A (Technologically Enhanced) Sound Education: Implementation,
Experimentation and Analysis of Raymond Murray Schafer’s Exercises
Veronica Curioni, Luca A. Ludovico and Giorgio Presti
LIM – Laboratorio di Informatica Musicale, Dipartimento di Informatica “Giovanni Degli Antoni”,
Universit
`
a degli Studi di Milano, Via G. Celoria 18, 20133 Milano, Italy
Keywords:
Listening, Sound, Education, Schafer, Technology.
Abstract:
Raymond Murray Schafer is considered one of the leading experts in the field of music ecology. In one of
his works, he proposed specific exercises to encourage listening awareness in young students. This paper
aims to describe an experimental activity in which three exercises extracted from Schafer’s work have been
implemented in the form of computer-based tools, so as to be administered both in a traditional and in a
technologically enhanced way. The experimentation has been conducted on 233 primary school students,
showing to what extent the adoption of technology can be applied to listening attention and awareness.
1 INTRODUCTION
The adoption of technology and computer-based ap-
proaches in teaching has neither positive nor nega-
tive connotation per se; rather, technological advance-
ments offer tools that, when profitably used, can im-
prove the didactic experience and the learning pro-
cess. Scientific literature contains many examples of
technologies profitably usable in teaching, but it also
reveals how their misuse may lead to negative effects
(Ellis, 1973; Kay, 1996; Ribble and Bailey, 2004).
This work starts from a very general research
question: is the administration of educational expe-
riences through technology more effective than a tra-
ditional one? Needless to say, this question is hard to
answer in general terms, so we restricted the investi-
gation field to a narrow context. In particular, we tried
to understand how children relate to the surrounding
sound environment, and how technology may help
them in acquiring soundscape awareness. To this end,
we applied a quantitative methodology to the admin-
istration and evaluation of specific exercises, so as to
extract objective data on the effects produced on chil-
dren by both traditional methods and technological
tools.
The remainder of this paper is organized as fol-
lows: Section 2 will introduce the theoretical bases
that inspired our research; Section 3 will describe the
state of the art about technologies that foster acoustic
awareness and listening skills; Section 4 will present
the experiences we administered to our control and
experimental group; Section 5 will provide details
about the experimental protocol; Section 6 will dis-
cuss the results we achieved; finally, Section 7 will
present our conclusions.
2 A SOUND EDUCATION
Raymond Murray Schafer, born in Canada in 1933, is
a multifaceted man: composer, librettist, pedagogue,
writer, educator, and environmentalist. He is best
known for his interest towards acoustic ecology, a
discipline that studies the relationship between hu-
man beings and their environment mediated through
sound (Wrightson, 2000). In 1966, he formed a team
at Simon Fraser University, Vancouver to investigate
themes related to acoustic ecology. Among his most
relevant publications, it is worth mentioning (Schafer,
1977), (Schafer, 1986), and (Schafer, 1992).
The latter work, titled A Sound Education: 100
Exercises in Listening and Soundmaking”, aims to
improve the listening skills of children, stimulating
attention and awareness towards surrounding sound-
scapes. Since in his opinion the modern civilized
world is becoming deaf because of the noise, the au-
thor stresses the importance of re-education to sound.
In the introduction, he says: “I believe that the way to
improve the world’s soundscape is quite simple. We
must learn how to listen.
As a possible solution, Schafer proposes a num-
ber of exercises dealing with soundmaking and listen-
Curioni, V., Ludovico, L. and Presti, G.
A (Technologically Enhanced) Sound Education: Implementation, Experimentation and Analysis of Raymond Murray Schafer’s Exercises.
DOI: 10.5220/0007766004730480
In Proceedings of the 11th International Conference on Computer Supported Education (CSEDU 2019), pages 473-480
ISBN: 978-989-758-367-4
Copyright
c
2019 by SCITEPRESS Science and Technology Publications, Lda. All rights reserved
473
ing, gradually moving from ear cleaning (the ability to
distinguish sound from noise) to the design of sound-
scapes. All training activities do not require previ-
ous skills, and have not been conceived to be per-
formed systematically from start to finish, rather they
are intended for casual performance as the occasion
demands. In this sense, our approach i.e. the selec-
tion of a very limited number of exercises to be pro-
posed in a classroom environment – does not conflict
with the pedagogical principles enunciated by Schafer
(see Section 4).
3 STATE OF THE ART
In this section we will provide a short overview on
research initiatives that make use of technology to
encourage acoustic awareness and develop listening
skills.
First, it is worth mentioning those initiatives col-
lectively referred to as ear training, aiming to iden-
tify, solely by hearing, typical elements of music,
such as pitches, intervals, melody, chords, rhythms,
etc. Ear training is considered one of the components
of formal musical education. An early application of
technology to ear training is reported in (Hofstetter,
1975): it describes an experiment conducted with an
ear-training class at the University of Delaware in or-
der to determine the impact on student achievement
in harmonic dictation.
In more recent times, McGill University devel-
oped a computer-aided system for timbral ear train-
ing. The system lets students develop their sensitiv-
ity to timbral changes and their memory for timbre
through a set of specific listening tasks of increas-
ing complexity, thus providing an objective and effec-
tive training method for the development of listening
skills (Quesnel and Woszczyk, 1994).
As the use of computers became widespread, a
growing number of institutions integrated ear-training
computer-assisted instruction (CAI) into their music-
theory programs. A comprehensive and systematic
review dating back to late 90’s, comparing more than
60 ear-training CAI programs, is contained in (Span-
gler, 1999).
Finally, it is worth mentioning those pedagogi-
cal approaches and computer-based applications that
take full benefit from multi-layer music description,
namely from an integrated and synchronized repre-
sentation of score symbols, audio and video tracks,
alternative notation, and so on. Examples include
first-sight reading, collaborative music performance,
score following, etc. Some case studies are reported
in (Barat
`
e and Ludovico, 2012) and (Barat
`
e et al.,
2014).
4 THE SELECTED EXERCISES
Within the corpus of exercises proposed in (Schafer,
1992), we chose three activities. The basic idea was
to administer them to primary school students both
in a traditional form and using technology-powered
tools (e.g, a personal computer, a sound reproduction
system, etc.) in order to track the differences in per-
formance between the control group and the experi-
mental group.
The criteria we used to select exercises included:
Heterogeneity in the skills to be developed in chil-
dren;
The possibility to implement a suitable and easy-
to-use tool to enhance the traditional administra-
tion model;
The need to overcome a number of logistical
constraints typical of a school environment (a
few hours to conduct the experimentation, limited
spaces, available technological devices, etc.).
Below, we provide a short description of the se-
lected exercises and their goals within our pedagog-
ical experimentation. Regarding the commonalities,
all activities aim to develop listening and attention
skills, and are proposed in a collaborative environ-
ment. Please note that the administration modes will
be different from an exercise to another, as explained
in Section 5.
4.1 Exercise 1
Exercise 1 aims to enhance the awareness of young
listeners about the soundscape around them. Schafer
describes this activity as follows: “WRITE DOWN
ALL THE SOUNDS YOU HEAR. Take a few min-
utes to do this; then, if you are in a group, read all
the lists out loud, noting differences. Everyone will
have a different list, for listening is very personal;
and though some lists may be longer than others, all
answers will be correct. This simple exercise can
be performed anywhere by anyone. It would be a
good idea to try it several times in contrasting envi-
ronments in order to get into the habit of listening.
This exercise, fundamental to raise attention towards
the soundscape, is actually the first one both in our
experimentation and in Schafer’s book.
In our experimentation, children were asked to list
all the sounds they are able to detect around them,
when immersed in a natural soundscape or in an arti-
ficial one.
CSEDU 2019 - 11th International Conference on Computer Supported Education
474
Figure 1: Indoor and outdoor listening activities for Exer-
cise 1.
As for the traditional administration, children
were taken to 4 different indoor and outdoor school
environments (hallway, amphitheater, courtyard, and
sports field) and invited to listen carefully for 10 min-
utes (see Figure 1). Then, they were asked to list on a
paper form all the recognized sound elements, distin-
guishing between sounds perceived as far and close
only to keep children concentrated on their task. Dur-
ing the observation phase, it was noted that sounds
could be clustered into 4 groups: classroom sounds
(teacher and student voices, chalk on the blackboard,
etc.); animal sounds (singing birds, barking dogs,
buzzing flies, etc.); street traffic sounds (cars, buses,
bicycles, etc.); sounds of nature (whistling of the
wind, rustling of the grass, falling rain, etc.).
As for the enhanced mode, children had to fill
in the same card, but they were asked to listen to
4 soundscapes coming from an amplification sys-
tem. Scenarios had been previously created by editing
recorded live sounds or elements from sound libraries.
Audio tracks deliberately mixed stimuli that could
hardly coexist in a natural acoustic environment, but
supposedly familiar to students: noises out of school,
animal sounds, house soundscapes, metallic sounds,
water-related noises, farm sounds, and urban noises.
4.2 Exercise 2
Exercise 2 seeks to investigate the concept of synaes-
thesia between sound and color. This subject has been
thoroughly investigated from different points of view
artistic, psychological, clinical, perceptive, etc.
and further information can be retrieved in (Vernon,
1930), (Zilczer, 1987), and (Neufeld et al., 2012),
to cite but a few. Concerning the correlation be-
tween color, music, and emotion, it is worth citing
(Palmer et al., 2013), who scientifically proved the
existence of an instinctive connection, a sort of “emo-
tional palette”, between some music and colors.
The activity proposed to children corresponds to
Exercise 41 from (Schafer, 1992): “Do sounds have
colours? For some people they do. Discuss what
colours some of the sounds in your collection might
be. Why?” Specifically, children were asked to as-
sociate a number of music themes with colors picked
from a limited palette. In this case, the administra-
tion to the control group was carried out through a pa-
per form to be colored, while the experimental group
used a web interface (see Figure 2). For both groups
the palette included red, blue, yellow, green, orange,
fuchsia, white, and black. Such a simple palette in-
cluded 3 primary, 3 secondary and 2 neutral colors,
balanced in number and mixed in their order, so as
not to influence children in their choices.
Concerning music, we proposed 8 themes from
Sergei Prokofiev’s Peter and the Wolf, a sym-
phonic fairy tale for children where each character
in the story is associated with a particular instru-
ment/ensemble and a musical theme: bird (flute),
duck (oboe), cat (clarinet), grandfather (bassoon),
wolf (French horns), hunters (woodwinds and trum-
pet), gunshots (timpani and bass drum), Peter (string
instruments).
4.3 Exercise 3
Exercise 3 aims to refine the ability to listen to a sound
and mimic it. In (Schafer, 1992) such an activity is
proposed in Exercise 56: “Expression is trained by
imitation. Musicians know this and spend many hours
imitating musical sounds. But any sound can serve as
a model for imitation. Once I brought a set of bam-
boo chimes into a class and asked the class to come
as close as possible to imitating the chimes with their
voices. We listened to the original, then tried to repro-
duce it, listened again, tried again, until we began to
comprehend all the parameters of this devious sound.
A (Technologically Enhanced) Sound Education: Implementation, Experimentation and Analysis of Raymond Murray Schafer’s Exercises
475
Veronica Curioni 858221 es.1
Sono un maschio
Sono una femmina
Figure 2: The paper form vs. the Web form for Exercise 2.
You could do the same with other sound-producers:
an alarm clock, a mechanical toy, the sweeping of a
broom, a child’s rattle, etc. The main thing is to keep
at it, listening and imitating, until you come as close
as you can.
This exercise was proposed in a classroom set-
ting by focusing on animal sounds, supposedly famil-
iar to children but not trivial to be imitated by voice.
Assignments were made by educators according to
rhythmic and melodic criteria, considering the age of
children: the hen for the first grade, the owl for the
second, the cat for the third, the sheep for the fourth
and the horse for the fifth. Children reproduced ani-
mal sounds during three sessions: a first time imme-
diately after listening to the original sound, the sec-
ond and the third time after some practice and ad-
ditional listening. The exercise was administered in
three ways:
1. Traditional mode Children carried out the task
cooperatively, in groups of 6 or 7 elements. Lis-
tening occurred via a loudspeaker. In order to
record the voice imitation, each group elected a
representative. There was no possibility to listen
to the recorded performance, so feedback was ex-
clusively based on schoolmates’ comments;
2. Individual enhanced mode Children performed
the activity independently. Listening was done
via headphones. Self-assessment was possible via
recording playback;
3. Cooperative enhanced mode Children were ad-
ministered listening and training activities both in-
dividually and cooperatively. In order to record
the final sound, each group elected a represen-
tative, who could improve his/her performance
through playback-based training and group ad-
vice. In this case, listening occurred both via
headphones and through loudspeakers.
5 EXPERIMENTAL PROTOCOL
In this section, we will provide details about the ex-
perimental protocol, focusing on the composition of
the sample and the administration modes. Results will
be presented and discussed in Section 6.
5.1 Sample Composition
Experimental activities involved 233 primary-school
students attending the Istituto Comprensivo “G.
Puecher”, Erba, Italy. The sample was composed by
43 children aged 6 to 7 from first grade classes, 51
aged 7 to 8 from the second grade, 43 aged 8 to 9
from the third grade, 53 aged 9 to 10 from the fourth
grade, and 43 aged 10 to 11 from the fifth grade. Each
grade included two classrooms, named A and B. The
number of participants actually varied from a session
to another due to absences.
The sample was quite heterogeneous concerning
home Countries: 56 students out of 233 (about 24%)
were foreigners. In detail: 6 children from Albania,
1 from Bangladesh, 2 from Benin, 1 from Brazil, 4
from Burkina Faso, 4 from China, 1 from Ecuador, 1
from Egypt, 1 from El Salvador, 1 from Philippines, 1
from Ghana, 2 from Guinea, 7 from Morocco, 1 from
Moldavia, 2 from Pakistan, 1 from Peru, 4 from Ro-
mania, 1 from Rwanda, 1 from Russia, 4 from Sene-
gal, 6 from Syria, 1 from Togo, 2 from Tunisia, and 1
from Turkey.
The sample was subdivided into two groups, on
the base of the attended classroom: 119 students
formed the control group (classes 1A to 5A), and 114
the experimental group (classes 1B to 5B).
5.2 Administration Modes
As mentioned before, the sample was given 3 activ-
ities based on (Schafer, 1992). Experiments were
administered in two ways: the control group experi-
enced traditional methods (e.g., natural acoustic envi-
ronment, use of physical media, etc.), while the exper-
imental group was asked to complete the same tasks
CSEDU 2019 - 11th International Conference on Computer Supported Education
476
Table 1: Soundscapes and participants for Exercise 1.
Soundscape ]5 was administered to both the experimental
and the control group.
Natural soundscapes
Place Number of participants
Backyard 112
Amphitheater 116
Entrance 111
Hallway 115
Artificial soundscapes
Place Number of participants
Soundscape ]1 102
Soundscape ]2 105
Soundscape ]3 107
Soundscape ]4 102
Soundscape ]5 (joint) 110 + 101
by using technological devices (e.g., computers, ear-
phones, etc.) and computer-supported tools such as
web applications.
Activities were conducted from April 23 to May
25, 2018 during lesson time, and lasted 15 to 60 min-
utes, depending on the type of exercise. Each class-
room took part in about 5 weekly sessions.
6 RESULTS
6.1 Exercise 1
The first exercise focused on the ability to perceive
the soundscape and recognize its elements. Details
about soundscapes and the number of participants are
provided in Table 1. During listening activities, chil-
dren were asked to take note of all perceived sounds.
For each landscape, either natural (control group) or
artificial (experimental group), the number of sounds
identified by each participant was recorded and com-
pared to the total number of elements in the sound-
scape.
The behavior across consecutive sessions was ob-
served, and the cumulative score of each group was
compared in a final joint session. The improvements
achieved by each group were noticeable only in the
first sessions. Moreover, the influence of administra-
tion modes was not relevant: during the final joint ses-
sion, attended by both the control and the experimen-
tal group, their scores did not show significant differ-
ences (see Figure 3). This outcome seems to suggest
that an effective auditory training can be performed
by means of both traditional methodologies and tech-
nological tools.
Traditional mode Augmented mode
0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0
Figure 3: Results achieved by the experimental and the con-
trol group during the joint session – Soundscape ]5 for Ex-
ercise 1.
Some interesting aspects emerged during the anal-
ysis of results. For instance, Soundscape ]5 contained
the sound of a metal teaspoon beating on a glass.
Even if this noise should be familiar, few children
were able to recognize it; rather, many of them iden-
tified it as the ringtone associated to a short message.
This is an example of schizophonia, a term coined by
Schafer to describe the splitting of an original sound
and its electroacoustic reproduction. Observing the
differences in listening abilities, children aged 8 to
11 obtained better scores than their younger school-
mates, and this seems to indicate an age-related in-
crease in attentional capacity. With respect to gender,
there was no substantial differences in the results of
the control group, whereas in the experimental group
females showed a greater ability in sound recognition
(in any case, median values are very similar).
Finally, two interesting aspects emerged from the
observation of the traditional administration. The first
effect can be described as sound perception driven by
sight: e.g., during a rainy day, many children recog-
nized the sound of raindrops even if doors and win-
dows were closed and that sound was not actually per-
ceivable. The second aspect concerns self-production
of sounds (e.g., coughing, sneezing, etc.) when ad-
ministered sounds had already been recognized: in
this way, students could take note of additional items
and artificially improve their performance.
6.2 Exercise 2
Concerning this exercise, focusing on the association
between music themes and colors, we have analyzed
two aspects: 1. The distribution of the colors chosen
by participants for each music theme; 2. The correla-
tions between music features and the color selected
by the majority of children. In particular, the fea-
tures were: key, average beats per measure (BPM),
register, median (B
M
) and interquartile range (B
IQR
)
A (Technologically Enhanced) Sound Education: Implementation, Experimentation and Analysis of Raymond Murray Schafer’s Exercises
477
0%
25%
50%
75%
100%
Peter Bird Duck Cat Wolf Grandfather Gunshots Hunters
21.9%
28.2%
25.1%
17.2%
19.9%
43.6%
35.5%
24.8%
0%
25%
50%
75%
100%
Peter Bird Duck Cat Wolf Grandfather Gunshots Hunters
19.1%
30.5%
22.6%
18.9%
30.6%
25.8%
37.6%
20.4%
Figure 4: Association of colors to music themes for Exer-
cise 2: traditional administration (top) vs. enhanced mode
(bottom). Top percentages are shown in the corresponding
colored area.
of the brightness, as defined in (Presti and Mauro,
2013). Differences in age have not been evaluated,
while the two administration modes have been ana-
lyzed separately. The values of the mentioned fea-
tures for each music theme are the following (TM =
traditional mode, EM = enhanced mode):
Peter (string quartet) – Key: C; Average BPM: 95;
Register: high; B
M
= 0.87; B
IQR
= 0.57;
Bird (flute) – Key: C; Average BPM: 144; Regis-
ter: high; B
M
= 1.25; B
IQR
= 0.7;
Cat (clarinet) Key: D; Average BPM: 42; Reg-
ister: low; B
M
= 0.49; B
IQR
= 0.34;
Duck (oboe) Key: E[; Average BPM: 84; Reg-
ister: high; B
M
= 1.3 B
IQR
= 0.42;
Grandfather (bassoon) Key: B[ min.; Average
BPM: 83; Register: low; B
M
= 0.38; B
IQR
= 0.29;
Wolf (French horns) Key: C min.; Average
BPM: 88; Register: low; B
M
= 0.48; B
IQR
= 0.17;
0%
25%
50%
75%
100%
6 7 8 9 10 11
years
colors
0%
25%
50%
75%
100%
6 7 8 9 10 11
years
colors
Figure 5: The choice of colors associated to the bird’s theme
in Exercise 2, organized by age (horizontal axis). The upper
diagram shows results for the traditional administration, the
lower one those for the enhanced mode.
Gunshots (timpani) Key: C; Average BPM: 87;
Register: low; B
M
= 0.33; B
IQR
= 0.18;
Hunters (woodwinds) Key: F min.; Average
BPM: 54; Register: low; B
M
= 0.45; B
IQR
= 0.35;
The associations of colors to music themes in the
two administration modes are reported in Figure 4.
White is more present in enhanced experiences, prob-
ably due to the presence of the color in the palette,
whereas in the traditional mode children had to inten-
tionally leave a space uncolored.
Often the most selected color was the same for the
two administration modes. But it is particularly in-
teresting the case of the bird’s theme, where yellow
stimulated complementary behaviors in the two ad-
ministration modes: in the traditional mode, it is the
preferred choice among the lateral age groups, while
in the enhanced mode it is the top choice for the cen-
tral bands (see Figure 5).
Observing the outcomes of the second analysis,
i.e. music features and colors, results are correlated
CSEDU 2019 - 11th International Conference on Computer Supported Education
478
with the data about brightness and music key. For
those themes where B
M
is low and the key is minor,
children mainly indicated darker colors; conversely,
for themes in major key and with a high value of B
M
,
they selected brighter colors. It is worth noting that,
during the final discussion, children asked for a wider
palette. This observation underlies the need to express
themselves in a more complex and precise way.
6.3 Exercise 3
This exercise focused on the ability to imitate sounds.
In this case, we adopted a slightly different experi-
mental protocol, introducing 3 administration modes,
as explained in Section 4.3.
Methodologically, for each age group a registra-
tion was chosen as the best instance of each mode to
be compared with the other modes. In the case of col-
laborative modes, the sound was chosen by the class
by vote, whereas in the individual mode it was the
sound automatically recognized as the most similar to
the original (a choice also subjectively validated by
teachers).
In addition, automatic analysis was used to com-
pare administration modes. For each recording, five
tests were performed taking the original animal sound
as the reference. The analytical process automat-
ically extracted indicators about timbral, rhythmic
and melodic characteristics. Specifically: timbre
was investigated by calculating the Mahalanobis dis-
tance (De Maesschalck et al., 2000) between the
Mel-frequency cepstral coefficients (MFCCs) of the
two sounds; rhythm was analyzed by observing the
distance extracted through Dynamic Time Warping
(M
¨
uller, 2007) and the cross-correlation between the
amplitude envelopes of the signals; melodic aspects
were evaluated through the correlation between the
two signals’ chromagrams and, in order to exclude the
effects of pitch shifting, through the highest correla-
tion between different circular shifts of the chroma-
grams. For a definition of the mentioned audio fea-
tures, in particular MFCCs and chromagrams, please
refer to (Al
´
ıas et al., 2016). Figure 6 shows the di-
agram obtained by computing the mentioned indica-
tors over all the considered recordings, clustered by
administration mode: Group A traditional mode
(blue), Group B individual enhanced mode (or-
ange), Group C cooperative enhanced mode (yel-
low).
Finally, through the use of the decision method
known as Utopia point (Mart
´
ınez-Iranzo et al., 2009),
we tried to understand which administration mode
obtained the best results, and which participants got
the maximum and minimum distance from the Utopia
Figure 6: Aggregated indicators for Exercise 3. In blue
Group A, in orange Group B, in yellow Group C.
Table 2: Mean and standard deviation of the distances
scored by the 3 groups from the Utopia point in Exercise
3.
Mean Standard deviation
Group A 0.7341 0.3684
Group B 0.8414 0.2702
Group C 0.9164 0.2855
point, i.e. the worst and the best result respectively.
Table 2 shows the mean and the standard deviation of
the distances from the Utopia point registered by the
three groups: Group A traditional mode, Group B
individual enhanced mode, Group C cooperative
enhanced mode.
In the assessment phase, timbre-analysis results
were similar for all the administration modes; con-
versely, in the melodic analysis, and specifically in
pitch detection, results were very sparse, indepen-
dently from the administration mode.
With respect to the Utopia point, the administra-
tion mode that best scored was the traditional one,
while the other two returned similar results, not so far
from the former. The top-score participant reached
a distance d = 0.128 from the Utopia Point, work-
ing in traditional mode; conversely, the worst result
(d = 1.2385) was obtained by a child who was ad-
ministered the individual enhanced mode.
Once again, the observation highlighted some in-
teresting aspects not directly related to sound percep-
tion. For example, during the vocal performance of
sounds, many children tended to physically mimic the
animal they were interpreting. When asked to explain
this behavior, children revealed that it was intended to
improve the imitation.
A (Technologically Enhanced) Sound Education: Implementation, Experimentation and Analysis of Raymond Murray Schafer’s Exercises
479
7 CONCLUSIONS
The educational activities proposed in our experimen-
tation confirmed that technology is not positive or
negative in absolute terms, at least in the context of
soundscape self-consciousness: for some types of lis-
tening experiences, the adoption of technology can
be more involving and effective with respect to tra-
ditional methods, whereas, in other cases, advantages
are not evident, and technological aids can even cause
distraction.
Even if the activities reported in this work in-
volved more than 230 students, in order to produce
meaningful and reliable results the experimentation
should be extended to other primary-school classes,
include additional exercises extracted from (Schafer,
1992), and propose other strategies to take benefit
from technological means.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors gratefully wish to acknowledge the teach-
ing staff and the students of the Istituto Comprensivo
“G. Puecher” Scuola primaria via Battisti, Erba,
Italy, for their availability and their enthusiastic par-
ticipation in the experimental activities.
REFERENCES
Al
´
ıas, F., Socor
´
o, J., and Sevillano, X. (2016). A review of
physical and perceptual feature extraction techniques
for speech, music and environmental sounds. Applied
Sciences, 6(5):143.
Barat
`
e, A. and Ludovico, L. A. (2012). New frontiers in
music education through the IEEE 1599 standard. In
Cordeiro, J., Helfert, M., and Martins, M. J., edi-
tors, Proceedings of the 4th International Conference
on Computer Supported Education (CSEDU 2012),
Porto, Portugal, volume 1, pages 146–151, Set
´
ubal.
SCITEPRESS - Science and Technology Publications,
Lda.
Barat
`
e, A., Ludovico, L. A., and Mangione, G. R. (2014). A
new paradigm for music education: Creating active e-
books through the IEEE 1599 standard. In 2014 IEEE
14th International Conference on Advanced Learning
Technologies, Athens, Greece, 7-10 July 2014, pages
614–616, Athens. IEEE Computer Society Confer-
ence Publishing Services (CPS).
De Maesschalck, R., Jouan-Rimbaud, D., and Massart,
D. L. (2000). The Mahalanobis distance. Chemomet-
rics and intelligent laboratory systems, 50(1):1–18.
Ellis, A. B. (1973). The use & misuse of computers in edu-
cation. McGraw-Hill Companies.
Hofstetter, F. T. (1975). Guido: An interactive computer-
based system for improvement of instruction and re-
search in ear-training. Journal of Computer-Based In-
struction, 1(4):100–106.
Kay, A. (1996). Revealing the elephant: The use and misuse
of computers in education. Educom Review, 31(4):22.
Mart
´
ınez-Iranzo, M., Herrero, J. M., Sanchis, J., Blasco,
X., and Garc
´
ıa-Nieto, S. (2009). Applied Pareto
multi-objective optimization by stochastic solvers.
Engineering applications of artificial intelligence,
22(3):455–465.
M
¨
uller, M. (2007). Dynamic time warping. Information
retrieval for music and motion, pages 69–84.
Neufeld, J., Sinke, C., Dillo, W., Emrich, H., Szycik, G.,
Dima, D., Bleich, S., and Zedler, M. (2012). The neu-
ral correlates of coloured music: A functional MRI
investigation of auditory–visual synaesthesia. Neu-
ropsychologia, 50(1):85–89.
Palmer, S. E., Schloss, K. B., Xu, Z., and Prado-Le
´
on,
L. R. (2013). Music–color associations are mediated
by emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 110(22):8836–8841.
Presti, G. and Mauro, D. A. (2013). Continuous bright-
ness estimation (CoBE): Implementation and its pos-
sible applications. In 10th International Sympo-
sium on Computer Music Multidisciplinary Research
(CMMR), Marseille (France), 15-18 October, 2013.
Quesnel, R. and Woszczyk, W. R. (1994). A computer-
aided system for timbral ear training. In Audio En-
gineering Society Convention 96. Audio Engineering
Society.
Ribble, M. and Bailey, G. (2004). Monitoring technology
misuse & abuse. Technology Horizons in Education,
32(1):22–25.
Schafer, R. M. (1977). The tuning of the world. Alfred A.
Knopf.
Schafer, R. M. (1986). The thinking ear: Complete writings
on music education. Arcana Editions.
Schafer, R. M. (1992). A Sound Education: 100 Exercises
in Listening and Soundmaking. Arcana Editions.
Spangler, D. R. (1999). Computer-assisted instruction in
ear-training and its integration into undergraduate
music programs during the 1998-1999 academic year.
Michigan State University.
Vernon, P. (1930). Synaesthesia in music. Psyche.
Wrightson, K. (2000). An introduction to acoustic ecol-
ogy. Soundscape: The journal of acoustic ecology,
1(1):10–13.
Zilczer, J. (1987). Color music: Synaesthesia and
nineteenth-century sources for abstract art. Artibus et
Historiae, pages 101–126.
CSEDU 2019 - 11th International Conference on Computer Supported Education
480