Is Self-efficacy
R
elated to Students’ Moral
R
easoning?:
A Research on Students’ Absentee Behavior
Aurelius Ratu, Ni Gusti Made Rai, Niken Prasetya, and Dyah Satya Yoga
Sepuluh Nopember Institute ofTechnology, Keputih, Surabaya, Indonesia
Keywords: Self-Efficacy, Moral Reasoning, Deviant Behavior.
Abstract: Some previous studies on moral reasoning related to self-efficacy in an academic setting have reported
students widely held moral belief that they know very well what is morally wrong or good. Generally, a
student’s moral awareness is presumably taken for granted at a college. This tendency often evokes the
deviant-behavioral tendencies either because of internal or external factors. The aim of this study was to
examine a correlation between self-efficacy and moral reasoning on students’ absentee behavior. Through a
bivariate correlation analysis with 0.05 alpha level, the result showed that the higher self-efficacy the more
they could make reasoning on moral decisions and the more they could justify a wrong action in certain
situations.
1 INTRODUCTION
Students ethics beliefs have been the subject of
numerous studies (Lawson, 2004; LaDuke, 2013).
Moreover, some studies have been devoted to
understand this growing phenomenon and to identify
factors related to the student's ethics. But, there were
just a few efforts to understand the ethics belief and
self-efficacy (Farnese et al., 2011a). As noted, in an
academic setting, the amoral behavior is often seen
as ‘nothing to worry about' or ‘everyone was doing
it'. It even has been regarded as customary behavior.
This is precisely the problem. The moral aspects are
unconsciously separated from academic purposes.
This study tries to give emphasis on moral
engagement acknowledge that academic dishonesty,
trusting oneself’ presence, typically calls forth moral
standard (Thorkildsen, Golant and Richesin, 2007).
Concerning the moral aspect, though it was
intrinsically discussed, some researches have been
done to get to a better understanding of this problem.
One of these researches indicated that there is a
correlation between a competitive system in an
academic setting with cheating behavior (Anderman
and Midgley, 2004; Cartwright and Menezes, 2014).
Another research indicated that students with the
highest grade point average actually had cheated or
done plagiarism (Patall and Leach, 2015). Even,
some students believed that unethical behavior is
essential to advance careers (Lawson, 2004).
Some people have viewed the breaking of the
regulations when they were in the academic process
as something beneficial. This inconsistency clearly
raises a question on the implementation of a
standard moral in an academic setting. Whereas
academic achievement is considered one important
criterion of educational quality, moral problems
were often considered as a part of personal
responsibilities (Hakimi, Hejazi and Lavasani,
2011). It is the case we want to elaborate. If we talk
about the moral standard, we talk about how
students' moral judgments on specific circumstance
relating to their moral principles (Palmer, 2005). In
other side, students' moral principles are related to
what Bandura called it a self-organizing, proactive,
self-reflective, and self-regulative mechanism
(Bandura, 2002).
In this paper, we focus on whether self-efficacy
have a correlation toward moral reasoning. In the
context of moral reasoning, we use four attributions
as a part of neutralization theory consisting of denial
of responsibility, appeal to higher priority/value,
denial of the injury, and denial of the victim
(Murdock and Stephens, 2007).
Ratu, A., Rai, N., Prasetya, N. and Yoga, D.
Is Self-efficacy Related to Students’ Moral Reasoning?: A Research on Students’ Absentee Behavior.
DOI: 10.5220/0008590003970405
In Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on Psychology in Health, Educational, Social, and Organizational Settings (ICP-HESOS 2018) - Improving Mental Health and Harmony in
Global Community, pages 397-405
ISBN: 978-989-758-435-0
Copyright
c
2020 by SCITEPRESS – Science and Technology Publications, Lda. All rights reserved
397
2 LITERATURE REVIEW
2.1 Theory of Neutralization -
Reasoning on Moral Decisions
This theory is part of theory deviant behavior.
Neutralization theory states a person knows that the
action is morally objectively wrong. But, because of
reasoning, that action seemed right. It means that a
person tries to avoid the responsibility and to
decrease negative emphasis from oneself and the
others. These attempts came out of doing something
wrong morally.
This theory was developed from Kohlberg's
hypothesis and was introduced firstly by Sykes and
Matza who observed that there are the tendencies
one’s moral development through education and
experience is not followed by the application of
moral conduct in certain situations (Murdock and
Stephens, 2007b; Sykes and Matza, 1957). In other
words, this neutralization lessens negative
judgments made by oneself and the others for the
behavior. Commonly, this neutralization is also
called justifications which are viewed as following
deviant behavior and as protecting the individual
from self-blame and the blame of others after the
act.
The emphasis on moral aspects in this study
leads to what becomes a moral standard in an
academic setting. Moral aspects should be inherently
regularities of the college as positive law. However,
several factors such as motivation to achieve a better
GPA, environmental and friendship influences
caused the existing rules forceless. Some studies
found this phenomenon as an inconsistency between
academic attitude and the moral behavior
(Cartwright and Menezes, 2014; Hakimi, Hejazi and
Lavasani, 2011; Iorga, Ciuhodaru and Romedea,
2013; Lawson, 2004; Turiel, 2015). One of these
inconsistencies is the behavior of the absentee who
entrusts oneself’s presence to the others for avoiding
his/herself from the lack of absent percentage in
class. That behavior revealed how students
understand their moral principles but at the same
time, they can neutralize those principles for some
reasons.
Absentee behavior, trust oneself’ presence,
basically is not only concerning how moral
awareness is examined in an academic setting. It
also brings an understanding of how the students
believe their moral standard and acts on it.
Following neutralization theory, we use the
attribution theory to examine the absentee behavior,
i.e. denial of responsibility, the higher priority,
denial of the injury, and the denial of the victims.
This theory rests on assumption that everyone is
innately motivated to make sense of his or her world
particularly events that are negative, unexpected, or
not normative (Murdock and Stephens, 2007).
The first attribution is concerning how a person
can externalize responsibility. The second attribution
is concerning with capability of determining priority
scale. The third attribution is concerning with
capability of making an excuse for an illegal action
but morally not a wrong action. The fourth
attribution is concerning with capability of
transforming oneself into a victim of wrongdoing.
2.2 Theory of Self-efficacy
In an academic setting, self-efficacy was often
related to achievements either grade point average,
learning strategies, or even dishonest behavior
(Farnese et al., 2011b; Murdock, Tamera B. and
Murdock, 2006; Stajkovic et al., 2018). Self-efficacy
is the belief in self-capacity to drive one’s
motivation. It is also affected by cognitive aspects
and rises from the need to cope with specific
situations. Self-efficacy has some functions to
predict an important task concerning the work
behavior, the learning ability, and so forth. Based on
Bandura ’s social cognitive theory, self-efficacy has
three dimensions that of magnitude, strength, and
generality (Bandura, 1978; Bandura et al., 2001).
The first dimension is dealing with a level of
difficulties. The Second dimension is dealing with
the willingness to show the results from the
difficulty level of the task. The third dimension is
dealing with the belief in the task based on
experiences gained.
Furthermore, self-efficacy makes a person be
able to predict his future actions (Azizli et al., 2015).
The belief on self-capability will encourage a person
to adjust oneself in achieving the best results for the
future career. This self-capability even predisposes a
person to organize the strategies and the planning in
order to achieve the expected goals. But the moral
problems could emerge if orientation towards the
expected goals was not in accordance with the
regulations. In this study, self-efficacy could be a
moral justification in the sense that absentee
behavior is made personally and socially acceptable
by portraying it as serving socially worthy or moral
purposes. It even occurs when the students can act
on moral imperative and preserve their view of
themselves as moral agents while inflicting harms on
others (Bandura, 1978, 2002, 1999; Bandura et al.,
1996).
ICP-HESOS 2018 - International Conference on Psychology in Health, Educational, Social, and Organizational Settings
398
3 METHOD
3.1 Participants
Five hundred twenty-one students from 29
departments at Sepuluh Nopember Institute of
technology were asked to fill in a questionnaire. It
was done during the regular period of the learning
process. The data concerning gender, age, and grade
point average (GPA) is shown in table 1.
Table 1: Demographic Variables of the samples (N = 521).
Number* Percenta
g
e*
Gender Man 289 55,5
Woman 232 44,5
A
g
e 18-20 460 88,3
21-23 61 11,7
GPA <2.00 5 1,0
2.01-2.5 31 6,0
2.51-3.00 120 23,0
3.01-3.50 234 44,9
>3.51 131 25,1
Note: *Number and percentages based on cases with valid
responses
3.2 Measures
Self-efficacy was assessed by an 8-item Likert
Scale. Response option was provided in a five-point
format ranging from 1= strongly disagree to 5 =
strongly agree (Chen, Gully and Eden, 2001). All
statements of Self-efficacy were acceptable at 0,78
Cronbach's alpha for the validated scale. The
students were also asked to express their opinion
regarding self-perception during the academic
process. For the Likert scale’s result, we divided the
level of self-efficacy into quartiles as shown in table
2.
Table 2: Frequency Statistic from the total score on self-
efficacy (N=521).
N Valid 521
Missing 0
Mean 29,67
Median 30,00
Std. Deviation 3,670
Percentiles 25 28,00
50 30,00
75 32,00
The result of socio-demographic' relation
(gender, age, and GPA) with the level of self-
efficacy is shown in table 3.
The moral reasoning was assessed by a twenty-
item Likert scale. The scale was specifically
developed for this study on the basis of ethical
questionnaire measures developed by Don Forsyth
(Forsyth, O’Boyle and McDaniel, 2008). We,
furthermore, classified the twenty statements into
four attributions theory as suggested by Sykes and
Matza (Stephens, 2007; Sykes and Matza, 1957).
The statements of moral reasoning were acceptable
at 0,86 Cronbach's alpha (three questions were not
valid, we did not include them for further analysis).
Table 3: Frequency statistic on self-efficacy’s quartile (N=521)
Category Low Medium High Very High
Percentile <25% 25-49% 50-74% >75%
N N N N
Gender Man 73 69 77 70
Woman 55 51 52 74
Age 18-20 111 108 115 126
21-23 17 12 14 18
GPA <2.00 2 1 0 2
2.01-2.5 9 9 4 9
2.51-3.00 30 30 27 33
3.01-3.50 55 52 61 66
>3.51 32 28 37 34
Is Self-efficacy Related to Students’ Moral Reasoning?: A Research on Students’ Absentee Behavior
399
3.3 Hypotheses
In this study, the first hypothesis we propose, i.e.:
H1: Act of trusting one self’s presence is morally a
wrong behavior for students.
The next hypothesis will examine GPA’ students
related to their level of self-efficacy. For this, we
hypothesize:
H2: In general, the higher the students’ self-
efficacy, the higher their GPA.
Based on first two-hypotheses above, concerning
moral reasoning and their belief in their moral
standard, we hypothesize:
H3: Students with higher self-efficacy are more
likely to do morally right actions.
4 RESULTS
Each table in this section was measured by
Kendall’s Tau-B (because of non-normality
distribution of the responses, use of non-parametric
statistic was desirable).
Table 4 summarizes the responses to the four
chosen statements related to moral principles in
trusting one' presence. The mean result related to
their moral principles as shown by the first statement
indicated that the higher students' self-efficacy the
more, they understand moral behavior. Significance'
result of 2-tailed probability (the second and the
third statements) in students with a low, medium,
and high self-efficacy showed that the null
hypothesis was accepted because there was no
relationship between the belief in moral principles
and their perception of the absentee behavior.
Responses to the statements that absentee
behavior is just an academic agreement and has a
smaller impact than getting low scores in
exam/assignment were statistically not significant.
Meanwhile, the belief in moral principles and the
guilty in all level of students' self-efficacy showed a
negative sign. In this case, a negative sign indicated
that students felt guilty when they did an action
which was morally wrong. A correlation of first and
fourth statement, however, was statistically
significant only at students with very high self-
efficacy. It might be that they maintain their belief in
moral principles by feeling guilty when trusting their
presence.
Table 5 displays the moral reasoning’s mean for
each level of self-efficacy were 56,66 (low), 55,82
(medium), 58,35 (high), and 55,93 (very high). The
mean of all students’ moral reasoning was 56.68.
This result indicated that students with high self-
efficacy had an average score above the overall
average of students’ moral reasoning which was
followed in the second order by students with low
self-efficacy. The result also showed that the highest
score of moral reasoning’ scale (84) was in the
student with low self-efficacy. Meanwhile, the
highest GPA’ mean was in students with high self-
efficacy. The lowest GPA’ mean was in students
with medium self-efficacy. The interesting result
was that students with lowest GPA’ mean are more
Table 4: Students' perception of absentee behavior and their belief in moral principles
Low Medium High Very High
Mean 2-tailed Mean 2-tailed Mean 2-tailed Mean 2-tailed
I believe that my moral principles are
truly good and right
4.01 1 4.15 1 4.29 1 4.43 1
In general, I believe that absentee
behavior is a matter of the
rules/academic agreements, not the
issue of moral principles
2.47 0.377 2.56 0.941 2.74 0.877 2.54 0.808
I believe that an absentee behavior only
has a smaller impact than getting a low
score on the exam/assignment
2.63 0.079 2.50 0.979 2.61 0.422 2.56 0.488
I do not feel guilty when I did an
absentee behavior
1.93 - 0.854 1.90 - 0.092 2.05 - 0.081 1.83 - 0.035*
Note: * negative sign shows an opposite relationship (p-value < 0.05)
ICP-HESOS 2018 - International Conference on Psychology in Health, Educational, Social, and Organizational Settings
400
likely to make reasoning on the moral decisions.
Their correlation is statistically significant at p-value
<0.05.
Table 6 shows the correlation between all
statements on moral reasoning and each level of
self-efficacy. There were three statements which had
a significant correlation to students' self-efficacy.
The students' responses to ‘I believe that my moral
principles are truly good and right’ and ‘I am able to
convince my friends to tolerate my wrong actions’
were statistically significant in the students with
very high self-efficacy. This result also showed that
the mean for students' moral reasoning was more
likely to increase from low self-efficacy to very high
self-efficacy. Responses to ‘I will not tell the truth if
the lecturer found that my friend trusted his/her
presence through me' was statistically significant in
students with low self-efficacy. The negative sign
for the probability indicated that the higher self-
efficacy the more they will tell the truth about an
existing absentee behavior in the class.
5 DISCUSSION
The results of this study indicated that in doing the
absentee deed there is a very strong relationship
between students' moral principles and their level of
self-efficacy. This result, unfortunately, showed the
inconsistency in the students' behavior regarding
moral principles and their self-efficacy. It might be
that this inconsistency was related to the
neutralization efforts since they encountered the
uncertainty of outcomes (Rettinger, 2007).
Table 5: Socio-demographic variables, moral reasoning, and level of self-efficacy
Category Low Medium High Very High
Percentile <25% 25-49% 50-74% >75%
Students' Moral
Reasoning
minimum scores 35 35 35 35
maximum scores 84 79 83 81
56.66 55.82 58.35 55.93
GPA Mean
2.83 2.81 3.02 2.84
2-tailed* 0.433 0.021** 0.747 0.833
Gender
Man 73 69 77 70
woman 55 51 52 74
N 128 120 129 144
Note: * a correlation between GPA and moral reasoning
** p-value < 0.05
Table 6: A correlation between significant self-report statistically on absentee behavior and level of self-efficacy
Category Low Medium High Very High
<25% 25-49% 50-74% >75%
I believe that my moral principles are truly
good and right
p-value. (2-tailed) -0,295 0,182 0,810 0,000**
mean 4.01 4.15 4.29 4.43
I am able to convince my friends to tolerate my
wrong actions
p-value. (2-tailed) -0,659 -0,270 0,853 0,020*
mean 3.01 3.27 3.40 3.44
I will not tell the truth if the lecturer found that
my friend trusted his/her presence through me
p-value. (2-tailed) -0,003** -0,267 0,556 -0,193
mean 2.63 2.31 2.64 2.24
Note: * p-value < 0.05
** p-value < 0.01
Is Self-efficacy Related to Students’ Moral Reasoning?: A Research on Students’ Absentee Behavior
401
5.1 Denial of Responsibility
Following Murdock and Stephens (Stephens, 2007),
students might know that absentee behavior is
morally wrong but they feel no guilt and personally
are not responsible for behaving in a manner
consistent with their moral judgment. Our finding
showed that students with very high self-efficacy
were able to convince their friends regarding their
deeds which were morally wrong action. This ability
to convince appears along with the level of self-
efficacy where it was indicated by the increasing
mean of students’ moral reasoning.
Moral engagement in making decisions under
risky circumstances seemed to lose moral weight.
The process was termed as externalizing
responsibility to avoid the self-blame and at the
same time to justify their deviant behavior. Our
finding, however, also showed that students with
very high self-efficacy were more likely to feel
guilty of doing absentee than students with low,
medium and high self-efficacy. This result suggested
that the null hypothesis of H1 was accepted.
5.2 Appeal to Higher Priority
As indicated by Sykes and Matza (Sykes and Matza,
1957), students did not necessarily repudiate a
normative system. Rather, they felt that they are
trapped in a dilemma that must be resolved,
unfortunately, by violating the regulations. The most
important point is that the students' deviant behavior
might occur not because the norms are rejected, but
other norms held to be a more pressing priority.
Moral decisions they made on the basis of
conflicting values was often subordinated under the
academic goals. This moral dilemma might relate to
the duty for doing an action that is morally good
despite the not illegal action, mala prohibita, but was
only imposed on the students personally. The
personal goals to become a qualified student were
only inflicted on each the students. As Thorkildsen
et all stated that communal value necessarily offers
helpful practices for encouraging students to resist
the temptation (academic dishonesty) (Thorkildsen,
Golant and Richesin, 2007). In this case, we found
the inconsistency among students regarding their
moral manifestation in certain situations. Yanif et all
stated this dilemma as sacrificing moral principles
(Yaniv, Siniver, and Tobol, 2017)
Opportunities to be students with the best
academic scores enhanced the perceptions that one
has a greater chance and justification for absentee
behavior (Patall and Leach, 2015). The priority in
achieving the academic goals more or less explains
why only students with medium self-efficacy have
statistically a significant probability. The given
result describes what Davis stated as an effort to
choose between a wrong and another wrong (Davis,
2007). This GPA’ variable, however, is not the main
predictor for this attribution. There might be other
predictors which were not entered into the analyses.
The null hypothesis of H2 was accepted that the
higher students’ self-efficacy did not correlate with
the higher students’ GPA.
5.3 Denial of the Crime – The Victim
It was very hard for the students to admit that the
absentee deed was more related to moral behavior
than an illegal action. It might occur since they
defined that their action would not harm the others,
despite in fact their action was contrary to the
regulations. We precisely observed that students
could make a distinction in evaluating the
wrongfulness of their behavior. For the students
with very high self-efficacy, they could turn on a
question of interpretation on their deviant behavior
into whether or not anyone has been hurt by his
actions. In this sense, peers behavior could facilitate
academic dishonesty (Farnese et al., 2011b).
For this kind of reasoning, we suggest that the
moral awareness of students was weakened by
external circumstances which they saw more
important than keeping their moral standard. At the
same time, when caught doing the action, students
pretended to be a victim of what they did earlier.
This was, of course, a strategy to rationalize or even
to externalize moral indignation of the other
(teacher, college) to something abstract. As stated by
Sykes and Matza, internalized norms and
anticipations of the reactions of others must
somehow be activated, if they are to serve as guides
for behavior. It is possible that a diminished
awareness of the victim plays an important part in
determining whether or not this process is set in
motion (Sykes and Matza, 1957). This was made
clear by the results that the students with high self-
efficacy were more likely able to neutralize their
moral standard to be accepted by the others
(classmate, lecturer). Thus, the null hypothesis of H3
was accepted that of the higher self-efficacy did not
correlate with morally good behavior in certain
situations.
ICP-HESOS 2018 - International Conference on Psychology in Health, Educational, Social, and Organizational Settings
402
6 CONCLUSION
The students' self-efficacy in this study
demonstrated an understanding of how they held the
moral belief in attaining personal academic goals. In
circumstances that demand moral decisions the
higher self-efficacy, the more students are capable of
neutralizing their moral principles to justify or to
make sense of his or her world particularly events
that are negative, unexpected, or not normative.
Students’ GPA indicated that this factor could be a
predictor for increasing the significant probability
into students’ self-efficacy towards moral reasoning.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank LPPM Sepuluh Nopember Institute of
Technology for funding our research. The views
conveyed in this publication do not necessarily
represent the views of the supporting institution.
REFERENCES
Anderman, E.M. and Midgley, C., 2004. Changes in
self-reported academic cheating across the
transition from middle school to high school.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, [online]
29(4), pp.499–517. Available at:
<http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S036
1476X04000232>.
Azizli, N., Atkinson, B.E., Baughman, H.M. and
Giammarco, E.A., 2015. Relationships between
general self-efficacy, planning for the future, and
life satisfaction. Personality and Individual
Differences, 82, pp.58–60.
Bandura, A., 1978. Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying
theory of behavioral change. Advances in
Behaviour Research and Therapy, [online] 1(4),
pp.139–161. Available at:
<https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/p
ii/0146640278900024> [Accessed 12 Jul. 2018].
Bandura, A., 1999. Moral Disengagement in the
Perpetration of Inhumanities. Personality and
Social Psychology Review, [online] 3(3), pp.193–
209. Available at:
<http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1207/s1532
7957pspr0303_3> [Accessed 5 Jul. 2018].
Bandura, A., 2002. Selective moral disengagement
in the exercise of moral agency. Journal of
Moral Education, [online] 31(2), pp.101–119.
Available at:
<http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/0
305724022014322> [Accessed 5 Jul. 2018].
Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G.V. and
Pastorelli, C., 1996. Mechanisms of moral
disengagement in the exercise of moral agency.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
[online] 71(2), pp.364–374. Available at:
<http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/002
2-3514.71.2.364> [Accessed 5 Jul. 2018].
Bandura, A., Caprara, G.V., Barbaranelli, C.,
Pastorelli, C., and Regalia, C., 2001.
Sociocognitive self-regulatory mechanisms
governing transgressive behavior. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, [online]
80(1), pp.125–135. Available at:
<http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/002
2-3514.80.1.125> [Accessed 5 Jul. 2018].
Cartwright, E. and Menezes, M.L.C., 2014. Cheating
to win: Dishonesty and the intensity of
competition. Economics Letters, [online] 122(1),
pp.55–58. Available at:
<http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.econlet.2013.10.016
>.
Chen, G., Gully, S.M. and Eden, D., 2001.
Validation of a New General Self-Efficacy Scale.
Organizational Research Methods, [online] 4(1),
pp.62–83. Available at:
<http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/10944
2810141004>.
Davis, N.A., 2007. Moral Dilemmas. In: A
Companion to Applied Ethics. [online] Oxford,
UK: Blackwell Publishing Ltd, pp.487–497.
Available at:
<http://doi.wiley.com/10.1002/9780470996621.c
h36>.
Farnese, M.L., Tramontano, C., Fida, R. and
Paciello, M., 2011a. Cheating Behaviors in
Academic Context: Does Academic Moral
Disengagement Matter? Procedia - Social and
Behavioral Sciences, [online] 29, pp.356–365.
Available at:
<https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/p
ii/S187704281102711X> [Accessed 5 Jul. 2018].
Farnese, M.L., Tramontano, C., Fida, R. and
Paciello, M., 2011b. Cheating Behaviors in
Academic Context: Does Academic Moral
Disengagement Matter? Procedia - Social and
Behavioral Sciences, [online] 29, pp.356–365.
Available at:
<http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.11.250>
.
Forsyth, D.R., O’Boyle, E.H. and McDaniel, M.A.,
2008. East Meets West: A Meta-Analytic
Investigation of Cultural Variations in Idealism
and Relativism. Journal of Business Ethics,
Is Self-efficacy Related to Students’ Moral Reasoning?: A Research on Students’ Absentee Behavior
403
[online] 83(4), pp.813–833. Available at:
<http://link.springer.com/10.1007/s10551-008-
9667-6>.
Hakimi, S., Hejazi, E. and Lavasani, M.G., 2011.
The relationships between personality traits and
students’ academic achievement. Procedia -
Social and Behavioral Sciences, 29, pp.836–845.
Iorga, M., Ciuhodaru, T. and Romedea, S.-N., 2013.
Ethic and Unethic. Students and the Unethical
Behavior During Academic Years. In: Procedia -
Social and Behavioral Sciences. [online]
Elsevier, pp.54–58. Available at:
<https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/p
ii/S1877042813032540> [Accessed 17 Jan.
2018].
LaDuke, R.D., 2013. Academic Dishonesty Today,
Unethical Practices Tomorrow? Journal of
Professional Nursing, [online] 29(6), pp.402–
406. Available at:
<http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.profnurs.2012.10.00
9>.
Lawson, R.A., 2004. Is Classroom Cheating Related
to Business Students’ Propensity to Cheat in the
‘Real World’? Journal of Business Ethics,
[online] 49(2), pp.189–199. Available at:
<http://link.springer.com/10.1023/B:BUSI.00000
15784.34148.cb>.
Murdock, Tamera B. and Murdock, E.M., 2006.
Motivational Perspectives on Student
Cheating:Toward an Integrated Model of
Academic Dishonesty. Educational Psychologist,
41(3), pp.129–145.
Murdock, T.B. and Stephens, J.M., 2007. Is
Cheating Wrong? Students’ Reasoning about
Academic Dishonesty. In: E.M. Anderman and
T.B. Murdock, eds., Psychology of Academic
Cheating. [online] Burlington: Elsevier, pp.229–
251. Available at:
<https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/p
ii/B9780123725417500140>.
Palmer, E.J., 2005. The relationship between moral
reasoning and aggression, and the implications
for practice. Psychology, Crime & Law, [online]
11(4), pp.353–361. Available at:
<http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/1
0683160500255190>.
Patall, E.A. and Leach, J.K., 2015. The role of
choice provision in academic dishonesty.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, [online]
42, pp.97–110. Available at:
<http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2015.06.0
04>.
Rettinger, D.A., 2007. Applying Decision Theory to
Academic Integrity Decisions. In: Psychology of
Academic Cheating. [online] Academic Press,
pp.141–167. Available at:
<https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/p
ii/B9780123725417500115> [Accessed 19 Jul.
2018].
Stajkovic, A.D., Bandura, A., Locke, E.A., Lee, D.
and Sergent, K., 2018. Personality and Individual
Di ff erences Test of three conceptual models of
in fl uence of the big fi ve personality traits and
self-e ffi cacy on academic performance : A
meta-analytic path-analysis. Personality and
Individual Differences, [online] 120(August
2017), pp.238–245. Available at:
<http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.08.014>.
Stephens, J.M., 2007. Is Cheating Wrong? Students’
Reasoning about Academic Dishonesty.
Psychology of Academic Cheating, [online]
pp.229–251. Available at:
<https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/p
ii/B9780123725417500140> [Accessed 5 Jul.
2018].
Sykes, G.M. and Matza, D., 1957. Techniques of
Neutralization: A Theory of Delinquency.
American Sociological Review, [online] 22(6),
p.664. Available at:
<https://is.muni.cz/el/1423/podzim2016/BSS166
/um/Sykes__Matza_Techniques_of_Neutralizati
on.pdf>.
Thorkildsen, T.A., Golant, C.J. and Richesin, L.D.,
2007. Reaping What We Sow. In:
Psychology of
Academic Cheating. [online] Elsevier, pp.171–
202. Available at:
<http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/B978
0123725417500127>.
Turiel, E., 2015. Moral Reasoning in Psychology.
In: International Encyclopedia of the Social &
Behavioral Sciences. [online] Elsevier, pp.803–
805. Available at:
<https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/p
ii/B9780080970868250204> [Accessed 12 Jul.
2018].
Yaniv, G., Siniver, E. and Tobol, Y., 2017. Do
higher achievers cheat less? An experiment of
self-revealing individual cheating. Journal of
Behavioral and Experimental Economics,
[online] 68, pp.91–96. Available at:
<http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S221
4804317300411>.
ICP-HESOS 2018 - International Conference on Psychology in Health, Educational, Social, and Organizational Settings
404
APPENDIX
Appendix 1: Self-Efficacy scale
No Item
1 Sa
y
a mam
p
u menca
p
ai seba
g
ian besar tu
j
uan
y
an
g
sudah sa
y
a teta
p
kan
2 Ketika saya menghadapi tugas sulit maka saya yakin akan dapat menyelesaikannya
3 Secara umum, saya akan bisa mendapat hasil baik yang penting bagi diri saya
4 Saya percaya bahwa saya berhasil dengan maksimal untuk usaha yang saya tetapkan
5 Sa
y
a akan mam
p
u men
y
elesaikan ban
y
ak tantan
g
an den
g
an bai
k
6 Sa
a
akin bahwa sa
a bisa tam
il efektif dalam berba
i hal
7 Dibandin
g
kan den
g
an oran
g
lain, sa
y
a bisa melakukan tu
g
as den
g
an san
g
at baik
8 Bahkan ketika menghadapi persoalan yang sulit, saya dapat menyelesaikannya dengan bai
k
Appendix 2: Moral Reasoning scale.
No Item
1 Sa
y
a
y
akin bahwa
p
rinsi
p
moral
y
an
g
sa
y
a anut adalah sun
gg
uh-sun
gg
uh baik dan bena
r
2 Secara umum, saya meyakini bahwa tindakan titip absen adalah persoalan Aturan/Kesepakatan Akademis
b
ukan
p
ersoalan
p
rinsi
p
Moral
3 Sa
y
a me
y
akini bahwa teman satu
j
urusan da
p
at diandalkan untuk melakukan titi
p
absen
4 Sa
y
a
p
erca
y
a bahwa teman sa
y
a sun
gg
uh-sun
gg
uh memahami men
g
a
p
a sa
y
a harus melakukan titi
p
absen
5 Di tengah kesibukan dan banyak tuntutan tugas, saya mampu mengatur waktu tanpa mengorbankan
kehadiran di kelas selama
p
erkuliahan
6 Saya percaya bahwa tindakan titip absen hanya berakibat lebih kecil daripada mendapat nilai rendah
dalam ujian/tugas.
7 Saya mampu melaksanakan prinsip moral yang saya anut dengan setia
8 Sa
y
a
p
erca
y
a bahwa sa
y
a mam
p
u mematuhi setia
p
p
eraturan den
g
an baik dan bena
r
9 Sa
y
a mam
p
u me
y
akinkan teman sa
y
a untuk mentoleransi tindakan sa
y
a
y
an
g
salah
10 Sa
y
a berani melakukan titi
p
absen karena
p
roses
p
erkuliahan
y
an
g
sa
y
a ikuti tidak be
g
itu menarik.
11 Saya tidak merasa bersalah ketika melakukan titip absen
12 Menurut saya, tindakan titip absen tidak berdampak pada sanksi akademis yang fatal
13 Jika ada teman sa
y
a
y
an
g
melakukan titi
p
absen, sa
y
a ber
p
enda
p
at itu adalah tindakan
y
an
g
lumrah/wa
j
a
r
14 Sa
y
a ber
p
ikir bahwa titi
p
absen adalah tindakan
y
an
g
tidak terlalu serius
15 Sa
y
a men
g
etahui den
g
an san
g
at baik kebi
j
akan tentan
g
aturan titi
p
absen
16 Teman-teman saya cenderung membiarkan jika saya melakukan titip absen
17 Demi solidaritas, saya bersedia melakukan titip absen untuk teman saya
18 Saya akan memberikan alasan yang masuk akal jika ketahuan titip absen
19 Menurut sa
y
a, sa
y
a melakukan titi
p
absen karena ban
y
akn
y
a tu
g
as akademis.
20 Saya tidak akan mengatakan yang sebenarnya jika dosen mengetahui teman saya melakukan titip absen
melalui saya
Is Self-efficacy Related to Students’ Moral Reasoning?: A Research on Students’ Absentee Behavior
405