and parents' birth certificates to obtain
land rights to prove they are not Chinese citizens.
The existence of rules that seem discriminative will
be vulnerable to trigger of inter-ethnic conflict.
Therefore, the researcher assumed that in
Yogyakarta acceptance of Chinese is still not
complete, in turn this will have an impact on the
development of prejudice against Chinese.
Social psychology theories suggest that
discrimination is one of the effects of prejudice.
Even other theories reveal that discrimination is a
form of prejudice. The existence of rules that are
considered to be discriminatory for a particular
group is the result of prejudice. As previously stated,
the regulation that prohibits Chinese from having
rights to land in Yogyakarta stems from an attempt
to equalize indigenous rights (the term at the time)
so as not to be dominated by Chinese who were then
considered powerful investors who could master the
economy of Yogyakarta. The assumption that
Chinese are economic ruling groups should be
limited, teaching the next generations that the group
could be a source of threat to the survival of
indigenous communities. This feeling of being
threatened will keep one's distance from the sources
that threaten it. The theory of prejudice suggests that
most threatened groups are regarded as the most
prejudiced group. However, the results of a study
showing a negative relationship between ethnic
identity and child ethnic prejudices in Yogyakarta
showed that although originating from the ethnic
majority this does not mean the children show their
prejudice to Chinese. Perhaps these discriminatory
implications are regarded as the "legacy" that must
be followed without need to understand in depth the
history behind the rules. It is also possible that
children in Yogyakarta are not aware of the rules, so
it can’t affect their attitudes towards Chinese.
Meanwhile, a different situation occurred in
Bangkalan. At first the researchers assumed that
Madura is an island dominated by the Madurese, so
there are not many Chinese living on that island. But
it turns out that Chinese also live on this island but
most of them live in Sumenep. While in Bangkalan
there are also areas that include Chinese. The
Chinese in Bangkalan live in a village called
"Kampung Pecinan Demangan" because in that
village the majority are Chinese (Adomatussa and
Warsono, 2017). The Chinese and Madurese in this
area have a significant difference in terms of
religion, culture, economy and lifestyle. However,
there conflict between the two ethnic groups in the
area was rare. However, the concentration of
Chinese in one area illustrates the separation of
territories inhabited by the Chinese in Bangkalan.
That condition may have occurred since the
Dutch era, where the separation between Chinese
and local people is a strategy by the Dutch to create
a boundary between the two ethnic groups.
Unfortunately, this situation continues to the next
generations. This situation is clearly different from
that in Surabaya (as a city near to Bangkalan), where
Chinese are not concentrated in one area but have
spread with local communities in almost all areas of
Surabaya. This can continue because Chinese may
feel that they have a feeling of the concept of
environmental psychology called attachment to the
place, which is a condition where one feels
attachment to the place of residence so as to feel
reluctant to move elsewhere. The concentration of
Chinese ultimately made the confounding process
only happen in the region, so people in other areas
rarely or even never mixed with Chinese. The
Madurese are known to have strong and familiar
characters. The Madurese have a very distinctive
and special culture that makes them unlike other
ethnic communities in terms of language,
submission, courage and submission to the four
main figures in life. The four figures are Buppa',
Babbu', Guru, Rato tires (father, mother, teacher and
leader in government). Madura is also a largely
inhabited island (approximately 97-99%) Muslim.
So that Madura appeared characteristic of Islam,
especially in the actualization of obedience to the
normative teachings of their religion (Adomatussa
and Warsono, 2017). The existence of strong
adherence to the norms and rules that apply to affect
ICP-HESOS 2018 - International Conference on Psychology in Health, Educational, Social, and Organizational Settings
212
patterns of relations developed Madurese especially
in this case with Chinese. So even though they have
a strong ethnic identity, the attitudes of prejudice
shown tend to be low. Along with this research
results from Adomatussa and Warsono (2017) show
that ethnic Madurese openness to Chinese can be
seen at the time of religious events, marriages,
participation during disasters, congratulation at the
holiday and participating in some activity, freedom
of worship, tadarrus activity during the month of
ramadhan and during the event of the Prophet's
mawlid. So, in the city conflict is relatively rare
between the two.
Another result of this study concludes that there
is no relationship between ethnic identity and
children’s ethnic prejudice in Medan. Medan is one
of the multicultural cities with various ethnic and
racial groups (Tionghoa and India). In general, the
subjects in this study come from various ethnic
groups namely Malay, Batak, Padang and Aceh.
Thus, it seems that children in this city are already
familiar with the diversity. What is interesting that
researchers found in this city was that each ethnic
and racial group can co-exist without being
disturbed by the presence of each other. This
reflected in daily activities, showing that each ethnic
and racial group lived life in a way that was taught
by their culture without the need to force other
ethnic groups to behave similarly to them. For
example, Chinese can freely use Mandarin as their
everyday language; ethnic Indians can also perform
common rituals in their culture without fear of
feeling different from other local ethnicities. Again,
this proved that in the context of ethnic prejudice,
the important factor is not just how much interethnic
contact there is, but the main thing is how the
prevailing social culture and norms teach positive
moral values. Maybe each of these ethnic groups has
rare interethnic contact, but this does not necessarily
make them feel negative or prejudiced against other
ethnic groups. However, it is necessary to be careful
in understanding these results. There are two reasons
why it is necessary to be careful in understanding the
absence of a relationship between ethnic identity and
ethnic prejudice in children in Medan. First, it is
possible that the conditions present suggest a
relatively stable hierarchy of status among groups,
so the dominant group may not be openly prejudice
(Hogg, Abrams and Patel, 1987 in Masson and
Verkuyten, 1993). Secondly, there is a "modern" or
"symbolic" approach of racism (McConahay and
Hough, 1976 in Masson and Verkuyten, 1993) that it
is impossible in a modern era to be prejudiced or
express an overt attitude in degrading ethnic
minorities. This is because of the social norm that
prohibits them from openly showing negative
attitudes toward other ethnic groups. But it is not
immediately to assume that prejudice has been lost
in the modern era, because ethnic prejudice can
present in other ways, in more subtle forms as
conceptualized by Meertens and Pettigrew (1995) as
a blatant and subtle prejudice.
This study also shows that the correlation
between the two variables in the sample in
Bangkalan is stronger than the sample in
Yogyakarta. The strong ethnic identity of the
children in Bangkalan, which has developed with
strong cultural and religious backgrounds, has given
Bangkalan’s children a strong belief in their own
ethnic groups. As explained earlier this strong ethnic
identity makes individuals develop a more positive
perspective on aspects of their lives. This is what
makes Bangkalan children have a more positive
view of the presence of other ethnic groups, as long
as the presence of other ethnic groups does not
interfere with religion and honor. Perhaps this was
truly understood by the Chinese in their relationship
with the Bangkalan community. This gave a positive
experience for children in Bangkalan, and later
influenced their perceptions of Chinese.
5 CONCLUSION
The study concluded that there is a negative
relationship between ethnic identity and a child’s
ethnic prejudice. A negative relationship between
two variables occurred in two cities Bangkalan and
Yogyakarta. This means that the stronger the ethnic
identity of a child, the lower the ethnic prejudice and
vice versa. However, in Medan the results show
differently, in other words there is no relationship
between ethnic identity and ethnic prejudice.
However, in general, the results of this study
indicate that one effort to eliminate prejudice,
especially in multicultural countries like Indonesia,
is by strengthening the ethnic identity of children.
This can be done through the process of
socialization of the family, school and the
environment around the children. In the future, it is
hoped that similar research can also be conducted in
other areas by considering the weakness of this
research. For example, the selection of subjects
could be more representative, using a more
comprehensive statistical analysis and adding other
variables such as parents’ ethnic socialization, self-
esteem, peers, school climate, group norms, that may
affect directly or indirectly the prejudice so that the
Ethnic Identity and Ethnic Prejudice in Children: A Comparative Study of the Three Cities in Indonesia
213
picture of the prejudice development will be more
comprehensive. The contribution of this research is
that to eliminate prejudice in children we need to
strengthen their ethnic identity, make them feel
proud of the ethnicity they have so they can see the
existence of other ethnic groups as more equal.
REFERENCES
Aboud, F.E., Levy, S.R., 2000. Interventions to reduce
prejudice and discrimination in children and
adolescents. In S. Oskamp (Ed.), Reducing prejudice
and discrimination (pp. 269–293). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Adomatussa, M., Warsono., 2017. Toleransi antara etnis
madura dengan etnis Tionghoa di pecinan kabupaten
Bangkalan Madura. Kajian Moral dan
Kewarganegaraan, Volume 05, Nomor 02, 1-16.
Allport, G., 1954. The nature of prejudice. Cambridge,
MA: Addison-Wesley.
Byrd, M., 2011. The measurement of racial/ethnic identity
in children : A critical review Christy. Journal of
Black Psychology 2012 38: 3.
Cokley, K., 2007. Critical issues in the measurement of
ethnic and racial identity: A referendum on the state of
the field. Journal of Counseling Psychology, Vol. 54,
No. 3, 224–234.
Costello, K, Hodson, G., 2012. Explaining
dehumanization among children: The interspecies
model of prejudice.
British Journal of Social Psychology vol. 53 (1) 175-197.
Dubow, E.F., Pargament, K.I., Boxer, P. Dan Tarakeswar,
N., 2000. Initial investigation of Jewish early
adolescent’s ethnic identity, stress and coping. The
Journal of Early Adolescence, vol. 20, issue 4.
Isajiw, W., 1999. Definitions and dimensions of ethnicity.
Toronto: University of Toronto Press.
Kail, R. V., 2010. Children and their development 5
th
ed.
New Jersey : Pearson Prentice Hall.
Killen, R., 2007. Teaching strategies for outcomes-based
education. Juta and Company Ltd.
Killen, M., Hitti, A., Mulvey, K.L., 2015. Social
development and intergroup relations. APA Handbook
of Personality and Social Psychology: Vol. 2. Group
Processes, M. Mikulincer and P. R. Shaver (Editors-
in-Chief).
Lubis, R., 1995. Pribumi dimata orang Cina. Medan : PT.
Pustaka Widyasarana.
Masson, C. N., Verkuyten, M., 1993. Prejudice ethnic
identity, contact and ethnic group preferences among
Dutch young adolescents. Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, Vol. 23. Issue 2, 156-168.
Markus, H. R., 2008. Pride, prejudice, and ambivalence:
Toward a unified theory of race and ethnicity.
American Psychologist, November.
Myers, D. G., 1999. Social psychology (5
th
ed). New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Ocampo, K.A., Knight, G. P., Bernal, M. E., 1997. The
development of cognitive abilities and social identities
in children: The case of ethnic identity. Journal of
Behavioral Development, 21 (3), 479-500.
Pettigrew, T. F., Meertens, R.W., 1995. Subtle and blatant
prejudice in Western Europe. European Journal of
Social Psychology, 25, 57-75.
Pehrson, S., Brown, R., Zagefka, H., 2009. When does
national iden- tification lead to the rejection of
immigrants? Cross-sectional and longitudinal evidence
for the role of essentialist in-group definitions. British
Journal of Social Psychology, 48, 61–76.
Rutland, A., Killen, M., Abrams, D., 2010. A New social-
cognitive developmental perspective on prejudice: The
interplay between morality and group identity.
Perspective on Psychology Science 5(3) 279-291.
Salmeria, S., Pellerone, M., 2015. The role of identity
development on latent and manifest prejudice: the
perception of immigrants in Italian university context.
Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 174, 952
958.
Santi, D.E., 2014. The relationships among ethnic
prejudice, centripetal-centrifugal tendencies and
interpersonal attraction native children toward
Chinese. Paper presented at the International
conference psychology in health, educational, social
and organizational settings. Surabaya: Airlangga
University.
Scottham, K. M., Sellers, R. M., Nguyên, H. X., 2008. A
measure of racial identity in African American
adolescents: the development of the Multidimensional
Inventory of Black Identity--Teen. Cultural Diversity
and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 14(4), 297.
Sellers, R.M., Copeland-Linder, P., Martin, P.P., Lewis
R.L’H., 2006. Racial identity matters: The relationship
between racial discrimination and psychological
functioning in African American adolescents. Journal
of Research on Adolescence, 16(2), 187-216.
Smith, T. B., Sylva, L., 2011. Ethnic identity and personal
well-being of people of color: A metaanalysis. Journal
of Counseling Psychology, 58, 42-6.
Smith, E. P., Walker, K., Fields, L., Brookins, C. C. Seay,
R. C., 1999. “Ethnic identity and its relationship to self
esteem, perceived efficacy and pro social attitudes in
early adolescence”, Journal of Adolescence, 22, 867–
880.
Tajfel, H. T. JC., 1986. The social identity theory of
intergroup behavior. Psychology of Intergroup
Relations. Nelson-Hall, Chicago. Págs, 7-24.
Tajfel, H., Turner, J. C. 1979. An integrative theory of
intergroup conflict. See Austin & Worehel 1979, pp.
33-47
Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D.,
Wetherell, M. S., 1987. Rediscovering the social
group: A self-categorization theory. Contemporary
Sociology.
Umaña-Taylor, A. J., Diversi, M., Fine, M.A., 2002.
Ethnic identity and self-esteem of Latino adolescents :
Distinctions among the Latino populations. Journal of
Adolescent Research, 17: 30.3.
ICP-HESOS 2018 - International Conference on Psychology in Health, Educational, Social, and Organizational Settings
214
Wittig, M. A., Molina, L. E., Giang, M. T., Ainsworth, A.,
2007. Responding to racial-ethnic diversity: A mutual
acculturation model of prejudice reduction. Dalam R.
Brown, D. Capozza, & O. Licciardello (Eds.).
Immigrazione, accculturazione, modalita di contatto
(pp. 214-232).
Yogeeswaran, K., Adelman, L., Parker, M. T., Dasgupta,
N., 2014. In the eyes of the beholder: National
identification predicts differential reactions to ethnic
identity expressions. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic
Minority Psychology Vol. 20, No. 3, 362–369.
Ethnic Identity and Ethnic Prejudice in Children: A Comparative Study of the Three Cities in Indonesia
215