The Effects of Augmented Reality: A Comparative Study in an
Undergraduate Physics Laboratory Course
Sebastian Kapp
1 a
, Michael Thees
1
, Fabian Beil
1
, Thomas Weatherby
2
, Jan-Philipp Burde
3
,
Thomas Wilhelm
2
and Jochen Kuhn
1
1
Physics Education Research Group, TU Kaiserslautern, Erwin-Schr
¨
odinger-Str. 46, Kaiserslautern, Germany
2
Department for Physics Education Research, Goethe-University Frankfurt,
Max-von-Laue-Str. 1, Frankfurt am Main, Germany
3
Physics Education Research Group, Eberhard Karls University T
¨
ubingen, Auf der Morgenstelle 14, T
¨
ubingen, Germany
Keywords:
Augmented Reality, Physics Laboratory Experiments, Cognitive Load, Split-attention Effect, Inquiry
Learning.
Abstract:
The use of augmented reality (AR) in inquiry-based learning has become of increasing interest to researchers.
Recent studies highlight the benefits of AR in various instructional scenarios concerning knowledge acqui-
sition and cognitive load compared to traditional settings. Particularly in the context of physics laboratory
experiments, previous research examined the context of simple electrical circuits. However, results were lim-
ited to laboratory studies and showed contrasting impacts on knowledge acquisition. While one study reported
a higher knowledge acquisition in a tablet-based AR setting, another study reported a higher knowledge ac-
quisition and a reduction in extraneous cognitive load in a two-dimensional non-AR setting compared to a
smartglasses-based AR setting. Consequently, the importance of context specific aspects must be considered
more deeply. In this study we present a randomized controlled trial in a graded physics laboratory course
evaluating the effects of a smartglasses-based AR environment on cognitive load and conceptual knowledge
acquisition compared to a two-dimensional non-AR setting. The sample consists of a total of N = 56 students
in two groups performing a set of eight traditional inquiry-based experiments exploring the relationships in
basic circuit theory. While both groups reported low extraneous cognitive load and achieved a significant
knowledge acquisition, no group differences were detected.
1 INTRODUCTION
1.1 Objective and Rationale
Inquiry-based learning can be described as a form of
engaged learning in which learners are presented with
situations they need to manipulate in order to create
the information they are supposed to learn by them-
selves (de Jong, 2019). Therefore, as most labora-
tory courses require the learner to find an answer to a
given scientific question, they can be taken as an an
example of inquiry-based learning (de Jong, 2019).
In general, inquiry-based learning does not require
the use of a certain technology. However, a specific
technology can be an essential part of its implementa-
tion (de Jong et al., 2018; de Jong, 2019). Traditional
physics laboratory courses, while providing physical
a
https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1052-8901
interaction and implementing inquiry-based learning,
do not automatically guarantee positive learning out-
comes (Husnaini and Chen, 2019; Kapici et al., 2019;
Wilcox and Lewandowski, 2017). Yet, adding vir-
tual content to such a setup is claimed to be benefi-
cial for learning outcomes in STEM courses (de Jong
et al., 2013; Jones and Sharma, 2019). Augmented
reality is thereby capable of displaying virtual com-
ponents together with the real-world physical setup
(Azuma, 1997; Billinghurst and Duenser, 2012; Kuhn
et al., 2016; Santos et al., 2014; Strzys et al., 2018)
and can be used to provide spatial and temporal con-
tiguity between physical and virtual components (Bu-
jak et al., 2013; Altmeyer et al., 2020; Thees et al.,
2020a,b). Ib
´
a
˜
nez and Delgado-Kloos (2018) addi-
tionally emphasized the application of AR in inquiry-
based STEM learning. Overall, Garz
´
on and Acevedo
(2019) observed a significant learning gain by us-
ing AR (medium effect, d = .68) in various instruc-
Kapp, S., Thees, M., Beil, F., Weatherby, T., Burde, J., Wilhelm, T. and Kuhn, J.
The Effects of Augmented Reality: A Comparative Study in an Undergraduate Physics Laboratory Course.
DOI: 10.5220/0009793001970206
In Proceedings of the 12th International Conference on Computer Supported Education (CSEDU 2020) - Volume 2, pages 197-206
ISBN: 978-989-758-417-6
Copyright
c
2020 by SCITEPRESS Science and Technology Publications, Lda. All rights reserved
197
tional scenarios. However, one should keep in mind
that learning outcomes are not only governed by spa-
tial and temporal contiguity but also depend on the
instructional goal of the lesson as well as the func-
tional relation between virtual and physical compo-
nents (Ainsworth, 2006; Garz
´
on and Acevedo, 2019;
Rau, 2020; Thees et al., 2020a).
Recently, the subject area of electrical circuits in
physics laboratory courses was introduced as a new
area of research (Altmeyer et al., 2020; Thees et al.,
2020a). While Altmeyer et al. reported a bene-
fit of tablet-based AR regarding the learning gain in
physics laboratory courses, Thees et al. reported a
benefit in learning gain as well as a reduction in ex-
traneous cognitive load for a two-dimensional repre-
sentation in comparison to a smartglasses-based AR
environment. This result could be explained by ap-
plying the theory of a spatial contiguity failure (Beege
et al., 2019).
With both studies being limited in the form of
clinical laboratory studies with voluntary participants,
their ecological validity with respect to a real-world
scenario is unclear. We present a randomized con-
trolled trial in a graded laboratory course at a German
university with N = 56 participants in which perfor-
mance and cognitive load were assessed as dependent
variables.
1.2 Multiple Representations, Cognitive
Load and Multimedia Learning
As learning environments using technology can con-
tain multiple sources of information like visual ele-
ments (e.g. real laboratory setup and visualized data)
and verbal information (e.g. numerical data, work-
sheets etc.; Ainsworth, 2006), one can consider ba-
sic principles of multimedia learning to evaluate them
(Moreno, 2006).
Combining multiple representations in such a way
is particularly prominent in augmented reality se-
tups, as they are able to add multiple visualizations
of information and data into the learner’s existing
physical environment (Azuma, 1997; Billinghurst and
Duenser, 2012; Santos et al., 2014). Adding addi-
tional information can be described using Cognitive
Load Theory (Sweller, 1988; van Merri
¨
enboer and
Sweller, 2005) and Cognitive Theory of Multimedia
Learning (Mayer, 2005, 2009).
Cognitive Load Theory (CLT; Sweller, 1988; van
Merri
¨
enboer and Sweller, 2005) assumes that the ca-
pacity of an individual’s working memory is limited.
Performing a learning task thereby creates a cognitive
load on the working memory. According to Sweller
et al. (1998), this load can be described in three cat-
egories: intrinsic cognitive load (ICL, caused by the
inherent complexity of the task), extraneous cognitive
load (ECL; caused by performing task-irrelevant cog-
nitive processes), and germane cognitive load (GCL;
caused by constructing conceptual knowledge from
information sources). However, in recent years re-
searchers also suggest a realignment towards a two-
factor ICL/ECL model in which the effectiveness of
the instruction is indicated by the GCL (Sweller et al.,
2019).
The Cognitive Theory of Multimedia Learning
(CTML; Mayer et al., 1999) states that an individ-
ual must actively engage in integrating multimedia in-
formation into mental representations of the learning
content to achieve meaningful learning. Processing
multimedia instructions can be described in terms of
three main processes in one’s working memory: se-
lection, organization, and integration. In accordance
with the cognitive load theory, CTML also assumes
that working memory is of limited capacity regarding
the amount of information that can be processed si-
multaneously. To reduce the cognitive load induced
by the integration of information and to prevent over-
load, several design principles that lead to an eco-
nomic use of a learner’s working memory were de-
veloped (Mayer, 2009).
Highly important to our work are the principles of
temporal and spatial contiguity (Mayer, 2009; Mayer
and Moreno, 2003). They suggest that related infor-
mation should be presented simultaneously as well
as spatially close to each other to promote positive
learning outcomes and reduce extraneous processing
(Ginns, 2006; Schroeder and Cenkci, 2018). The
split-attention effect thereby describes the required
division in the learner’s attention when information
is presented spatially and temporally split from each
other causing search processes irrelevant to learning.
(Mayer and Moreno, 2003; Mayer and Pilegard, 2014;
Sweller and Chandler, 1994; Sweller et al., 2019). By
presenting information in a spatially and temporally
contiguous way this effect can be avoided, and the
extraneous load reduced. In their review of differ-
ent instructional multimedia environments, Schroeder
and Cenkci (2018) confirm the advantages of inte-
grated design formats over split-source formats and
thus consolidate the split-attention effect as a rather
stable finding in multimedia research.
However, under certain circumstances integrated
designs fail to lead to higher learning gains and lower
cognitive load when compared to split-source for-
mats (Beege et al., 2019; Cammeraat et al., 2020).
Such scenarios seem to present a boundary condition
for the split-attention effect and Beege et al. (2019)
coined the term spatial contiguity failure for these
CSEDU 2020 - 12th International Conference on Computer Supported Education
198
cases. The failure occurs for example when the el-
ement interactivity within a text is high before it is
integrated into other representations. An element is
thereby defined as anything that needs to be learned
(Sweller, 2010). In the case of high element inter-
activity parts of the text can only be understood in
combination with each other and learners have to rein-
tegrate the pieces of information that are now split
across the instructional material. This reintegration
of information can counter the advantages of inte-
grated formats by causing elevated extraneous cog-
nitive load. Therefore, it depends on the instructional
material under consideration whether an integrated or
a split format leads to better learning outcomes.
1.3 Prior Work
Prior work concerning electric circuits showed that
AR-based learning environments can avoid split-
attention effects in university STEM laboratory
courses by presenting virtual information in close spa-
tial proximity to their physical counterparts:
Altmeyer et al. (2020) used a tablet-based ap-
proach to create an AR environment supporting stu-
dents’ knowledge acquisition by visualizing real time
measurement data. The visualization was either pre-
sented spatially anchored to the real world using
tablet-based AR or presented spatially split in the
form of a two-dimensional grid on a tablet screen.
Their goal was to minimize the learners’ extraneous
processing while conducting the experiment. In their
study, no group-specific reduction of extraneous load
was detected and performance scores revealed just
minimally higher learning gains in favor of the AR
setting.
Based on these results, Thees et al. (2020a) as-
sumed that the setting of Altmeyer et al. (2020) could
depict a case of spatial contiguity failure as described
by Beege et al. (2019). To support their hypothesis,
Thees et al. conducted another study based on the
same instructional content as Altmeyer et al.. There,
the non-AR group used the same two-dimensional vi-
sualization of the measurement data on an tablet as
Altmeyer et al.. The AR group, however, used smart-
glasses to create an integrated, three-dimensional and
hands-free visualization. Based on these settings, two
types of spatial contiguity were comparable: In the
non-AR setting the visualizations were closely related
to each other as they were presented in a grid. In
the AR setting the visualizations were closely related
to the real experimental components by being spa-
tially anchored to them. However, due to the lim-
ited field of view of the smartglasses, not all visu-
alizations were visible at all times. Based on these
differences in the presentation of the visualizations a
difference in the learning outcome of the students was
expected. The results showed a lower extraneous load
for the non-AR group. Regarding their conceptual
knowledge, both groups showed a significant learn-
ing gain after the intervention. However, the non-AR
group showed a significantly higher gain regarding
instruction-related conceptual and transfer tasks com-
pared to the AR group.
1.4 Hypotheses
The purpose of the present study is to validate the hy-
potheses from Thees et al. (2020a) in practical use
during a graded laboratory course. To reach this goal,
the same conditions as Thees et al. were used with the
instructions and test instruments adapted to the differ-
ent target audience.
As the instruction primarily requires the learner to
identify relations between physical values at different
points in the circuit, it is more important for the
learner to be able to quickly compare these values
between each other instead of precisely locating
them in the real setup. With the examined circuits
only consisting of a few components, their spatial
complexity was low and easy to remember. Therefore
a case of spatial contiguity failure regarding the
spatial position of the visualizations is assumed to
be present (Beege et al., 2019), leading to the first
hypotheses:
Hypothesis 1: Working with tablet-based two-
dimensional visualizations results in a lower ECL
compared to the smartglasses-based AR visualization.
Following the spatial contiguity principle, the
close spatial presentation of related information
during multimedia learning can support generative
processing and therefore increase the efficacy of
learning. This efficacy is defined here as the increase
in conceptual knowledge (Pundak and Rozner, 2008;
Vosniadou, 2007).
Hypothesis 2: Working with tablet-based two-
dimensional visualizations results in an increased
knowledge gain compared to the smartglasses-based
AR visualization.
The Effects of Augmented Reality: A Comparative Study in an Undergraduate Physics Laboratory Course
199
Figure 1: Screenshot of the two-dimensional visualization
as presented on the tablet PC.
2 MATERIALS AND METHODS
2.1 Development of the
Technology-enhanced Science
Learning Environments
We developed a universal science environment cover-
ing electrical circuits to foster the acquisition of con-
ceptual knowledge. The corresponding concept was
first presented in Kapp et al. (2019) and also used in
Altmeyer et al. (2020) and Thees et al. (2020a). It
consists of custom designed measurement nodes in-
tegrated into the experimentation components as well
as accompanying mobile applications for both a tablet
PC (Apple iPad) as well as a pair of smartglasses
(Microsoft HoloLens). The measurement nodes com-
municate their measurements to a tablet in real time,
which then either displays them in a two-dimensional
representation or sends the data to the smartglasses to
be visualized in an AR environment.
Staying as close to traditional physics learning
scenarios as possible, the experimentation compo-
nents offer two sockets to integrate them into the elec-
trical circuit as well as the electronic symbol and de-
tails of the integrated component. However, the mea-
surement node added is also kept transparent to the
learner using a transparent bottom. This measurement
node constantly measures the current and voltage ap-
plied to the component and makes the measurements
available to the clients using a Bluetooth Low Energy
(BLE) service. To measure the voltage and current
applied to the circuit by the power supply, a custom
3D printed enclosure also houses an identical mea-
surement node as the components.
In total there were three custom applications used
Figure 2: Representation of the AR view as seen through
the smartglasses (limited field of view indicated by white
rectangle).
in the present study. All applications were developed
using the Unity3D game development engine target-
ing either an Apple iPad or the Microsoft HoloLens.
The first application targets the two-dimensional
visualization using an Apple iPad and is used by the
non-AR group. It directly visualizes the measure-
ment data in a grid on the display of the tablet (Figure
1). During the experiment the learner can select be-
tween the visualization of voltage (Spannung) or cur-
rent (Stromst
¨
arke) and is able to hide visualizations of
unused components.
The second two applications target the use of
smartglasses in the experiment and are used by the
AR group. They also use an Apple iPad which com-
municates with the measurement nodes and sends the
acquired data to a Microsoft HoloLens for the AR
view. The measurement data is first received and eval-
uated on the Apple iPad to reduce the load on the
HoloLens application and to enable multiple smart-
glasses to take part in the same experiment. This pre-
processed data is then sent to the HoloLens over Wi-
Fi. The tablet application is only used by the instruc-
tor and does not visualize the measurement data to
the participant. On the HoloLens the measurement
data is spatially anchored to the corresponding ex-
perimentation components resulting in an AR view
(Fig. 2). The position of the components is identi-
fied using visual markers which are recognized by the
Vuforia Engine integrated into Unity3D. After an ini-
tial localization using the visual markers, the position
of the experimentation components is transferred to
the tracking capabilities of the Microsoft HoloLens
which keeps the visualization stable even when the
marker recognition is lost. During the experiment
the learner can select between voltage and current
visualization using gesture controls provided by the
CSEDU 2020 - 12th International Conference on Computer Supported Education
200
Microsoft HoloLens. Currently unused components
are simply placed outside the experimentation area,
which also moves their visualization outside of the
field of view of the learner.
To enable the applications to support a multitude
of electrical circuits they can be individually config-
ured. In this initial configuration the instructor spec-
ifies which experimentation components will be used
and initializes the connections to the measurement
nodes. After saving the configuration, it is offered to a
learner starting the application. Selecting an existing
configuration results in the application automatically
connecting to all measurement nodes and visualizing
the corresponding measurement data. With this setup
the participant is immediately able to start the experi-
ment.
2.2 Procedure and Materials
A two-group pretest–posttest design was used in the
present study with participants randomly assigned to
the AR or non-AR group. The instructional mate-
rial as well as the visualizations were identical for
each group reducing the difference between them to
the technology used to present the visualizations and
therefore their spatial position.
The tests, design and questionnaires were derived
from Altmeyer et al. (2020) and Thees et al. (2020a)
while being modified to fit the target group of the
physics laboratory course. A conceptual knowledge
test was used in both the pre- and posttest to cap-
ture the prior knowledge as well as the learning gain.
In the posttest we also used a subjective rating scale
given to evaluate the cognitive load during the exper-
iment. The usability of the system was also evaluated
during the posttest using a subjective rating scale.
2.2.1 Sample
The sample comprises of N = 56 biology students
taking the second physics laboratory course at the
Goethe-University Frankfurt. All students had pre-
viously visited the corresponding physics lecture and
had taken the first physics laboratory course covering
mechanics, optics and thermodynamics.
2.2.2 Procedure
The present study was conducted during a graded lab-
oratory course taking place within the lecture period
and included one session per week. In each session,
teams of two students conducted one of the experi-
ments in the course. Every week the students were
assigned an experiment for the next session and re-
ceived an instruction manual with which they pre-
Table 1: Demographic data of the sample. (* voluntary dis-
closure).
non-AR group
(n = 29)
AR group
(n = 27)
gender
female 17 19
male 9 4
no answer* 3 4
age
M 20.83 20.44
SD 1.42 1.63
pared for it. During the session the total time to work
on the tasks from the instruction manual was limited
to three hours. After carrying out the experiment, the
students were asked to evaluate their results and to
prepare an experiment protocol by the next session,
one week later. The measures of the study took place
exclusively during the execution of the experiment.
At the beginning of the session, the students first
answered the pretest consisting of the knowledge test
as well as providing demographic data. Afterwards,
a short oral examination of the students regarding the
content of the experiment as well as a briefing regard-
ing the assistive system was conducted by the super-
visor, with both groups having the same supervisor.
pretest
conceptual knowledge, demographic data
(~15 min)
oral exam
(~5 min)
brieng
AR condition
(~5 min)
brieng
non-AR condition
(~5 min)
preparation
laboratory manual
(uncontrolled)
lab work
AR condition
(~135 min)
lab work
non-AR condition
(~135 min)
posttest
cognitive load, system usability,
conceptual knowledge
(~20 min)
analysis
laboratory manual
(uncontrolled)
Figure 3: Experimental procedure.
The Effects of Augmented Reality: A Comparative Study in an Undergraduate Physics Laboratory Course
201
For the group using smartglasses the briefing also in-
cluded an individual calibration of the device for each
student using the default calibration routine. After
this, the students independently worked on the exper-
imental tasks as given in the instruction manual. Each
task consisted of setting up an electrical circuit, an-
swering qualitative observations and capturing three
measurement series regarding the voltage and current
at each component. After all tasks had been com-
pleted, the supervisor inspected their work ensuring
its completeness. Once every task was completed the
students answered the posttest consisting of a ques-
tionnaire regarding the perceived cognitive load, the
usability of the used assistive system, and again the
knowledge test.
2.2.3 Instruction Manual and Experimental
Tasks
The instruction manual guides the participants
through the experiment and the subsequent analy-
sis. It first lists the terminology and theoretical back-
ground the student was supposed to prepare before
coming to the session. The experiment itself consists
of eight electrical circuits in total. The first six ex-
periments cover three serial and three parallel circuits
by systematically adding resistors and varying their
resistance. For each circuit the students were given
a set of qualitative observation tasks and were asked
to collect three series of measurements. The last two
experiments are combined circuits which include a
switch closing either a parallel branch in the first cir-
cuit or bridging a resistor in the second. For these
circuits only qualitative observation tasks are given,
aiming at the understanding of the circuit in both po-
sitions of the switch. In the analysis, the students had
to substantiate all their qualitative observations using
Kirchhoffs circuit laws. They also had to quantita-
tively evaluate whether their collected measurement
series fitted the predictions made using the laws. To
ease the quantitative evaluation the tolerances of the
measurement nodes used were provided as well as the
formula for the propagation of these tolerances.
2.2.4 Conceptual Knowledge Test
To evaluate the learning gain we used a conceptual
knowledge test regarding electrical circuits. It con-
sisted of 15 single-choice items and is based on a two-
tier test instrument developed by Ivanjek et al. (2020).
The focus of the test instrument is to assess students’
conceptual understanding of current, voltage and re-
sistance. Each item consists of an item stem describ-
ing the circuit as well as a corresponding circuit di-
agram. The original test instrument uses a two-tier
The circuit shown consists of a 6 V battery
and two identical resistors.
Compare the current through both resistors.
The current through R
1
is larger than through R
2
.
A current flows through R
1
but not through R
2
.
The current through R
2
is larger than through R
1
.
A current flows through R
2
but not through R
1
.
The current through both resistors is the same.
Figure 4: Example item from the conceptual knowledge test
used (based on: Ivanjek et al., 2020, translated for this pub-
lication).
structure in which students not only have to answer a
question on the first tier but also provide an explana-
tion for their reasoning on the second tier. However,
for the present study we only focused on the first tier
of the test instrument. An example item is presented
in Figure 4.
2.2.5 Cognitive Load
To investigate the intrinsic, extraneous and germane
cognitive load of each participant during the experi-
ment we adapted the cognitive load scale (CLS) by
Leppink et al. (2013, 2014) to a laboratory setting
as presented by Thees et al. (2020b). Thees et al.
reported a comparable internal structure as Leppink
et al. for the modified six-point Likert scale as well
as nearly excellent reliabilities for the three sub scales
(α
ICL
= .70, α
ECL
= .66, α
GCL
= .86). The same cog-
nitive load scale was used by Altmeyer et al. (2020).
2.2.6 Usability
We used the System Usability Scale (SUS) by Brooke
(1996) to investigate the usability of the assistive sys-
tems. There, the participants had to rate their level
of agreement with 10 statements concerning the han-
dling and usefulness of the used system on a ve-
point scale. The SUS was translated into German and
the term ”system” was specified as ”the interaction
between the digital assistive system, the correspond-
ing software application, and the experimental equip-
ment”. The score is calculated by multiplying the cu-
mulated item scores (value range: 0-4; some items
CSEDU 2020 - 12th International Conference on Computer Supported Education
202
inverted) with the factor 2.5 to achieve a value range
between 0 and 100 (best). The same scale was used
by Altmeyer et al. (2020) and Thees et al. (2020a).
3 RESULTS
The descriptive results for all dependent variables are
shown in Table 2. The assumptions for conducting an
ANOVA (independence of samples, normal distribu-
tion of residuals, and homogeneity of residuals’ vari-
ances) and ANCOVA (equal variance across groups,
homogeneity of variance) were met for all analyses
reported.
Table 2: Standardized Means (M) and Standard Deviations
(SD) for Dependent Variables, Separate for the AR and non-
AR group.
non-AR group
(n = 29)
AR group
(n = 27)
Cognitive load rating
ICL .22 (.15) .28 (.16)
ECL .17 (.17) .17 (.12)
GCL .67 (.20) .60 (.19)
Conceptual knowledge
Pre .40 (.12) .39 (.12)
Post .53 (.21) .48 (.18)
System usability 87.2 (12.6) 69.9 (17.7)
3.1 Effects on Cognitive Load
The internal consistencies for the cognitive load
scale by Leppink et al. (2013) as adapted by Thees
et al. (2020b) were acceptable to good (α
ICL
= 0.79,
α
ECL
= 0.75, α
GCL
= 0.84).
To evaluate significant group differences for each
type of load, an independent-samples t-test was ap-
plied to each subscale. No significant differences
were detected for ICL (t(52.6) = 1.38, p = .175),
ECL (t(50.8) = 0.08, p = .933) or GCL (t(54.0) =
1.39, p = .170).
3.2 Effects on Performance
The students’ performance was analyzed with an
analysis of variance for repeated measurements
(ANOVA-RM) with group as a between-subject fac-
tor and time as a within-subject factor. The analyses
showed a significant main effect of time (F(1,54) =
19.64, p < .001) with an effect size of η
2
p
= 0.27
(large effect). No significance was identified regard-
ing the main effect of group (F(1, 54) = 0.59, p =
.448) as well as the interaction between group and
time (F(1, 54) = 0.96, p = .330).
Utilizing the performance of the students in the
pretest as co-variate shows a significant correlation
with their performance in the posttest (F(1, 53) =
9.27, p = .004, r = .38). However, controlling for the
performance in the pretest using an analysis of covari-
ance (ANCOVA) the group difference in the posttest
stays not significant (F(1, 53) = 1.05, p = .311).
3.3 Usability
The internal consistency of the System Usability
Scale (Brooke, 1996) was nearly excellent (α = 0.88).
Following the scoring method by Brooke the av-
erage usability score is shown in Table 2. An
independent-samples t-test revealed a significant dif-
ference between the two conditions (t(46.64) = 4.18,
p < .001). In compliance with the review by Bangor
et al. (2009), the usability of the non-AR condition
can be classified as ”excellent” and that of the AR
condition as ”good”. These are the second and third
best rating-level classifications following ”best imag-
inable”.
4 DISCUSSION
The purpose of this study was to validate the re-
sults from Altmeyer et al. (2020) and Thees et al.
(2020a) in a graded physics laboratory course at a
German university. For this, the intervention and con-
trol groups from Thees et al. (2020a) were adopted
while adapting the instructions and tests to the differ-
ent target group. Both groups fulfilled the temporal
contiguity principle as described by the CTML.
Concerning the performance in the pretest the
groups were equally distributed. Both groups showed
a low intrinsic (ICL) and extraneous (ECL) cogni-
tive load and a high germane cognitive load (GCL)
with no significant differences. Importantly, both
reached a significant gain in their conceptual knowl-
edge (large effect) which is not guaranteed per se for
inquiry-based learning in a laboratory course (Hus-
naini and Chen, 2019; Kapici et al., 2019; Wilcox and
Lewandowski, 2017). However, we were unable to
confirm our hypotheses in form of a reduced extrane-
ous cognitive load and higher group-specific learning
gain in favor of the non-AR group (see Table 3).
Due to the limited field of view of the smart-
glasses, the AR group was unable to register all vi-
sualized measurement values at once. The non-AR
group in contrast was able to register them simultane-
ously due to the presentation on a tablet screen. The
The Effects of Augmented Reality: A Comparative Study in an Undergraduate Physics Laboratory Course
203
Table 3: Summary of main results regarding hypotheses.
Hypothesis (in short) Dependent Variable Hypothesis supported
H1) Less ECL for non-AR condition Subjective ECL rating score No
H2) Higher learning gain for non-AR condition Conceptual knowledge score No
predicted split-attention effect found by Thees et al.
(2020a) however seems to not show itself in the same
aspects in a real laboratory setting.
These results contrast not only the results pre-
sented by Thees et al. (2020a) and Altmeyer et al.
(2020) but also the reported benefits of the use of AR
versus non-AR in other studies (Garz
´
on and Acevedo,
2019; Ib
´
a
˜
nez and Delgado-Kloos, 2018; Strzys et al.,
2018). This may be due to the interaction between
participants in collaboration settings. As Janssen and
Kirschner (2020) showed, group interaction could
compensate for differences in cognitive load.
The usability score of the assistive systems
matched the results found in Thees et al. (2020a) and
showed a significantly lower score for the AR group
than the non-AR group. Nevertheless, they both can
still be categorized as acceptable. However, it is pos-
sible that the system usability has a higher influence
in a field setting compared to a laboratory setting re-
sulting in the contrasting results.
5 CONCLUSION AND FURTHER
RESEARCH
In summary, the present field study did not show dif-
ferences in extraneous cognitive load or knowledge
acquisition between the AR and the non-AR setting.
This contrasts previous results for the present subject
as well as other findings regarding the superiority of
AR in different subjects. However, these results do
not exclude a benefit of the use of AR in all cases.
Especially referring to the theory of spatial contiguity
failure, it is possible that the same digital assistance
system results in benefits when addressing different
experimental tasks.
Further studies should therefore evaluate the ef-
fects of this failure regarding two aspects: The use of
the more accessible format of tablet-based AR could
reduce the limitation of a small field of view and
therefore reduce the split between the visualized mea-
surement values. The same effect might be reached
using a future generation of smartglasses with an ex-
tended field of view. Additionally, a more complex
instruction, e.g. using more components inside the
circuits, might require more intrinsic and extraneous
processing, which could be addressed by the spatial
split-attention effect. A follow up study is intended
to evaluate the use of tablet-based AR in the same
graded laboratory course. In future studies the aspect
of collaboration could also be measured and imple-
mented as further dependent variable.
Although the present study and current research
shows the limitations of AR in educational settings,
they also show the possibilities of the use of AR. To-
gether with previous research it offers a more detailed
look into the requirements for a successful implemen-
tation.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
Support from the German Federal Ministry of Edu-
cation and Research (Bundesministerium f
¨
ur Bildung
und Forschung; BMBF) via the projects ”GeAR”
(Grant No. 01JD1811B) and ”gLabAssist” (Grant
No. 16DHL1022) is gratefully acknowledged.
We thank the Microelectronic System Design Re-
search Group/Technische Universit
¨
at Kaiserslautern
(chaired by Prof. Norbert Wehn), especially Frederik
Lauer and Carl Rheinl
¨
ander, for providing the hard-
ware components for the experiment.
REFERENCES
Ainsworth, S. (2006). Deft: A conceptual framework
for considering learning with multiple representations.
Learning and Instruction, 16(3):183–198.
Altmeyer, K., Kapp, S., Thees, M., Malone, S., Kuhn, J.,
and Br
¨
unken, R. (2020). The use of augmented reality
to foster conceptual knowledge acquisition in stem lab-
oratory courses — theoretical background and empiri-
cal results. British Journal of Educational Technology.
Advance online publication. doi:10.1111/bjet.12900.
Azuma, R. T. (1997). A survey of augmented reality.
Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments,
6(4):355–385.
Bangor, A., Kortum, P., and Miller, J. (2009). Determining
what individual sus scores mean: Adding an adjective
rating scale. Journal of Usability Studies, 4(3):114–
123.
Beege, M., Wirzberger, M., Nebel, S., Schneider, S.,
Schmidt, N., and Rey, G. D. (2019). Spatial continuity
effect vs. spatial contiguity failure. revising the effects
of spatial proximity between related and unrelated rep-
resentations. Frontiers in Education, 4:89.
Billinghurst, M. and Duenser, A. (2012). Augmented reality
in the classroom. Computer, 45(7):56–63.
CSEDU 2020 - 12th International Conference on Computer Supported Education
204
Brooke, J. (1996). Sus-a quick and dirty usability scale.
Usability evaluation in industry, 189(194):4–7.
Bujak, K. R., Radu, I., Catrambone, R., MacIntyre, B.,
Zheng, R., and Golubski, G. (2013). A psychological
perspective on augmented reality in the mathematics
classroom. Computers & Education, 68:536–544.
Cammeraat, S., Rop, G., and de Koning, B. B. (2020).
The influence of spatial distance and signaling on the
split-attention effect. Computers in Human Behavior,
105:106203.
de Jong, T. (2019). Moving towards engaged learning in
stem domains; there is no simple answer, but clearly a
road ahead. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning,
35(2):153–167.
de Jong, T., Ard Lazonder, Margus Pedaste, and Zacharias
Zacharia (2018). Simulations, games, and modeling
tools for learning. In Fischer, F., Hmelo-Silver, C. E.,
Goldman, S. R., and Reimann, P., editors, Interna-
tional Handbook of the Learning Sciences, pages 256–
266. Routledge, Taylor and Francis.
de Jong, T., Linn, M. C., and Zacharia, Z. C. (2013). Physi-
cal and virtual laboratories in science and engineering
education. Science (New York, N.Y.), 340(6130):305–
308.
Garz
´
on, J. and Acevedo, J. (2019). Meta-analysis of the im-
pact of augmented reality on students’ learning gains.
Educational Research Review, 27:244–260.
Ginns, P. (2006). Integrating information: A meta-analysis
of the spatial contiguity and temporal contiguity ef-
fects. Learning and Instruction, 16(6):511–525.
Husnaini, S. J. and Chen, S. (2019). Effects of guided in-
quiry virtual and physical laboratories on conceptual
understanding, inquiry performance, scientific inquiry
self-efficacy, and enjoyment. Physical Review Physics
Education Research, 15(1):31.
Ib
´
a
˜
nez, M.-B. and Delgado-Kloos, C. (2018). Augmented
reality for stem learning: A systematic review. Com-
puters & Education, 123:109–123.
Ivanjek, L., Schubatzky, T., Burde, J.-P., Hopf, M., Wil-
helm, T., Haagen-Sch
¨
utzenh
¨
ofer, C., Dopatka, L., and
Spatz, V. (2020). Development of a two-tier instrument
on simple electric circuits. Manuscript in preparation.
Janssen, J. and Kirschner, P. A. (2020). Applying collab-
orative cognitive load theory to computer-supported
collaborative learning: towards a research agenda.
Educational Technology Research and Development,
36(2):321.
Jones, K. A. and Sharma, R. S. (2019). An experiment in
blended learning: higher education without lectures.
International Journal of Digital Enterprise Technol-
ogy, 1(3):241.
Kapici, H. O., Akcay, H., and de Jong, T. (2019). Us-
ing hands-on and virtual laboratories alone or to-
gether—which works better for acquiring knowledge
and skills? Journal of Science Education and Technol-
ogy, 28(3):231–250.
Kapp, S., Thees, M., Strzys, M. P., Beil, F., Kuhn, J.,
Amiraslanov, O., Javaheri, H., Lukowicz, P., Lauer,
F., Rheinl
¨
ander, C., and Wehn, N. (2019). Augment-
ing kirchhoffs laws: Using augmented reality and
smartglasses to enhance conceptual electrical experi-
ments for high school students. The Physics Teacher,
57(1):52–53.
Kuhn, J., Lukowicz, P., Hirth, M., Poxrucker, A., Wepp-
ner, J., and Younas, J. (2016). gphysics—using smart
glasses for head-centered, context-aware learning in
physics experiments. IEEE Transactions on Learning
Technologies, 9(4):304–317.
Leppink, J., Paas, F., van der Vleuten, C. P. M., van Gog,
T., and van Merri
¨
enboer, J. J. G. (2013). Development
of an instrument for measuring different types of cog-
nitive load. Behavior research methods, 45(4):1058–
1072.
Leppink, J., Paas, F., van Gog, T., van der Vleuten, C. P.,
and van Merri
¨
enboer, J. J. (2014). Effects of pairs of
problems and examples on task performance and dif-
ferent types of cognitive load. Learning and Instruc-
tion, 30:32–42.
Mayer, R. E., editor (2005). The Cambridge handbook of
multimedia learning. Cambridge Univ. Press, Cam-
bridge.
Mayer, R. E. (2009). Multimedia Learning. Cambridge
University Press, Cambridge.
Mayer, R. E. and Moreno, R. (2003). Nine ways to reduce
cognitive load in multimedia learning. Educational
Psychologist, 38(1):43–52.
Mayer, R. E., Moreno, R., Boire, M., and Vagge, S. (1999).
Maximizing constructivist learning from multimedia
communications by minimizing cognitive load. Jour-
nal of Educational Psychology, 91(4):638–643.
Mayer, R. E. and Pilegard, C. (2014). Principles for manag-
ing essential processing in multimedia learning: Seg-
menting, pre-training, and modality principles. In
Mayer, R. E., editor, The Cambridge handbook of
multimedia learning, Cambridge handbooks in psy-
chology, pages 316–344. Cambridge University Press,
New York.
Moreno, R. (2006). Learning in high-tech and multime-
dia environments. Current Directions in Psychological
Science, 15(2):63–67.
Pundak, D. and Rozner, S. (2008). Empowering engi-
neering college staff to adopt active learning meth-
ods. Journal of Science Education and Technology,
17(2):152–163.
Rau, M. A. (2020). Comparing multiple theories about
learning with physical and virtual representations:
Conflicting or complementary effects? Educational
Psychology Review, 16(3):183.
Santos, M. E. C., Chen, A., Taketomi, T., Yamamoto, G.,
Miyazaki, J., and Kato, H. (2014). Augmented reality
learning experiences: Survey of prototype design and
evaluation. IEEE Transactions on Learning Technolo-
gies, 7(1):38–56.
Schroeder, N. L. and Cenkci, A. T. (2018). Spatial conti-
guity and spatial split-attention effects in multimedia
learning environments: a meta-analysis. Educational
Psychology Review, 30(3):679–701.
Strzys, M. P., Kapp, S., Thees, M., Klein, P., Lukowicz,
P., Knierim, P., Schmidt, A., and Kuhn, J. (2018).
Physics holo.lab learning experience: using smart-
glasses for augmented reality labwork to foster the
The Effects of Augmented Reality: A Comparative Study in an Undergraduate Physics Laboratory Course
205
concepts of heat conduction. European Journal of
Physics, 39(3):035703.
Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving:
Effects on learning. Cognitive Science, 12(2):257–
285.
Sweller, J. (2010). Element interactivity and intrinsic, extra-
neous, and germane cognitive load. Educational Psy-
chology Review, 22(2):123–138.
Sweller, J. and Chandler, P. (1994). Why some mate-
rial is difficult to learn. Cognition and Instruction,
12(3):185–233.
Sweller, J., van Merri
¨
enboer, J. J. G., and Paas, F.
(2019). Cognitive architecture and instructional de-
sign: 20 years later. Educational Psychology Review,
31(2):261–292.
Sweller, J., van Merrienboer, J. J. G., and Paas, F. G. W. C.
(1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional de-
sign. Educational Psychology Review, 10(3):251–296.
Thees, M., Altmeyer, K., Kapp, S., Rexigel, E., Beil, F.,
Klein, P., Malone, S., Br
¨
unken, R., and Kuhn, J.
(2020a). Augmented reality for inquiry learning in
stem laboratory courses: Opportunities and risks, but
no simple answers. Manuscript submitted for publica-
tion.
Thees, M., Kapp, S., Strzys, M. P., Beil, F., Lukowicz, P.,
and Kuhn, J. (2020b). Effects of augmented reality
on learning and cognitive load in university physics
laboratory courses. Computers in Human Behavior,
108:106316.
van Merri
¨
enboer, J. J. G. and Sweller, J. (2005). Cogni-
tive load theory and complex learning: Recent devel-
opments and future directions. Educational Psychol-
ogy Review, 17(2):147–177.
Vosniadou, S. (2007). Conceptual change and education.
Human Development, 50(1):47–54.
Wilcox, B. R. and Lewandowski, H. J. (2017). Develop-
ing skills versus reinforcing concepts in physics labs:
Insight from a survey of students’ beliefs about exper-
imental physics. Physical Review Physics Education
Research, 13(1):65.
CSEDU 2020 - 12th International Conference on Computer Supported Education
206