4 CONCLUSIONS
This initial work has demonstrated the presence of
sound-gustatory synesthesia in a typical coffeehouse
setting. We have seen that the speed of the music
being heard may alter the perception of the coffee
being tasted. In particular, majority of the
participants detected Sweetness when Slow music
was played, and Bitterness when Normal and Fast
music were played. Participants also perceived
Sourness and Saltiness, and the perception of
Sourness increased with the speed of the music
track, even when sour and salty components were
not present in their drinks.
We can improve the study by including baseline
measurements for taste (water) and sound (no
music). Stafford, Fernandes, and Agobiani (2012)
have shown that the presence of music altered taste
perception, serving as a “distraction” in the same
way as shadow multitasking.
To extend the previous sound-gustatory
synesthesia research, we can also have participants
ask if they inherently “like” or “dislike” the drink
and the music tested, to investigate associations
between hedonic and sensory perception of coffee.
Lastly, we can also look for possible effects of
respondent traits such as gender and age.
We expect multisensory, interconnected
technology in the Internet of Things to spread the
experience of synesthesia within a population, with
Big Data enabling researchers to detect and measure
synesthesia much more accurately.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We thank the students and faculty of Mapúa
University for participating in the study, the
Yuchengco Innovation Center for the resources in
preparing this manuscript, and our colleagues and
loved ones for their support. We also thank the
organizers of the IoTBDS 2019 Conference for
accepting this work and for the financial support.
REFERENCES
Bankieris, K. and Simner, J., 2013. Sound symbolism in
synesthesia: Evidence from a lexical-gustatory
synesthete. Neurocase, 20(6), pp.640-651.
Brang, D. and Ramachandran, V. S., 2011. Survival of the
synesthesia gene: Why do people hear colors and taste
words? PLoS biology, 9(11), e1001205.
Colizoli, O., Murre, J.M. and Rouw, R., 2013. A taste for
words and sounds: a case of lexical-gustatory and
sound-gustatory synesthesia. Frontiers in psychology,
4, p.775.
Crisinel, A.S. and Spence, C., 2009. Implicit association
between basic tastes and pitch. Neuroscience letters,
464(1), pp.39-42.
Gallace, A., Boschin, E. and Spence, C., 2011. On the
taste of “Bouba” and “Kiki”: An exploration of word–
food associations in neurologically normal
participants. Cognitive Neuroscience, 2(1), pp.34-46.
Hillis, J.M., et al., 2002. Combining sensory information:
mandatory fusion within, but not between, senses.
Science, 298(5598), pp.1627-1630.
Jazz and Blues Experience, 2016. New York Jazz Lounge –
Bar Jazz Classics. [Online] Available from:
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=_sI_Ps7JSEk
[Accessed September 2018].
Kantono, K., et al., 2016. Listening to music can influence
hedonic and sensory perceptions of gelati. Appetite,
100, pp.244-255.
Knöferle, K. and Spence, C., 2012. Crossmodal
correspondences between sounds and tastes.
Psychonomic bulletin and review, pp.1-15.
Mesz, B., Sigman, M. and Trevisan, M., 2012. A
composition algorithm based on crossmodal taste-
music correspondences. Frontiers in Human
Neuroscience, 6, p.71.
Reinoso Carvalho, F., et al., 2016. Music influences
hedonic and taste ratings in beer. Frontiers in
psychology, 7, p.636.
Spence, C., 2015. Multisensory flavor perception. Cell,
161(1), pp.24-35.
Stafford, L.D., Fernandes, M. and Agobiani, E., 2012.
Effects of noise and distraction on alcohol perception.
Food Quality and Preference, 24(1), pp.218-224.
van Campen, C., 2009. The Hidden Sense: On Becoming
Aware of Synesthesia1. Revista Digital de
Tecnologias Cognitivas, 1, pp.1-13.
Woods, A. T., et al., 2011. Effect of background noise on
food perception. Food Quality and Preference, 22(1),
pp.42-47.