The Stress Relief Effects of Foot Warming during Mental Workload
Masahiro Inazawa, Yuki Ban, Rui Fukui and Shin’ichi Warisawa
Development of Human and Engineered Environmental Studies, Trontier Sciences,
The University of Tokyo, Kashiwa, Chiba, Japan
Keywords: Temperature Stimuli, Stress Relief, Biomarker, Comfort Presentation.
Abstract: Stress has become a social problem in recent years, and stress control plays a key role in daily life. Researchers
have studied methods of stress detection and spontaneous stress relief such as listening to soothing music and
walking in a forest. However, some people are unable to take spontaneous breaks; therefore, the development
of a means of taking “nonexplicit breaks,to relieve stress unconsciously while working, is required. In this
study, we proposed a stress-relief method that did not disturb working. To relieve stress, individuals warm
their feet while working, because their hands and feet often become cold when they are under stress. We
examined the effect of stress relief via foot warming, and the results revealed that warming the feet caused an
increase in RR intervals related to relaxation levels.
1 INTRODUCTION
In recent years, suicide has become a serious problem
among workers, particularly in Japan. In total, 28.2%
of Japanese people who died by suicide were
employees, and 42.1% of the causes of suicide
involved mental diseases such as depression
(National Police Agency, 2017). Depression has been
found to result from chronic stress
(Yuanyuan, 2013),
and it is necessary to detect and relieve stress in the
work environment to prevent this. Studies have
examined some methods of stress detection and
measurement. These methods include questionnaires,
such as the Perceived Stress Scale 14, which uses
physiological signals (e.g., electrocardiograms and
respiration waveforms), and behavioral reactions
involving a keyboard or mouse movement (Alberdi,
2016). Stress can be relieved using luxury goods,
such as tobacco and coffee breaks, and activities such
as meditation
(Alberto, 2009), walking, listening to
music (Taelman, 2009), and looking at natural images
(Kotake, 2004; Ikei, 2013). The stress-relieving
effects of these methods have been demonstrated in
both actual experiences and research.
However, only workers who are able to interrupt
their work and take breaks voluntarily can use these
methods to relieve stress. Workers who accumulate
chronic stress tend to be poor at taking voluntary
breaks; therefore, spontaneous stress-relief methods
could be ineffective for this group. Moreover,
because it is common for workers to be unaware of
their own stress levels, they often fail to suspend their
work spontaneously and take stress-relieving action.
Therefore, there is a high demand for methods that
relieve stress effectively while allowing individuals
to continue working.
We proposed a stress-relief method that did not
disturb work. To relieve stress during work, we
presented participants with tactile stimuli to their feet,
because this conforms to the workplace environment
and does not interrupt tasks. Visual and auditory
stimuli can interfere with work, and olfactory stimuli
are difficult to present selectively because of
diffusibility; therefore, these stimuli were unsuitable
for our proposed approach. Of the tactile stimuli, we
chose to expose participants’ feet to heat during work.
The reason for this choice was that the skin
temperature of the foot decreases with mental stress
(Elam, 1987), and we hypothesized that the opposite
would be true.
2 RELATED WORK
Research regarding stress is largely divided into that
involving stress detection and that examining stress
relief.
2.1 Stress Detection
Research examining stress detection is roughly divid-
Inazawa, M., Ban, Y., Fukui, R. and Warisawa, S.
The Stress Relief Effects of Foot Warming during Mental Workload.
DOI: 10.5220/0007368901470154
In Proceedings of the 12th International Joint Conference on Biomedical Engineering Systems and Technologies (BIOSTEC 2019), pages 147-154
ISBN: 978-989-758-353-7
Copyright
c
2019 by SCITEPRESS Science and Technology Publications, Lda. All rights reserved
147
ed into research to calculate stress using biological
information with electrocardiography and pulse
waves as indicators, and that exploring the extraction
of feature quantity using behavior such as keyboard
typing and mouse and body movement. In addition,
some studies have confirmed the usefulness of
psychological questionnaires in stress detection
(Alberdi, 2016).
With respect to biological information, it is clear
that the RR interval (RRI), which is the R wave
interval in the electrocardiogram, and its variance
value decrease in stress tasks
(Taelman, 2009). The
Cardiac Sympathetic Index (CSI) and Cardiac Vagal
Index (CVI), which are calculated from the RRI
Lorentz plot and were developed by Toichi
(Toichi,
1997), are stress indices. Moreover, Hayashi
confirmed an increase in CSI scores and decline in the
CVI following a stress task, while the opposite
occurred after a relaxing presentation
(Hayashi, 2018).
The skin temperature in the body’s extremities is also
an indicator of stress. Zenju reported that the
difference between the skin temperature on the nose
and the forehead increased with pleasant stimulation
via classical music and decreased with deep
stimulation via scratching sounds
(Zenju, 2004).
In addition, behavioral reactions involve using
input feature quantities of keyboards and mice (Vizer,
2009; Salmeron, 2014), and psychological
questionnaires include the Perceived Stress Scale
developed by Cohen (Cohen, 1983) and the Stress
Response Scale 18 developed by Suzuki
(Suzuki,
1997).
In this study, we selected biological information
that could be obtained under experimental conditions
while participants were in a sitting position, even
during cognitive tasks. Because the RRI and CVI
scores increased during relaxation following the
cognitive task in our preliminary experiments, we
selected them as indicators of stress. We paid
particular attention to change rates in these indicators
before and after stress relief.
2.2 Stress Relief
As this study aimed to reduce stress in the workplace,
we examined related research examining stress relief
from the perspective of methods that can and cannot
be performed in the work environment. Those that
cannot be performed in the workplace but exert
stress-relieving effects include forest bathing
(Hansmann, 2007; Stigsdotter, 2017) and exercise
(Guszkowska, 2004).
Stress relief in work environments is based on
active behavior or passive presentation. Mindfulness
therapy is an example of an actively performed stress-
relief method, and numerous studies have
demonstrated that this method exerted stress-
relieving effects (Grossman, 2004). However,
Bohlmeijer et al. showed that mindfulness did not
alleviate depression caused by chronic stress
(Bohlmeijer, 2010).
Examples of the stress mitigation method
involving passive presentation include viewing
natural images (Kotake, 2004; Ikei, 2013), listening
to classical music (Yuko, 2011), and inducing
respiration (Zhu, 2017). However, these methods
stimulate visual and auditory senses, which are
important in the workplace, and could affect task
performance.
Therefore, we selected foot warming as a stress-
relief method that did not interrupt tasks and
conformed to the work environment. The reason why
we chose this approach was that exposing the feet to
stimuli should not interrupt desk work. Moreover,
because it is clear that foot temperature decreases
with stress (Elam, 1987), we hypothesized that the
opposite would be true. In fact, some studies have
succeeded in making the impression of drink better
by warming user’s nose, which is based on the
opposite fact that a nose temperature decreases in
stress condition (Chie Suzuki, 2014).
2.3 Stress Relief via Warming
Previous studies examining thermal and relaxing
effects have verified the effect of wrist warming on
stress reduction following cognitive tasks (Ayami,
2017). Although a significant difference in
psychological questionnaire scores was observed
according to exposure to warming, there was no
significant difference in biological information.
Furthermore, presenting thermal stimuli to the wrist
could inhibit task execution in work environments
that involve mainly personal computer use. In
addition, this previous research did not explain how
thermal stimuli affected users task performance.
Therefore, in the current study, we exposed
participants’ feet to heat to relieve stress, as we aimed
to relieve stress in the workplace without hindering
task performance. Moreover, we verified that our
method was compatible with the work environment
by examining the influence of foot warming on task
performance.
BIOSIGNALS 2019 - 12th International Conference on Bio-inspired Systems and Signal Processing
148
3 STUDY 1: THE
STRESS-RELIEVING EFFECT OF
FOOT WARMING
We conducted an experiment to determine the extent
of stress reduction resulting from foot warming
during a task, using biological information. Nine
participants (seven men and two women, aged 2225
years) were included in the experiment. To examine
the stress-reducing effect of warming, we analyzed
change rates for the RRI and CVI. In previous studies,
RRI compared between breaks and a Mensa test
changed by 0.968, from 0.816 ± 0.13 to 0.790 ± 0.13
(Taelman, 2009). In the transition from a stress state
to a relaxed state, we expected the RRI to be 1.033
times the reciprocal number. Therefore, we
hypothesized that RRI would change by 1.033
following foot warming.
3.1 Foot-Warming Method
Prior to Study 1, we conducted a preliminary
experiment to examine the effect of warming. In this
experiment, we exposed participants’ toes to a 35° C
thermal stimulus, using the Peltier element. However,
there was no trend in biological information. We
concluded that the reason for this result was that the
temperature was low, and the size of the stimulated
area was narrow. Thus, in Study 1, we used a Hottie
(Figure 1) to warm the entire foot at a high
temperature. By injecting boiling water into the water
injection port, the contact temperature with the foot
increased to 50° C at maximum, as shown in Figure
2, and it is possible to provide a high temperature and
thermal stimulus to the large area of the foot, which
was not possible with the Peltier element in the
preliminary experiment. Regarding the distribution of
the presentation temperature, the foot temperature
was highest, and the heel temperature was lowest.
Figure 1: Experimental environment and Hottie.
Figure 2: Contact temperature with the foot.
3.2 Procedure
We performed experiments according to the
flowchart shown in Figure 3. The experimental
environment is shown in Figure 1.
Figure 3: Experimental flow.
3.2.1 5-Min Rest
Participants rested for 5 min before performing a
cognitive task. The aim of this stage was to collect
biological information while they were a calm state.
3.2.2 30-Min Cognitive Task
Participants performed an n-back cognitive task for
30 min (Kirchner, 1958). Continuous numerical
values were displayed on the computer, and
participants provided n numbers before the displayed
numerical values (Figure 4). In this study, n = 2, and
a number between 1 and 9 was presented every
second.
There were two reasons for choosing the n-
back task. The first was that it was necessary to set a
problem with a high degree of difficulty. We had
conducted experiments using another task, but it was
too easy for participants, and task performance did
not change; therefore, we could not verify the effect
of foot warming on task performance. The second
reason was that it was necessary to impose fatigue on
participants, given the work environment. In the n-
back task, participants use working memory, which is
needed for desk work. Participants provide numbers
in the task, using the keyboard, and have not been
informed about the task duration.
The Stress Relief Effects of Foot Warming during Mental Workload
149
Figure 4: The n-back task. The upper image shows the
numbers displayed, and the lower image shows the numbers
provided by participants.
3.2.3 10-Min Foot Warming (Temperature
Presentation Time)
Warming was performed 20 min after the cognitive
task, using a device that connected a silicone tube to
a Hottie via a funnel (Figure 1). During the
presentation of the temperature stimulus, 1 liter of
water was poured manually into the funnel. The
temperature of the water differed between the
experimental conditions described in Section 3.3.
Participants were not informed as to when the
temperature presentation would begin.
3.2.4 Questionnaire
After completing the cognitive task, the participants
completed a questionnaire concerning the
temperature stimulus. The questionnaire items
pertained to temperature sensation,
pleasure/discomfort, and a free response.
Temperature sensation was measured in seven grades
ranging from cold to hot, and comfort was measured
in seven grades ranging from discomfort to pleasure.
3.3 Experimental Condition Design
To compare the effects of the thermal stimulus, each
participant completed two types of task. One task
involved a treatment condition in which boiled water
was poured at “temperature presentation time, at a
maximum of 50° C. The other task involved a control
condition in which water was poured at room
temperature (approximately 30° C ) at “temperature
presentation time.
Each participant completed the two tasks in 1
day. Participants took a break of at least an hour
between tasks. The order of the tasks was
counterbalanced across participants. The participants
received no information about the experimental
conditions.
3.4 Biological Information
It is necessary to measure increases and decreases in
stress to verify the effect of stress relief resulting from
foot warming. We referred to previous studies and
preliminary experiments and used the RRI and CVI
scores as indicators of stress.
3.4.1 Electrocardiogram
Electrocardiogram waveforms were acquired using
an electrocardiogram sensor. The RRI is an indicator
of stress. If the variance value of RRI is large, the
individual is in a relaxed state, and when it is small,
the individual is in a state of concentration or stress
(Taelman, 2009). SCI and CVI scores, which are
calculated via Lorentz plot, are also stress indices
(Toichi, 1997; Hayashi, 2018). Of the information
obtained from these indices, RRI and CVI were used
to indicate stress in this experiment.
3.5 Results
RRI is the average value for 1 min, and CVI is
calculated using the Lorentz plot of the RRI for 1 min.
In addition, the number of correct answers per minute
of the n-back task was defined and calculated as task
performance. Consequently, the RRI increased after
foot warming (Figure 5). We calculated the number
of times each index increased 5 min before
(approximately 20 min from the start of the cognitive
task) and after temperature presentation. The average
change rates for the RRI, CVI, task performance, and
standard errors under in the warming condition were
as follows: RRI: 1.045 ± 0.0106, CVI: 0.966 ± 0.0141,
and task performance: 0.912 ± 0.0291. The results
observed for the control condition were as follows:
RRI: 1.019 ± 0.00679, CVI: 0.976 ± 0.0111, and task
performance: 0.980 ± 0.0368 (Table 1, Figure 6). In
addition, the rates of change in the warming and
control conditions for each indicator were subjected
to a two-tailed t test with correspondence (assuming
that the population variance was equal). The p values
were as follows: RRI: .07, CVI: .60, task
performance: .19. The questionnaire results showed
that the score for discomfort/pleasure resulting from
warming was 3.8 ± 0.8 out of 7,0, and that for the
control condition was 3.4 ± 0.4 out of 7.0. Further, the
temperature sensation scores were 6.8 ± 0.2 for
warming and 3.8 ± 0.37 in the control condition.
BIOSIGNALS 2019 - 12th International Conference on Bio-inspired Systems and Signal Processing
150
Figure 5: One participants result for the RR interval.
Table 1: Change rates observed after stimulus presentation.
Figure 6: Change rate after stimulus presentation.
3.6 Discussion
We sought to determine whether the two-back task
induced stress, by examining task performance
reduction. Previous studies showed that stress
induced working memory loss (schoofs, 2008).
Therefore, two-back task performance reduction
could be considered to reflect stress. In the current
study, means and standard errors for the first and last
5 min of performance were 52.322 ± 0.853 and
41.753 ± 1.112, respectively. The t-test (one sided, no
correspondence, with equal dispersion) p value was
<.01, suggesting that the two-back task induced stress.
Comparison of the warming and control
conditions showed that the increase in the RRI was
greater in the warming condition, relative to that
observed in the control condition (p = .074). As the
RRI increased by 1.045 ± 0.0106 following foot
warming, the change in the RRI resulting from
warming was similar to that resulting from rest.
Regarding CVI, although the p value was
nonsignificant (p = .52), CVI scores in the warming
condition decreased to a level lower than those
observed in the control condition. Because CVI
scores represent the function of the parasympathetic
nerve, they indicated that hyperthermia weakened the
action of this nerve. The reason for this finding could
have been that the participants were awakened by foot
warming.
Regarding performance, although it declined in
both conditions, the reduction in performance was
greater in the warming condition, relative to that
observed in the control condition (p = .19). This
decline could have occurred because working
memory decreased in the two-back task. In addition,
the reason for the greater decline in performance in
the warming condition, relative to that in the control
condition, could have been that participants in the
experimental group became distracted. In the
warming condition, the temperature of the foot
contact position reached a maximum of 5C, and
many participants reported that the stimulus
temperature was high.
Participants’ questionnaire responses regarding
temperature sensation were as expected, in that they
felt warmth upon exposure to heat and did not feel
warmth or coldness in the control condition. In the
responses regarding pleasure/discomfort, there was
little difference between conditions. Notably, one
participant answers two out of seven grades
(discomfort) in the questionnaire of
pleasant/discomfort after warming condition,
although his or her RRI changed higher than control
condition. That is, the RRI showed a greater increase
in the warming condition, relative to that observed in
the control condition. Therefore, although the RRI
reaction usually occurs in a relaxed state, this
subjective evaluation was contradictory.
Based on the above results, the p value for the
increase in the RRI was relatively low with foot
warming, and task performance declined. In addition,
the results showed possible conflict between the RRI
result and subjective evaluation. However, there was
some doubt as to whether the RRI increase occurred
as an effect of foot warming. There are three reasons
for this doubt. The first was that, after the experiment,
some participants reported that the feeling of water-
induced pressure on the foot was pleasant. The second
reason was that some participants RRIs increased in
both the warming and control conditions (Figure 7).
The third reason was that the RRI increased before
the temperature increase in one instance (Figure 8).
The Stress Relief Effects of Foot Warming during Mental Workload
151
Therefore, it is possible that the RRI increased
because of the pressure resulting from the water,
rather than foot warming. To clarify this issue, the
factors underlying the RRI increase were clarified in
Study 2.
Figure 7: One participants RR interval.
Figure 8: The RR interval and Hottie temperature.
4 STUDY 2: CLARIFICATION OF
THE STRESS-RELIEF FACTOR
To determine whether the increase in the RRI in
Study 1 occurred because of foot warming or the
pressure of the water, Study 2 included the following
two conditions. The experimental procedure and
environment were similar to those of Study 1. Nine
participants (six men and three women aged 2225
years) were included in Study 2A, and 10 participants
(eight men and two women aged 2225 years) were
included in Study 2B.
4.1 Foot Warming Method 2a
We used a cardboard form, as shown in Figure 9, to
avoid exposing participants’ feet to the pressure of the
water. The contact temperature with the foot in this
condition is shown in Figure 10. In Study 1, it reached
50° C, and we could not compare the temperature
with only that in the condition in Study 1. Therefore,
we established the condition in Study 2B.
Figure 9: Cardboard mould in the Hottie.
Figure 10: Contact temperature with the foot.
4.2 Foot-Warming Method 2b
To verify that there was an increase in the RRI in the
warming condition with the pressure experienced in
Study 2A, the temperature of the water was adjusted
to that presented in Figure 10. The equipment and
procedures used in Study 2 were the same as those
used in Study 1.
4.3 Results
Each indicator was calculated using the method used
in Study 1. Table 2 shows the increase in the RRI, the
CVI, and task performance in the warming and
control conditions in Studies 2A and 2B; the results
of Study 1 are included for comparison. Figures 11
and 12 show the results of the increase values for the
indices used in the warming and control conditions in
Studies 2A and B. The results obtained via
questionnaire are shown in Figure 14.
BIOSIGNALS 2019 - 12th International Conference on Bio-inspired Systems and Signal Processing
152
Table 2: Change rates after stimulus presentation.
Figure 11: Change rates after stimulus presentation in Study
2A.
Figure 12: Change rates after stimulus presentation in Study
2B.
Table 3: Questionnaire results.
4.4 Discussion
RRI in the warming condition showed a greater
increase relative to that observed in the control
condition in Study 2A, but there was almost no
difference between conditions in Study 2B (p = .85).
In a previous study (Taelman, 2009), weak warming
and a break in stimulus presentation increased the
RRI to the same extent; therefore, weak warming and
pressure impression presentation did not affect the
RRI. Looking at the questionnaire of the temperature
sense, the warmth is stronger in the order of STUDY
1, 2 - a, 2 - b, and the rate of increase of RRI also
follows this order. So it is suggested that RRI
increases due to feet warming, and the stronger the
temperature sense, the more RRI rise.
CVI scores showed similar trends in Studies 1
and 2. Therefore, regardless of the presentation of
pressure, CVI scores decreased following warming,
suggesting that warming reduced CVI scores
regardless of the extent of the temperature or pressure.
The findings suggested that the RRI increased
and CVI decreased with warming. Further, as CVI
scores decreased, parasympathetic nervous activity
decreased, and sympathetic nervous activity was
considered dominant (Toichi, 1997). Moreover,
because the RRI increased, participants were at rest,
even though they were in a state of
stress/concentration.
The results of the questionnaire and temperature
sensation was the same way as in STUDY 1. With
respect to pleasantness, two participants in Study 2A
and three people in Study 2B showed subjective
evaluation in conflict with the RRI during warming.
Moreover, task performance showed a greater
increase with warming, relative to that observed in the
control condition, in Study 2A (p = .17). However,
there was little difference between conditions in
Study 2B (p = 1.00). Through STUDY 1 - 2, the task
performance was lower only with strong warming
conditions (STUDY 1). Therefore, foot warming
affected task performance only when the warming
temperature was high.
5 CONCLUSIONS
In this study, we examined foot warming as a stress-
relief method that did not disturb task performance.
In Study 1, we warmed participants’ feet during a
two-back task. The results showed that the RRI
increased and CVI scores decreased with foot
warming, regardless of pressure. In addition,
sympathetic nervous activity resulting from foot
warming was similar to that observed with a break
from stimulus presentation. Moreover, the RRI and
subjective evaluation were in conflict. Future
research is required to clarify the causes of
sympathetic dominance and verify reactions with
cold stimulus presentation.
The Stress Relief Effects of Foot Warming during Mental Workload
153
REFERENCES
National Police Agency, January, 2017. Suicide situation in
Heisei 26, https://www.npa.go.jp/toukei/index.htm.
Yuanyuan, Bi, et al, 2013. Sex difference in stress reactivity
of hippocampal BDNF in mice are associated with the
female preponderance of decreased locomotor activity
in response to restraint stress, Annual Meeting of the
Japanese Society of Toxicology.
Alberdi, et al, 2016: 49-75. Towards an automatic early
stress recognition system for office environments based
on multimodal measurements: A review. Journal of
Biomedical Informatics 59.
Alberto, Chiesa, and Alessandro, Serretti, 2009: 593-600.
Mindfulness-based stress reduction for stress
management in healthy people: A review and meta-
analysis, Journal of Alternative and Complementary
Medicine 15, 5.
Taelman, Joachim, et al, 2009. Influence of mental stress
on heart rate and heart rate variability, 4th European
Conference of the International Federation for Medical
and Biological Engineering, Springer, Berlin,
Heidelberg.
Toichi, Motomi, et al, 1997: 79-84. A new method of
assessing cardiac autonomic function and its
comparison with spectral analysis and coefficient of
variation of RR interval, Journal of the Autonomic
Nervous System 62, 1-2.
Hayashi, Rina, et al, 2018. Proposal of therapy robot system
extracting stress relaxing effects according to the stress
state, Proceedings of the 2018 JSME Conference on
Robotics and Mechatronics No. 18-2, Japan.
Zenju, Hideyuki, et al, 2004. Estimation of unpleasant and
pleasure states by nasal thermogram, IEEJ, EIS, 124,
1.
Vizer, et al, 2009: 870-886. Automated stress detection
using keystroke and linguistic features: An exploratory
study, International Journal of Human-Computer
Studies 67, 10.
Salmeron-Majadas, Sergio, Santos, Olga C., and Boticario,
Jesus G, 2014: 691-700. An evaluation of mouse and
keyboard interaction indicators towards non-intrusive
and low cost affective modeling in an educational
context, Procedia Computer Science 35.
Cohen, Sheldon, Kamarck, Tom, and Mermelstein, Robin,
1983: 385-396. A global measure of perceived
stress, Journal of Health and Social Behavior.
Suzuki, Shinichi, et al, 1997: 22-29. Development of a new
psychological Stress Response Scale (SRS-18) and
investigation of the reliability and the validity, Japanese
Journal of Behavioral Medicine 4,1.
Grossman, Paul, et al, 2004: 35-43. Mindfulness-based
stress reduction and health benefits: A meta-
analysis, Journal of Psychosomatic Research 57, 1.
Bohlmeijer, Ernst, et al, 2010: 539-544. The effects of
mindfulness-based stress reduction therapy on mental
health of adults with a chronic medical disease: A meta-
analysis, Journal of Psychosomatic Research 68,6.
Hansmann, Ralf, Stella-Maria Hug, and Klaus Seeland,
2007: 213-225. Restoration and stress relief through
physical activities in forests and parks, Urban Forestry
& Urban Greening 6, 4.
Stigsdotter, Ulrika Karlsson, et al, 2017: 1-15. Forest design
for mental health promotionusing perceived sensory
dimensions to elicit restorative responses, Landscape
and Urban Planning 160.
Elam, M., and Wallin, B. G, 1987: 429-431. Skin blood
flow responses to mental stress in man depend on body
temperature, ActaPhysiologica129.3.
Chie Suzuki et al, 2014. Affecting tumbler: Affecting our
flavor perception with thermal feedback, 11th
Advances in Computer Entertainment Technology
Conference.
Ayami, Kasamatsu, 2017. The stress relief effects of wrist
warming after mental workload. The Society of Digital
Information and Wireless Communications.
Kirchner, Wayne K, 1958: 352. Age differences in short-
term retention of rapidly changing information, Journal
of Experimental Psychology 55, 4.
Kotake, Noriko, et al, 2004: 1-10. Research on the
relaxation effect of music therapy, Journal of the
Faculty of Global Communication, Siebold University
of Nagasaki, 5.
Ikei, Harumi, et al, 2013: 97-103. Physiological relaxation
of viewing rose flowers in high school
students, Japanese Journal of Physiological
Anthropology 18,3.
Yuko, Tsunetsugu, et al, 2011: 670-676. Psychological
relaxation effect of forest therapy. Japan Hygiene
Journal 66,4.
Zhu, Bin, et al, 2017. Designing, prototyping and
evaluating digital mindfulness applications: A case
study of mindful breathing for stress
reduction." Journal of Medical Internet Research 19, 6.
Guszkowska, Monika, 2004 : 611-620. Effects of exercise
on anxiety, depression and mood." Psychiatria
Polska 38,4.
Schoofs, et al, 2008: 643-653. Psychosocial stress induces
working memory impairments in an n-back paradigm.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 33,5.
BIOSIGNALS 2019 - 12th International Conference on Bio-inspired Systems and Signal Processing
154