The Effect of Age on Memory
Abdulkhaleq A. Al-Qahtani
1
1
Department of English, King Khalid University, Abha, Saudi Arabia
Keywords: Memory, Learning, Aging.
Abstract: This paper presents an overview of the memory system and how it’s affected by age. Each type of memory
is also explained and divided into their sub-memories; for sensory memory, there are three types namely:
iconic memory for visual stimuli, echoic memory for aural stimuli and haptic memory for touch; for long-
term memory, there are procedural and declarative memories. After each memory type, the paper presents
information found in the literature as to how some aspects of memory are influenced by age. The conclusions
are as follows: memory is not affected by age as a whole system, what is affected are certain functions of
memory subcategories. For example, little evidence shows that semantic memory, procedural memory and
episodic memory are affected by age. In addition, encoding and retrieving information from and to long-term
memory are found to be influenced. The paper concludes with an assertion that most if not all age-related
deficiencies can be overcome.
1 INTRODUCTION
Aging has been perceived to have a direct impact on
memory loss. This perception is not grounded on
research findings but was rather associated with
anecdotes of aging people. As people age, they
become sensitive as to what abilities they have lost
and what they have retained over the passing years;
memory is one of those abilities. The observations of
aging adults are not fully refuted by research on
memory and aging. There seems to be a fair amount
of correlation between aging and a certain number of
memory functions (Merriam & Caffarella, 1999).
In order to understand how age affects memory,
one needs to know how memory works in the first
place. There are three main types of memory. i.e.,
sensory memory, short-term memory (sometimes
referred to as the working memory), and long-term
memory. Each type has its own constituent parts that
may or may not be affected by age either entirely or
partially. This paper will attempt to outline the main
types of memory and their subsections and explore in
the light of research findings how each and every type
might be affected by age.
2 SENSORY MEMORY
This type of memory is the first part of the
memory system where information is first perceived.
The name sensory is connected directly to the
function of this type of memory, the senses. A sensory
memory is available for every sensory channel: iconic
memory for visual stimuli, echoic memory for aural
stimuli and haptic memory for touch. Here is how it
works; information is first encountered by the senses
sight, hearing, touch, taste, or smell. Then the
information is saved for a very short time from 1 5
seconds. Then the selected information is sent to the
next memory (the short term memory). Then the
function of this type of memory stops. The place in
the human brain in which information is saved is
called sensory registers. The information at this stage
is not processed (Matlin, 1998). At this point a
person decides which part of information is worth
further processing and which part is not through the
process of attention. It is worth noting that most of
the information received in the sensory registers is
filtered out of the system (Ormrod, 1995).
An example of sensory memory would be a key
word heard in a lecture after losing attention to what
the lecturer had just said that triggered a person’s
mind to get back to the lecture. Remembering the
word right before that key word is locating that word
in the echoic memory. Another example would be a
hearer’s attention to someone reading a sentence
when the hearer holds the words at the beginning of
the sentence until the speaker finishes to make sense
of the whole utterance. These words are held in the
Al-Qahtani, A.
The Effect of Age on Memory.
DOI: 10.5220/0008682601730177
In Improving Educational Quality Toward International Standard (ICED-QA 2018), pages 173-177
ISBN: 978-989-758-392-6
Copyright
c
2019 by SCITEPRESS Science and Technology Publications, Lda. All rights reserved
173
echoic memory and are not processed until the
sentence is uttered in full. A third example would be
watch someone/something move; the iconic memory
saves each snapshot of the moving picture and
connects it with the next before processing the whole
scene into a meaningful thought. It is just like the
pictures on a cartoon film where every picture is fixed
alone and then connected to the others to form a
motion on the film. Iconic memory retains
unprocessed pictures for a short while to connect
them to the rest and ultimately move on to the next
step in the memory system (the short-term memory).
The capacity of the sensory memory is not that
large (Ormrod, 1995). The fact that our brains can
take in large amount of information fairly accurately,
the length of time allowed in the sensory registers is
very short; and therefore, the maximum amount
allowed in this memory is almost one unit of
information at a time (Matlin, 1998). To be more
specific, iconic memory can only last for less than one
second, and echoic memory can only hold up to five
seconds. It is interesting to observe that the capacity
of sensory memory is measured by time not by space
or quantity (Salthouse, 1996).
From what is said about sensory memory thus far,
it is quite obvious that age could have a fairly strong
impact on at least two types of the sensory memories
namely the iconic and the echoic, but with lesser
impact on the haptic memory. Contrary to Merriam
& Caffarella (1999 )who argue that sensory memory
is not affected by age, as people age, they lose vision
and hearing more than other senses (touch, smell, and
taste). Thus, the amount received into the iconic and
echoic memories will be increasingly reduced, as
people grow older. On the other hand, the other
senses will remain intact, in most part, well into late
adulthood; and therefore, very little negative impact
may be ascribed to the haptic memory as well as the
registers for the smell and taste.
In sum, we cannot assert that the sensory memory
is affect by age because it is divided into sub-
memories, which are not all affected in the same way
or manner. Nevertheless, older people could make up
for such deficiencies by checking up their vision and
hearing and use aids to sharpen up these senses and
reduce aging consequences to this type of memory.
3 SHORT-TERM MEMORY
Short-term memory, sometimes called the working
memory, is the next step in the memory system right
after the sensory memory. What is working memory?
It is a limited-capacity store that can hold entering
information for a few seconds it is what is in your
thinking at the present moment. This store works in
the following manner: through selective attention,
information move from sensory memory to short-
term memory; here, two factors are crucial: the
amount of information and the duration. The
maximum normal amount of information that an adult
can hold in a single time is seven items of
information, and the duration is roughly from 20 to
30 seconds. The items of information could be
isolated units like one digit number or chunks where
a group of related smaller units are lumped together
to form a one-item chunk. The items are maintained
in short-term memory by means of rehearsal.
The process of chunking is proved to be valuable
for enhancing the performance of sort-term memory
(Salthouse, 1996). One chunk, or example, could
include the three digit numbers of an area code in a
telephone number instead of storing the same chunk
as a three different digits. The one chunk in this case
counts as a single unit for the possible seven units
mentioned earlier in the maximum capacity of short-
term memory. Consequently the maximum duration
for normal adult could also expand as a result of
chunking.
Short-term memory has been proven to have
direct impact on language learning (Gathercole and
Baddeley, 1993), and people who have problems in
this type of memory exhibit learning disabilities.
Reading, for example, engages short-term memory
directly in the process. The reader needs to hold
chunks of read words for a short time to connect them
to the words following them and the words before
them. Any disturbance in the short-term-memory,
therefore, would result in reading difficulty or even
disability (Hulme and Mackenzie, 1992).
The influence of aging on short-term memory is
found in literature. The speed by which young vs.
older adults process information varies. Older adults
tend to process information in their short-term
memory slower than that of younger adults (Bors and
Forrin, 1995). Salthouse (1991) conducted a study in
which he employed the most common way of
assessing short-term memory, which is the digit-span
test. In this test informants are given a sequence of
numbers or letters and then asked to recall them.
Older adults perform well in the recall as well as
younger adults but when the older ones were asked to
manipulate the information in other ways like recall
them in refers order, younger adults outperformed the
older. Salthouse explained this phenomenon as an
indicator that older adults have a fragile short-term
memory.
Another research conducted by Swanson (1999)
found that there are differences between older and
ICED-QA 2018 - International Conference On Education Development And Quality Assurance
174
younger adults in the general capacity of two different
processes: accessing new information and
maintaining old information. In this study, older
adults are found to have more problems accessing
new information than younger adults. As for the
efficiency of the maintenance of old information, the
study did not support a significant difference between
young and older adults. Thus, age affects only certain
tasks of short-term memory but not the whole
function. Older adults have problems accessing new
information but once the information enters the
system, older adults seem to keep the new
information as well as the younger adults (Swanson,
1999).
In short, age-related issues found in the literature
suggest that only two aspects are found to be relevant:
the speed of performance and the difficulty of
accessing new information. As for the speed of
performance, researchers like Salthouse suggest that
the slow performance of adults might have to do with
the physiological aspect of aging rather than a
cognitive process. As for the difficulty of accessing
new information for older adults, Swanson and others
argue that that older adults experience a strict intake
of new information due to two reasons: poor sensory
registers’ input and fixed expectations of what should
be processed. It stands to reason, as I see it, that
people lose increasing amount of their vision and
hearing as they grow older and this process affects the
amount of input to shot-term memory. Also, people
develop expectations of the world and what to
encounter as they grow older and novel input might
be overlooked because it was not expected. In other
words, the new information did not fit in the
established pattern/framework that has been
established over the years.
4 LONG-TERM MEMORY
The third and the last type of memory is long-term
memory. Long-term memory is the final destination
for the information input. As the name suggests,
long-term memory saves information taken in from
one minute to many years for later retrieval. Unlike
working memory/short-term memory, information in
the long-term memory is not there in a person’s
present thinking. Information is organized in files
based on meaningful connections between units of
information.
The long-term memory works in two-ways
directions: storing/depositing new information and
retrieving information from the store. As for
depositing information process, information is
transferred to long-term memory from short-term
memory by means of encoding and/or elaboration.
The key component of encoding and elaboration
processes is the meaningfulness of information and its
relations to what the person has already established
(schemata). For example, one piece of information
would be much more easier to encode and/or to
elaborate if the person can relate that new information
to prior experiences/memories he/she had stored
before. The second process, which is the retrieval
process, acts in two processes either by recognition or
by recall. In both processes, the person has to attend
to what is really needed and where to find them
among the meaningful files. Recognition is less
troublesome because the person is given clues, but
recall demands more efforts to retrieve the needed
information.
Long-term memory is divided into sub-memories
based on the type and function of different kinds of
information. The known categories are as follows:
declarative memory or explicit memory, which is the
memory that people actively retrieve (recall and
recognize) and procedural memory or implicit
memory which we learn over time and then perform
tasks as needed without much conscious effort, like
reading and swimming (Anderson, 1996). Skills
learned/deposited in the procedural memory are hard
to learn and hard to forget like the example of reading.
Declarative memory in turn is divided into two
subcategories based on the functions of these
memories: semantic memory and the episodic
memory. Semantic memories are those memories of
general knowledge and subject matter knowledge we
learn at schools and in life at large. It contains
concepts, verbal information, rules, and problem-
solving abilities. Semantic memory stores
information in a form of schemata rather than images.
Episodic memories are those memories related to life
events that people experience as they live. Usually
this type of memory is organized around episodes of
our lives in a form of images. Usually it helps us
remember related information around events and
make sense of what had happened.
Having presented the major components of long-
tem memory, one would wonder how would age
affect this type of memory and which part is more
affected than the other. Research findings suggest
that not all types of long-term memory are affected in
the same way and at the same level. Procedural
memory, under normal circumstances, for example, is
not affected by age as the declarative memory. And
semantic memory is not affected by age as the
episodic memory. Episodic memory, in fact, is the
one that is found to be more endangered by age more
than any other memory type (Bee, 1996).
The Effect of Age on Memory
175
Shinkeigaku (1994) conducted a study to measure the
affect of age on episodic memory. The subjects of
this study were two groups of Japanese, aged forty to
seventy-five years. They were asked 85 questions
about public events that are familiar to their Japanese
culture. The events were scatted over four decades.
The researcher found that as age goes up informants’
performance went down (missed correct answers).
Older informants did not do as good as the younger
ones in both old memories thirty years old events
and recent events ten and fewer years old events.
The results of this study confirm that episodic
memory is significantly affected by age. Semantic
memory, on the other hand, is less affected by age
because older people exhibit better or equivalent
knowledge as younger adults (Bee, 1996). As a
matter of fact, older people have more robust
schemata than younger adults due to longer
experiences and accumulating meaningful input.
In sum, the two main problems usually
experienced by older adults due to age, as it applies
to long-term memory, are the two processes of
encoding and retrieving information. As for the
encoding issue, researchers have detected problems in
the process of encoding new information. They
ascribed such observation to the fact that older adults
cannot fit first-time new information in any existing
schemata; here the well-established schemata resist
encoding the new information for lack of finding a
suitable place for it within the space already
organized (Swanson, 1999). Also, retrieval process
is problematic for older adults. This process is either
recognition or recall. If the task requires recognition,
the age variable is not found significant: older adults
and younger adults function relatively equally.
However, recall process is found to be more difficult
for older adults (Bee, 1996).
5 CONCLUSION
In this paper, I presented the way in which memory
works as a means to understand age’s impact on
memory. In the light of recent research reviewed in
this paper, age does not affect all types of memory in
the same ways. As a matter of fact, there are certain
aspects of memory that are not affected at all, like the
procedural memory. However, age’s impact is
detectible in other types of memories in different
ways. For example, in sensory memory, people
experience decline in the perception system (sight and
hearing), which would affect the amount of input
saved on the sensory register.
Working memory is affect by age as a result of the
slowing down of the processes of receiving and
sending information to the long-term memory. Also,
it is affected by the fact that information in short-term
memory tends to decay more rapidly in older adults.
Long term-memory is more affected in the area of
episodic memory more than the two other types. The
explanation given in the literature ascribed such
observation to the two processes of encoding and
retrieving to and from the long-term memory. At the
encoding level, older adults are inclined to
deposit/encode new input in well-established manner
by which new information get subsumed under
certain existing category; if the category is not there,
then the encoding process becomes more difficult. As
for the retrieval process, older people tend to exert
more effort to recall information due to confusion,
interferences, and ill categorized information.
The good news is that most of the difficulties cited
in the research on the effect of age on memory are not
fatal. As a matter of fact, people can overcome such
difficulties by employing certain techniques. For
example, at the sensory memory stage, older adult
could sharpen their vision and hearing by using
prescribed aids. Working memory could be saved by
rehearsing and chunking items of information. And
long-term memory processes of encoding and
retrieval could be maintained by practicing recall and
connecting new ideas to existing knowledge. Other
suggestions to improve long-term memory retrieval
and encoding may include focusing attention on the
tasks, avoiding distractions, intending to put effort,
avoiding tension, and using external aids like a paper
and a pencil.
REFERENCES
Anderson, J. 1996. "ACT: A simple theory of complex
cognition", American Psychologist, 51(4), 355-365.
Bee, H. 1996. The journey of adulthood (3rd ed.). Upper
Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Bors, D. A., & Forrin, B. 1995. Age, speed of information
processing, recall, and fluid intelligence. Intelligence,
20, 229-248.
Gathercole, S. and Baddeley, A. 1993. Working memory
and Language. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Hulme, C. & Mackenzie, S. 1992. Working Memory and
Severe Learning Difficulties. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates, Publishers
Merriam, S. & Caffarella, R. 1999. Learning in adulthood
(2nd addition). San Francesco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Matlin, M. 1998. Cognition (4th edition). Harcourt Brace
College Publishers, Orlando: FL.
ICED-QA 2018 - International Conference On Education Development And Quality Assurance
176
Ormrod, J. E. 1995. Human Learning 2nd edition.
Columbus, Ohio, Merrill.
Salthouse,T.A. 1996. The processing-speed theory of adult
age differences in cognition. Psychological
Review,103, 403 428.
Salthouse, T. A. 1991. Theoretical perspectives on
cognition and aging. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence
Erlbaum Press.
Shinkeigaku, R. 1994. Influence of age on long-term
memory. PubMed, 34 (8), 777-781
Swanson, H. L. 1999. What develops in working memory?
A life span perspective. Developmental psychology,
35(4), 9861000
The Effect of Age on Memory
177