The Association between Forgiveness and Life Satisfaction
Dewa Fajar Bintamur
Universita Indonesia
Keywords: Forgiveness, Life Satisfaction, Indonesian, Young Adulthood
Abstract: This study's objective is to investigate whether there is a relationship between dispositional forgiveness and
life satisfaction at working young adulthood in Indonesia. According to Thompson et al. (2005), there are
three forms or dimensions of dispositional forgiveness: Self-forgiveness, Other-forgiveness, and Situation-
forgiveness. While life satisfaction is the cognitive process of an individual's subjective evaluation of one's
entire life, and it is an indicator of one's well-being. Previous studies have shown that both forgiveness and
life satisfaction associated with social and cultural factors. The instruments to measure forgiveness and life
satisfaction were Heartland Forgiveness Scale (HFS) and SWLS (Satisfaction with Life Scale). Convenient
sampling was the sampling technique to collect data from 167 participants. They were males and females
who in the young adulthood stage, and live in Jakarta, Bogor, Depok, Tangerang, and Bekasi (Jabodetabek
region). The result shows a significant positive correlation between forgiveness and life satisfaction. Where
Self-forgiveness and Situation-forgiveness highly significant with life satisfaction, while Other-forgiveness
moderately significant with life satisfaction.
1 INTRODUCTION
The fast advancement of technology-led human to
another industrial revolution, industrial revolution
4.0, where the convergence of cyberspace with the
physical world could happen, where mass
production systems would no longer produce
uniform products, but it could produce customized
products. Industrial revolution 4.0 is more than just a
change in production and distribution systems. It
also would significantly change the process of
formation, exchange, and distribution of economic,
political, and social values (Philbeck & Davis,
2018). In other words, the world is in a disruptive
era, an era that would change the societies' and
humans' lifestyle. Efforts to survive and adapt to the
changes that occur could cause conflicts between the
parties who had previously cooperated. An ironical
condition because the goal of technological
advancements is to make human life more
comfortable and happier.
The Japanese cabinet in 2016 proposed "Society
5.0" or a "Super Smart Society." Technological
advancements, in that proposal, were utilized as
much as possible for human security and welfare.
Humans become the central actors in the
development of technology and science. Technology
means to meet the needs of human life sustainably
regardless of age, sex, region, and language
(Shiroishi, Uchiyama & Suzuki, 2018).
A joint effort or a process carried out by various
parties will be needed to create Society 5.0.
Unfortunately, there would be the possibility of
conflict among people in it. Conflicts can bother the
achievement of group or individual goals. Mistakes
made by oneself, others, and the situation could
cause failure to achieve goals. Therefore, the ability
to forgive self, others, and situations where needed.
The ability to forgive allows one to keep trying to
achieve his goals. Goals achievements will provide a
sense of satisfaction in life. This study wants to
examine the relationship between forgiveness and
life satisfaction at working young adults since they
are most affected by this disruptive era.
2 LITERATURE REVIEW
Forgiveness is a process that has a motivational and
emotional component (Sandage & Williamson,
2007; Cheng & Yim, 2008; Swickert, Robertson, &
Baird, 2016). The main features of the forgiveness
process are the reduction of vengeful and angry
thoughts, feelings, and motives, which can be
174
Bintamur, D.
The Association between Forgiveness and Life Satisfaction.
DOI: 10.5220/0009440601740180
In Proceedings of the 1st International Conference on Psychology (ICPsy 2019), pages 174-180
ISBN: 978-989-758-448-0
Copyright
c
2020 by SCITEPRESS Science and Technology Publications, Lda. All rights reserved
accompanied by an increase in positive thoughts,
feelings, and motives (Wade, et. Al., 2014 in Cerci
& Colucci, 2018). The definition of forgiveness in
this study is from Thompson et al. (2005) that
defined forgiveness as “... the framing of a perceived
transgression such that one's responses to the
transgressors, transgression, and sequelae of the
transgression are transformed from negative to
neutral or positive. The source of a transgression,
and therefore the object of forgiveness, maybe
oneself, another person or person, or a situation that
views one being beyond anyone's control (e.g., an
illness, '' fate, '' or a natural disaster) "
Forgiveness allows one to overcome
interpersonal offense through a prosocial process
that would have a positive impact on victims and
perpetrators, not through denial, justification, or
revenge (Fehr, Gelfand, & Nag, 2010). Forgiveness
allows one to eliminate hurt feelings and hatred
when responding to the transgression (Swickert,
Robertson, & Baird, 2016). Forgiveness has a
positive impact on survivors. Because of the process
of forgiveness, factors such as cognition,
physiological responses, behavioral intentions,
emotions, motivation, and possibly behavior toward
the offender become more positive over time
(Fernández-Capo et al., 2017).
There are two levels to measure forgiveness in
research, dispositional level or offense-specific level
(McCullough, Pargament, & Thoresen, 2000 in
Cerci & Colucci, 2018). Dispositional forgiveness or
trait forgiveness is the forgiving nature of someone
that stable in various situations and times. Offense-
specific forgiveness is forgiveness that has been
done by someone for a specific event in his or her
life (Cerci & Colucci, 2018). In this study, the
forgiveness level measured was dispositional or trait
forgiveness. There are three forms of dispositional
forgiveness according to Thompson et al. (2005):
Self-forgiveness or ability to forgive oneself; Other-
forgiveness or ability to forgive another person or
persons; Situation-forgiveness or ability to forgive a
situation which is beyond anyone's control, such as
an illness or natural disaster.
Forgiveness is related to age, gender, beliefs,
social-cultural, and religious practice. Older adults
are more willing to forgive than people who are in
the stage of middle-aged and young adults (Mullet et
al., 1998; Cheng & Yim, 2008). Allemand's study
(2008) showed that there was a difference between
forgiveness that has been done by older people
compared to forgiveness that has been done by
younger people. Older people would forgive their
acquaintances and their friends as well. On the other
hand, younger people would prefer to forgive their
friends than their acquaintances.
There were also small to moderate yet significant
differences between gender and forgiveness,
according to a study by Miller, Worthington Jr. &
McDaniel (2008). Swickert, Robertson, & Baird
(2016) found that women were more forgiving than
men. Younger women were more likely to
empathize with transgressors compared to younger
men. However, Swickert, Robertson, & Baird (2016)
stated that there was still a lack of clarity in the
existing literature on the relationship between
gender and forgiveness.
Social-cultural factors also related to forgiveness
(Ho & Fung, 2011; Sandage & Williamson, 2007).
Forgiveness is an interpersonal construct since it is a
process that involves changes in cognition, emotion,
motivation, and behavior of someone against the
transgressor (Ho & Fung, 2011). People who live in
individualistic cultures have a different focus than
people who live in collectivistic cultures. Those who
live in individualistic cultures focus more on
distinguishing themselves from others and striving
to achieve personal goals, while people who live in
collectivistic cultures emphasize more on the norms
of togetherness and relationships with others to be
able to live in harmony with others (Ho &
Worthington, 2018).
People who have a lower level of forgiveness
would have lower levels of distress tolerance and
tend to be more hostile (Matheny et al., (2017).
Longitudinal research conducted by Toussaint, et al.
(2018) concluded that there is a more significant
association of hostility with cognitive impairment
that occurs more than ten years, and the effects
associated with hostility on this cognition lessened
with being more forgiving. Lack of forgiveness is
also associated with hyper-competitiveness, while
personal development competitiveness is positively
associated with forgiveness (Collier et al., 2010).
The level of forgiveness is also related to scores
related to PTSD symptoms. However, it also needs
to consider other variables such as demographics,
the relationship between transgressors and survivors
of trauma, type, and severity of the trauma, and
other relevant variables (Cerci & Colucci, 2018).
Research conducted by Bryan, Theriault, and Bryan
(2015) on military and veteran personnel indicated
that self-forgiveness significantly distinguishes
participants who attempt suicide and those who only
think of committing suicide. Besides being mentally
healthier, forgiveness is also related to one's physical
health and the longevity of Toussaint, Owen, &
Cheadle (2012).
The Association between Forgiveness and Life Satisfaction
175
A forgiving person would have a low level of
distress and a lower level of distress associated with
a higher level of happiness (Toussaint et. Al., 2016).
In other words, there is a positive correlation
between forgiveness and well-being (Toussaint &
Friedman, 2009; Toussaint et al., 2016). One of the
constructs of well-being is subjective well-being
from Diener (1984), and life satisfaction is a
component of subjective well being. Life satisfaction
is the cognitive process of an individual's subjective
evaluation of his entire life (Diener, Suh, Lucas &
Smith, 1999). The evaluation is based on a
comparison of the life experienced by a person with
subjective standards (Diener et al., 1985).
According to Diener, Lucas, Schimmack, and
Helliwell (2009), life satisfaction is an indicator of
individual well-being and social well-being.
Individuals who have a high level of life satisfaction
are those who are successful in developing
relationships with others, at work, and in their
physical functions. They also have made more
money and have been better at dealing with the
diseases they experience (Lewis, 2010 in Dogan &
Celik, 2014).
Life satisfaction also has related to age. Buetel et
al. (2009) found that there was a relationship
between age and life satisfaction on female
respondents from adolescent to late adulthood stage.
Another research conducted by Beutel et al. (2010)
on male respondents at the same developmental
stage (adolescence to late adulthood) showed no
significant difference in life satisfaction that related
to age. While Baird, Lucas, & Donnellan's (2010)
research finding concluded that life satisfaction does
not decrease too much throughout adulthood.
However, there is a sharp decline in respondents
aged over 70 years (Baird, Lucas, & Donnellan,
2010). A study conducted by Jovanović (2017)
showed that there was different life satisfaction
between adolescents and adults. Adolescents' life
satisfaction was higher than in adults' life
satisfaction. Siedlecki, Tucker-Drob, Oishi, &
Salthouse (2008) research also obtained similar
results. They also found that the level of education
was related to the satisfaction of one's life.
Several studies have found that there are
differences in the level of life satisfaction between
men and women. Women reported higher levels of
life satisfaction than women (Jovanović, 2017).
Research conducted by Beutel, et al., (2009)
revealed that factors related to life satisfaction in
general in women are the level of resilience, good
household income, the presence of partners, the
absence of anxiety and depression, not unemployed,
positive self-esteem, religious affiliation, and age.
General life satisfaction in men is related to the level
of resilience, previous unemployment conditions, the
presence of partners, high self-esteem, household
income, the absence of generalized anxiety disorder,
and depression (Beutel et al., 2010).
Cultural factors also affect one’s life satisfaction.
Previous studies have shown differences in life
satisfaction levels in different countries.
Communities in Pacific Rim countries do not have a
strong tendency to evaluate abstract things
positively, while people in Latin American countries
have a strong tendency to value global domains
positively (Diener et al. 2000). Diener, Inglehart,
and Tay (2013) said that life satisfaction level based
on earned income in Latin American society is
higher than in Asian societies, which have a
Confucian culture. The ability of individuals to meet
the cultural demands that exist in a collectivistic
society affects the satisfaction of life, while this does
not apply to ones that live in an individualistic
society. Since in an individualistic society,
independence, uniqueness, and autonomy are
considered culturally relevant (Li & Hamamura,
2010).
This study investigated whether there was a
significant relationship between forgiveness and life
satisfaction at working young people who live in
Jakarta, Bogor, Depok, Tangerang, and Bekasi
(Jabodetabek region). Although previous researches
indicate that there has been a significant relationship
between forgiveness and well-being, this study still
needed to be done because both forgiveness and life-
satisfaction variables have associated with
demographic variables such as age, gender, social,
and cultural.
3 RESEARCH METHOD
The respondents of this study were working young
adults who live in the Greater Jakarta area (Jakarta,
Bogor, Depok, Tangerang, and Bekasi) or
Jabodetabek region. The sampling technique to
obtained cross-sectional data in this non-
experimental study was convenience sampling.
There were a total of 167 people who participated in
this study, 118 females and 49 males. Each
participant was asked to fill a self-report instrument
(questionnaire) which distributed online.
The Bahasa Indonesia version of the Heartland
Forgiveness Scale (HFS), which originally
developed by Thompson et al. (2005), used to
measure dispositional forgiveness. There were three
ICPsy 2019 - International Conference on Psychology
176
dimensions in HFS: self-forgiveness, other-
forgiveness, and situation-forgiveness. Each
dimension consisted of 6 items, and each item
consisted of six Likert-like scales; scale: 1 =
Strongly disagree to 6 = Strongly agree. The
minimum score was 18, and the maximum score was
108. The reliability value of HFS on the self-
forgiveness dimension α = 0.673, the other-
forgiveness dimension α = 0.773, and the situation-
forgiveness dimension α = 0712. HFS reliability
scores for measuring forgiveness constructs are α =
0.814 and ω = 0.822.
Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS) was a
unidimensional measurement tool which initially
developed by Diener et al. (1985) to measure life
satisfaction. This research used the Bahasa Indonesia
version of SWLS, which consists of 5 (five) items
with 6 Likert scale likes; scale 1 = Strongly disagree
to 6 = Strongly agree. Thus, the minimum score for
the variable life satisfaction was 5, and the
maximum score was 30. There were no unfavorable
(reversed) items in this measuring instrument. The
SWLS reliability value of the sample data in this
study is α = 0.788.
Demographic data and distribution scores of the
research variables were analyzed using descriptive
statistics. The statistical technique for testing the
hypothesis of this research is the correlational
technique. This study used Jamovi statistical
software version 1.0.0 (Jamovi, 2019) to analyze
research data.
4 RESULT
Table 1: Demographic Data.
Demographic Data
Mean Std Dev
Age 28.6 4.50
Frequency Percentage
Sex
Male 49 29.3
Female 118 70.7
Education
Senior High
School
3 1.8
Diploma/Academy 7 4.2
Undergraduate 115 68.9
Graduate 42 25.1
Marital Status
Not Married 107 64.1
Married 60 35.9
Religion
Islam 107 64.1
Christian 22 13.2
Catholic 32 19.2
Hindu 1 0.6
Buddha 2 1.2
Others 3 1.8
Demographic data on respondents' age reveals
that most of the respondents are younger than 33
years old of age (M = 28.5, SD = 4.48). Most of the
respondent’s gender are women (70.7%), and less
than a third of them are men (29.3%). The education
level of the respondents is mostly Bachelor (68.9%),
followed by Masters (25.1%), Diploma/Academy
(4.2%), and Senior High School level (1.8%). The
proportion of unmarried respondents are nearly
doubled (64.1%) than the proportion of married
respondents (35.9%). Most respondents’ religions
are Muslim (64.1%), then Catholic (19.2%),
Protestant (13.2%) and Buddhists, Hindus, and
Others (3.6%).
Table 2: Descriptive Statistics.
Forgiv
eness
Self-
forgiv
eness
Other-
forgiv
eness
Situati
on-
forgiv
eness
Life-
Satisfa
ction
N 167 167 167 167 167
Mean 4.05 3.86 4.10 4.17 4.04
Stand
ard
deviat
ion
0.559 0.742 0.782 0.687 0.808
Skew
ness
-0.351 -0.134 -0.580 -0.110 -0.367
Std.
error
skew
ness
0.188 0.188 0.188 0.188 0.188
Kurto
sis
0.573 0.215 0.341 0.416 -0.393
Std.
error
kurto
sis
0.374 0.374 0.374 0.374 0.374
Shapi
ro-
Wilk
p
0.281 0.447 0.002 0.195 0.004
The Association between Forgiveness and Life Satisfaction
177
The mean score of self-forgiveness (Mean =
3.86, SD = 0.742) is lower than other-forgiveness
(M = 4.10, SD = 0.782) and situation-forgiveness
(M = 4.17, SD = 0.687). One of the possible
explanations for that condition is because the
reliability of the self-forgiveness dimension was
modest = 0.673). Another explanation is because
it is harder to determine self-forgiveness since there
was no feedback from other people that one’s could
use as a reference. However, unlike self-forgiveness,
for the two other dimensions, which are other-
forgiveness and situation-forgiveness, respondents
can get feedback or comparison that can be used to
determine the level of forgiveness. Therefore, it
needs further researches on self-forgiveness in
collectivistic culture societies.
The distribution scores of other-forgiveness (M =
4.10, SD = 0.782) and life satisfaction (M = 4.04,
SD = 0.808) are not normal (Shapiro-Wilk < 0.01).
Whereas the distribution of the other scores is
considered normal. The alternative statistical
technique to calculate the correlations among those
variables was Spearman's Rho correlation.
Spearman's Rho is a correlation technique which
categorized as a non-parametric statistic.
Consequently, the result of this study could not be
generalized to the population.
Table 3: Correlations
Varia-
bles
1 2 3 4 5
1. Life
Satisfac
tion
- 0.382
***
0.299
***
0.178
*
0.326
***
2.
Forgive
ness
- 0.730
***
0.711
***
0.810
***
3. Self-
forgive
ness
- 0.224
**
0.488
***
4.
Other-
forgive
ness
-
0.401
***
5.
Situatio
n-
forgive
ness
-
Note. * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001
Forgiveness and life-satisfaction have a
significant positive correlation (165) = 0.382, p>
0.001, r
2
= 0.146. This means, the higher one's level
of forgiveness, his or her level of life-satisfaction is
also higher. All dimensions of forgiveness have
significant positive correlation with life-satisfaction.
The dimension of forgiveness which has the highest
correlation with life-satisfaction is Situation-
forgiveness (165) = 0.326, p> 0.001, r
2
= 0.106,
then the correlation level between the dimensions of
Self-forgiveness and life-satisfaction (165) =
0.299, p> 0.001, r
2
= 0.089, while the dimension
with the lowest level of correlation with life-
satisfaction is the Other-forgiveness dimension
(165) = 0.178, p = 0.011, r
2
= 0.032.
5 DISCUSSION
The result of this study shows that there is a
significant positive relationship between life-
satisfaction and dispositional forgiveness. All its
dimensions, namely self-forgiveness, other-
forgiveness, and situational-forgiveness, also have
significant positive correlations with life-
satisfaction. These results are similar to the results
of studies that were conducted by Toussaint &
Friedman (2009), who also used self-report methods
(HFS & SWLS questionnaires), and cross-sectional
design. The coefficient correlations in their study
were slightly higher than the coefficient correlations
in this study. It might due to their participants were
relatively homogeneous respondents (outpatient
psychotherapy).
The measurement of forgiveness in this study is
general forgiveness or dispositional level
forgiveness (Thompson et al., 2005), not case-
specific forgiveness. Therefore, a study on the
relationship between case-specific forgiveness and
life satisfaction needs to do. Because in specific
cases, several things could affect one's forgiveness,
such as the relationship between transgressors and
survivors of trauma, type, and severity of the trauma
(Cerci & Colucci, 2018). The results of that study
would figure out the consistency of the association
between forgiveness and life satisfaction.
Forgiveness construct in this study has a similar
meaning with forgivingness construct (Suwartono,
Prawasti, & Mullet, 2007); both of those constructs
refer to dispositional forgiveness. Several studies
have revealed that dispositional forgiveness
associated with culture. A study about forgivingness
conducted by Suwartono, Prawasti, & Mullet (2007)
has shown the influence of individualistic culture
and collectivistic culture on forgiveness. However,
research conducted by Paz, Neto, & Mullet (2008)
ICPsy 2019 - International Conference on Psychology
178
shows that there were no differences caused by
cultural factors. According to Paz et all (2008), there
may be other factors in the culture that can influence
forgiveness; one of them is religion. That opinion
referred to Paz, R., Neto, F., & Mullet, E. (2007)
study result, which showed that there was different
forgivingness in Buddhists and Christians who live
in China.
Other than those things above, since this study
only controlled the developmental stage (young
adulthood) or respondents. Consequently, it cannot
provide a picture or comparison of forgiveness and
life satisfaction with other developmental stages.
Other demographic factors, such as gender,
occupation, income, and ethnicity, were not
controlled, and since the distribution of respondents
in those demographic factors equivalently
distributed, then statistical calculations to make the
comparison.
Another limitation of this study is convenient
sampling (non-random sampling), self-administered
data collection, and cross-sectional design. The
convenience sampling method only could reach
people that can be met or contacted and who have
had the willingness to participate in data collection.
Usually, in this method, people in negative affect
would not want to participate in data collection. A
self-administered method is also vulnerable to
faking-good or faking-bad responses, and the cross-
sectional method could only measure forgiveness
and life-satisfaction at a time.
6 CONCLUSIONS
In conclusion, Forgiveness had a highly significant
positive correlation with life satisfaction. Where
Self-forgiveness and Situation-forgiveness were
highly significant with life satisfaction, while Other-
forgiveness was moderately significant with life
satisfaction.
These study results imply that further research
about factors that relate to forgiveness in Indonesia
will be needed since Indonesia is a plural country. A
country which consists of multi-cultures and multi-
religions country. Both forgiveness and life-
satisfaction constructs are related to the culture
where someone lives and religion that one's belief.
REFERENCES
Allemand, M. (2008). Age differences in forgivingness:
The role of future time perspective. Journal of
Research in Personality, 42(5), 1137-1147.
Arrindell, W. A., Meeuwesen, L., & Huyse, F. J. (1991).
The Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS):
Psychometric properties in a non-psychiatric medical
outpatients sample. Personality and individual
differences, 12(2), 117-123.
Baird, B. M., Lucas, R. E., & Donnellan, M. B. (2010).
Life satisfaction across the lifespan: Findings from
two nationally representative panel studies. Social
indicators research, 99(2), 183-203.
Beutel, M. E., Glaesmer, H., Decker, O., Fischbeck, S., &
Brähler, E. (2009). Life satisfaction, distress, and
resiliency across the life span of women. Menopause,
16(6), 1132-1138.
Beutel, M. E., Glaesmer, H., Wiltink, J., Marian, H., &
Brähler, E. (2010). Life satisfaction, anxiety,
depression, and resilience across the life span of men.
The Aging Male, 13(1), 32-39.
Bryan, A. O., Theriault, J. L., & Bryan, C. J. (2015). Self-
forgiveness, posttraumatic stress, and suicide attempts
among military personnel and veterans. Traumatology,
21(1), 40.
Cerci, D., & Colucci, E. (2018). Forgiveness in PTSD
after man-made traumatic events: A systematic
review. Traumatology, 24(1), 47.
Cheng, S. T., & Yim, Y. K. (2008). Age differences in
forgiveness: The role of future time perspective.
Psychology and Aging, 23(3), 676.
Collier, S. A., Ryckman, R. M., Thornton, B., & Gold, J.
A. (2010). Competitive personality attitudes and
forgiveness of others. The Journal of Psychology,
144(6), 535-543.
Cotton Bronk, K., Hill, P. L., Lapsley, D. K., Talib, T. L.,
& Finch, H. (2009). Purpose, hope, and life
satisfaction in three age groups. The Journal of
Positive Psychology, 4(6), 500-510.
Diener, E., Inglehart, R., & Tay, L. (2013). Theory and
validity of life satisfaction scales. Social Indicators
Research, 112(3), 497-527.
Diener, E., Napa-Scollon, C. K., Oishi, S., Dzokoto, V., &
Suh, E. M. (2000). Positivity and the construction of
life satisfaction judgments: Global happiness is not the
sum of its parts. Journal of happiness studies, 1(2),
159-176.
Duffy, R. D., Allan, B. A., Autin, K. L., & Bott, E. M.
(2013). Calling and life satisfaction: It's not about
having it, it's about living it. Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 60(1), 42.
Fehr, R., Gelfand, M. J., & Nag, M. (2010). The road to
forgiveness: a meta-analytic synthesis of its situational
and dispositional correlates. Psychological Bulletin,
136(5), 894.
Fernández-Capo, M., Fernández, S. R., Sanfeliu, M. G.,
Benito, J. G., & Worthington Jr, E. L. (2017).
Measuring forgiveness. European Psychologist.
The Association between Forgiveness and Life Satisfaction
179
Fugl-Meyer, A. R., Melin, R., & Fugl-Meyer, K. S.
(2002). Life satisfaction in 18-to 64-year-old Swedes:
in relation to gender, age, partner, and immigrant
status. Journal of rehabilitation medicine, 34(5), 239-
246.
Ho, M. Y., & Fung, H. H. (2011). A dynamic process
model of forgiveness: A cross-cultural perspective.
Review of General Psychology, 15(1), 77-84.
Ho, M. Y., & Worthington, E. L. (2018). Is the concept of
forgiveness universal? A cross-cultural perspective
comparing western and eastern cultures. Current
Psychology, 1-8.
Li, L. M. W., & Hamamura, T. (2010). Cultural fit and life
satisfaction: Endorsement of cultural values predicts
life satisfaction only in collectivistic societies. Journal
of Psychology in Chinese Societies, 11(2), 109.
Matheny, N. L., Smith, H. L., Summers, B. J., McDermott,
K. A., Macatee, R. J., & Cougle, J. R. (2017). The role
of distress tolerance in multiple facets of hostility and
willingness to forgive. Cognitive therapy and
research, 41(2), 170-177.
Miller, A. J., Worthington Jr, E. L., & McDaniel, M. A.
(2008). Gender and forgiveness: A meta-analytic
review and research agenda. Journal of Social and
Clinical Psychology, 27(8), 843-876.
Mullet, E., Houdbine, A., Laumonier, S., & Girard, M.
(1998). “Forgivingness”: Factor structure in a sample
of young, middle-aged, and elderly adults. European
Psychologist, 3(4), 289-297.
Pavot, W., Diener, E. D., Colvin, C. R., & Sandvik, E.
(1991). Further validation of the Satisfaction with Life
Scale: Evidence for the cross-method convergence of
well-being measures. Journal of personality
assessment, 57(1), 149-161.
Paz, R., Neto, F., & Mullet, E. (2007). Forgivingness:
Similarities and differences between Buddhists and
Christians living in China. The international journal
for the psychology of religion, 17(4), 289-301.
Paz, R., Neto, F., & Mullet, E. (2008). Forgiveness: A
China-Western Europe comparison. The Journal of
Psychology, 142(2), 147-158.
Philbeck, T., & Davis, N. (2018). The Fourth Industrial
Revolution: Shaping a New Era. Journal of
International Affairs, 72(1), 17.
R Core Team (2018). R: A Language and environment for
statistical computing. [Computer software]. Retrieved
from https://cran.r-project.org/.
Revelle, W. (2019). Psych: Procedures for Psychological,
Psychometric, and Personality Research. [R package].
Retrieved from https://cran.r-
project.org/package=psych.
Sandage, S. J., & Williamson, I. (2007). Forgiveness in
cultural context. In Handbook of forgiveness (pp. 65-
80). Routledge.
Shiroishi, Y., Uchiyama, K., & Suzuki, N. (2018). Society
5.0: For human security and well-being. Computer
,
51(7), 91-95.
Siedlecki, K. L., Tucker-Drob, E. M., Oishi, S., &
Salthouse, T. A. (2008). Life satisfaction across
adulthood: Different determinants at different ages?.
The Journal of Positive Psychology, 3(3), 153-164.
Suwartono, C., Prawasti, C. Y., & Mullet, E. (2007).
Effect of culture on forgivingness: A Southern Asia–
Western Europe comparison. Personality and
Individual Differences, 42(3), 513-523.
Swickert, R., Robertson, S., & Baird, D. (2016). Age
moderates the mediational role of empathy in the
association between gender and forgiveness. Current
Psychology, 35(3), 354-360.
The jamovi project (2019). jamovi. (Version 1.0.0)
[Computer Software]. Retrieved from
https://www.jamovi.org.
Thompson, L. Y., Snyder, C. R., Hoffman, L., Michael, S.
T., Rasmussen, H. N., Billings, L. S., ... & Roberts, D.
E. (2005). Dispositional forgiveness of self, others,
and situations. Journal of personality, 73(2), 313-360.
Toussaint, L. L., Owen, A. D., & Cheadle, A. (2012).
Forgive to live: Forgiveness, health, and longevity.
Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 35(4), 375-386.
Toussaint, L. L., Shields, G. S., Green, E., Kennedy, K.,
Travers, S., & Slavich, G. M. (2018). Hostility,
forgiveness, and cognitive impairment over 10 years
in a national sample of American adults. Health
Psychology, 37(12), 1102.
Toussaint, L., & Friedman, P. (2009). Forgiveness,
gratitude, and well-being: The mediating role of affect
and beliefs. Journal of Happiness Studies, 10(6), 635.
Toussaint, L., Shields, G. S., Dorn, G., & Slavich, G. M.
(2016). Effects of lifetime stress exposure on mental
and physical health in young adulthood: How stress
degrades and forgiveness protects health. Journal of
health psychology, 21(6), 1004-1014.
ICPsy 2019 - International Conference on Psychology
180