Stealing Time?: The Effect of Conscientiousness, Procedural Justice,
and Felt Accountability on Attitude towards Time Theft
Yudis Sekar Prasasti
1
, Corina Deborah Riantoputra
1
1
Industrial and Organizational Psychology, Universitas Indonesia, Depok, Jawa Barat Indonesia, 16424
Keyword: Felt accountability, time theft, attitude, procedural justice, conscientiousness
Abstract: Time theft results in heavy financial losses and interferes with employees’ sustainable innovation. However,
factors affecting it are relatively unexplored. Advancing theory of counterproductive work behavior, this
paper argues that felt accountability acts as a powerful moderator in the relationship between
conscientiousness and attitude towards time theft among 201 employees with a minimum education level of
associate's degree. The results show that (1) conscientiousness (α = 0.84) and procedural justice (α = 0.91)
have a negative and significant effect on attitude towards time theft, and (2) although felt accountability (α =
0.77) does not have a significant impact on attitude towards time theft (α = 0.86), it decreases the effect that
conscientiousness has on attitude.
1 INTRODUCTION
Organizations need not only profit but also
sustainability, making innovations a key aspect in
every organization. Unfortunately, sustainable
innovation efforts are hampered by employee time
theft behavior, which is defined as production
deviance behavior resulting in the reduction of time
working (Kulas, McInnerney, DeMuth and
Jadwinski, 2007). The fact is, 70% of employees
frequently commit time theft in their productive
working time in the office. Time theft is classified as
a major problem in organizations, because itis
commonly done by employees, causes financial loss
(Henle, Reeve and Pitts, 2010), decreases morality
standards (Liu and Berry, 2013), and is part of
counterproductive work behavior (Henle, Reeve and
Pitts, 2010). The focus of this research is to examine
attitude towards time theft that are positively related
to time theft behavior (Syakina, 2018).
Prior research shows significant relationships
between time theft and various individual variables
such as conscientiousness (Bolton, Becker and
Barbe, 2010), employee satisfaction (Kulas,
McInnerney, DeMuth and Jadwinski, 2007), and
cynicism (Lorinkova and Perry, 2017). Some
situational variables that have been found to have a
significant relationship with attitude towards time
theft are organizational justice (Greenberg, 1990),
perceived behavioral control (Henle, Reeve and
Pitts, 2010) and organizational climate (Kulas,
McInnerney, DeMuth and Jadwinski, 2007). To
understand this phenomenon comprehensively, this
research examines the internal and external
antecedents of time theft. Further, this research
argues that felt accountability is a significant
moderator in the relationship between
conscientiousness and attitude towards time theft.
One of the internal aspects chosen for this
research is felt accountability, which is defined as an
implicit or explicit expectation that one’s decisions
or actions will be subject to evaluation by some
salient audience(s) with the belief that there exists
the potential for one to receive either rewards or
sanctions based on this expected evaluation
(Hochwarter, Ferris, Gavin, Perrewé, Hall and Frink,
2007). Perceptions of high levels of accountability
are likely to indicate that self-interested behavior is
controlled and might lead to preventive action being
taken by the organization (Cohen, 2016). For
example, the presence of job standards, such as
productivity goals and accountability to meet those
goals, are also likely to reduce time banditry
(Martin, Brock, Buckley and Ketchen, 2010). If
effective controls are in place, individuals have a
harder time engaging in counterproductive acts
(Cohen, 2016). Consequently, employees with high
level of felt accountability tend to avoid time theft
Prasasti, Y. and Riantoputra, C.
Stealing Time?: The Effect of Conscientiousness, Procedural Justice, and Felt Accountability on Attitude towards Time Theft.
DOI: 10.5220/0010058800002917
In Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on Social Sciences, Laws, Arts and Humanities (BINUS-JIC 2018), pages 615-621
ISBN: 978-989-758-515-9
Copyright
c
2022 by SCITEPRESS Science and Technology Publications, Lda. All rights reserved
615
because they know that time theft evaluation will
result in penalties and sanctions.
H1 : Felt accountability negatively affects
attitude towards time theft.
Another individual aspect chosen is
conscientiousness because it is the strongest
indicator of counterproductive work behavior
(Chang and Smithikrai, 2010). Conscientiousness is
the degree to which a person is ambitious,
responsible, abides by ethical principles, and
considers the consequences of behavior before
acting (Eschleman, Bowling and LaHuis, 2015).
Conscientiousness is an important virtue for every
employee. Compared to those who are low in
conscientiousness, highly conscientious persons tend
to be more motivated to perform well on the job
(Judge and Ilies, 2002) and therefore are likely to
achieve better performance through careful
planning, goal setting, and persistence (Le, Oh,
Robbins, Ilies, Holland and Westrick, 2011).
High conscientiousness employee tends to be
ordered, controlled, organized, ambitious,
achievement focused, and self-disciplined (Feist and
Feist, 2008). These characteristics help people to
complete tasks, to be generally rule-abiding, and to
avoid impulsive behavior. Because
counterproductive work behavior (including time
theft) often involves a lack of effort, the failure to
follow organizational rules, or impulse-driven
actions, workers who are high in conscientiousness
are likely to avoid these behaviors (Bowling, Burns,
Stewart and Gruys, 2011). Furthermore,
conscientious individuals are noted for their
dependability and achievement orientation, and
therefore are more likely to have good capability to
regulate his/her behavior to follow organizational
norms and rules (Bolton, Becker and Barber, 2010).
A study on 234 employees from USA confirms that
conscientiousness has a negative relationship with
many kinds of counterproductive work behavior,
such as withdrawal and sabotage (Bolton, Becker
and Barber, 2010). Therefore, by extension, we
hypothesize that:
H2 : Conscientiousness negatively affects
attitude towards time theft.
Interestingly, there is an indication that
employees with high conscientiousness are more
likely to commit time theft as a form of
counterproductive work behavior towards
organization in certain situations, for example, when
there is no other way of solving problems and when
their personal resources are threatened (Eschleman,
Bowling and LaHuis, 2015). This means that there is
also an indication that conscientiousness has a
significant effect on counterproductive work
behavior towards organization, and by extension,
may affect time theft. Therefore, research on the
relationship between conscientiousness and various
kinds of counterproductive work behavior should
show other variables that interact with
conscientiousness to more comprehensively explain
the dynamic connections between the two variables.
We argue that felt accountability potentially acts
as a moderator in the relationship between
conscientiousness and attitude towards time theft.
Felt accountability interacts with individual
characteristics and contextual factors that affect the
decision making and behaviors of employees
(Lerner and Tetlock, 1999). That is, individuals in
power who know they will be accountable for
something are more likely to consider social
consequences and take others’ interests into account.
This condition indicates that the presence of felt
accountability in employees who have high
conscientiousness makes them less likely to commit
time theft even in urgent situations, because they
understand that rewards or punishments will be
given based on evaluations of their behavior. The
evaluation is important for conscientious employees
because they have high achievement orientation
(Bolton, Becker and Barber, 2010). Therefore, felt
accountability is the most suitable moderator in the
relationship between conscientiousness and time
theft.
H3 : Felt accountability moderates the effect of
conscientiousness on attitude towards time theft.
Specifically, felt accountability increases the
negative effect of conscientiousness on attitude
towards time theft.
The external aspect chosen is perceived
organizational justice because it is one of the
strongest external predictors of counterproductive
work behavior (Krischer, Penney and Hunter, 2010).
Perceived organizational justice refers to perceived
fairness of interactions between individuals and
organizations (Krischer, Penney and Hunter, 2010).
Two of the most widely studied forms of
organizational justice are distributive justice
(perceived fairness of reward allocation) and
procedural justice (perceived fairness of the
processes by which decisions are made) (Krischer,
Penney and Hunter, 2010). This research only
focuses on procedural justice because it is the
strongest predictor for counterproductive work
BINUS-JIC 2018 - BINUS Joint International Conference
616
behavior (Devonish and Greenidge, 2010). An
empirical study on 211 employees across nine
organizations from the private and public sectors in
a developing country in the Caribbean shows that,
compared to other forms of organizational justice,
procedural justice has the strongest relationship with
the counterproductive work behavior towards
organizations (Devonish and Greenidge, 2010).
The relationship between procedural justice
and time theft is explained well by the social
exchange theory, with one of its basic tenets that for
relationships to evolve over time, they need trust,
loyalty, and mutual commitments. To do so, parties
must abide by certainrules of exchange. In this
way, rules and norms of exchange are “the
guidelines” of exchange processes (Cropanzano and
Mitchell, 2005). Individuals tend to do to others
what they perceive the other parties do to them. That
is, they maintain that, "If they treat me well, I am
going to treat them well". In addition, consideration
of fairness and justice comes to play (Stafford,
2008). In a workplace context, when employees
perceive that their organizations treat them well,
they tend to avoid time theft or other
counterproductive work behaviors. Specifically, it is
contended that perceived injustice of any type may
relate to counterproductive work behavior because
employees who feel unfairly treated may reduce
their cooperative behaviors to avoid exploitation
(Jones, 2009). Because perceived procedural justice
is mostly influenced by the way an organization acts
towards employee, a lack of it may lead to
counterproductive work behavior towards
organization, including time theft (Jones, 2009).
H4: Procedural justice negatively affects attitude
towards time theft.
2 METHOD
2.1 Sample and Procedure
Figure 1: Hypothetical Model (Note: (+) Positive
Prediction and (-) Negative Prediction).
We managed to collect 82 responses from an online
questionnaire and 119 responses from an offline
questionnaire. The sample (59.7% male and 40.3%
female) came from a variety of organization types,
which are categorized into two groups:
government (49.3%) and non-government (50.7%),
to simplify the statistical analysis. With regard to
their employee status, 60.2% were permanent and
39.8% were non-permanent employees. The range of
their job tenure was from 3 months to 36 years, and
their lowest educational degree was an associate's
degree. Having an associate's degree as the lowest
level of educational attainment was necessary
because employees with lower educational levels
tend to commit time theft more than those with high
education levels (Ganzach, 1998).
2.2 Measurement
All instruments used in this research were adapted
from previous studies' instruments, the validity of
which had been tested. Adaptation was done by
performing a back translation on previously
translated instruments. Before data collection, a pilot
study was conducted and some items were revised to
enhance their psychometric attributes. Every
instrument had a 6-point Likert scale (strongly
disagree to strongly agree). Reliability analysis for
all instruments used the internal consistency method,
with good reliability indicated by a score between
0.7 and 0.8 (Kaplan and Saccuzzo, 2005). Further,
we also assessed the Standardized Root Mean
Square Residual (SRMR). A good SRMR score
should be below 0.8 (Henseler, Hubona and Ray,
2016). Based on those standards, all instruments
used in this research were found to have good
reliability and SRMR scores, indicating all items in
each instrument were fit to measure the intended
construct.
2.2.1 Attitude towards Time Theft
Attitude towards time theft was measured using a
case-scenario questionnaire (α = 0.86, SRMR =
0.13), which is a questionnaire that presents some
cases and then asks participants to respond to the
cases. Cases pertaining to time theft in an Indonesian
context were used (Syakina, 2018). One example is
a case of an employee extending meal break by
saying he/she needs time to pray. Every scenario is
then followed by eight statements adapted from time
theft survey (Kulas, McInnerney, DeMuth and
Jadwinski, 2007).Example items are: “In my
opinion, that behavior is not acceptable” or “In my
Stealing Time?: The Effect of Conscientiousness, Procedural Justice, and Felt Accountability on Attitude towards Time Theft
617
opinion, that behavior is counterproductive” (reverse
coded).
2.2.2 Conscientiousness
Conscientiousness was measured using a
questionnaire (α = 0.84, SRMR = 0.13). An example
of the items is: “I pay attention to details” (Frink and
Ferris, 1999).
2.2.3 Felt Accountability
Felt Accountability was measured using a
questionnaire (α = 0.77, SRMR = 0.08). A sample
item is: “I am held accountable for my actions at
work” (Hochwarter, Ferris, Gavin, Perrewé, Hall
and Frink, 2007).
2.2.4 Procedural Justice
Procedural justice was measured using a
questionnaire (α = 0.91, SRMR = 0.03). An example
of one of the items is: “I can count on my
organization to have fair policies” (Fuchs, 2011).
3 RESULT
Bi-correlation analysis results are shown in Table 1.
From this table, it is evident that gender is related to
attitude towards time theft (r = 0.21, p < 0.01),
which requires control over its effect in the first step
of the regression analysis. Therefore, we conducted
a two-step regression analysis. In the first step,
gender functioned as the independent variable. In the
second step, standardized mean scores of
conscientiousness, felt accountability, procedural
justice, and the interaction between
conscientiousness sand felt accountability acted as
independent variables. The results from the second
step of regression yielded R
2
= 0.38, which indicates
that 38% of the variance of attitude towards time
theft can be explained by the model proposed. Felt
Accountability was not found to affect attitude
towards time theft (β = -0.03, ns), and therefore H1
is not supported. Meanwhile, conscientiousness
negatively affects attitude towards time theft (β = -
0.29, p < 0.01), thus providing support for H2.
Table 1: Bi-correlation.
M
ean SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1. Felt Accountabilit
y
4.73 0.66 -
2.Conscientiousness 4.76 0.85 0.26** -
3. Procedural Justice 4.27 1.18 0.37** 0.04 -
4. Attitude towards Time Theft 2.84 0.84 -0.17* -0.27** -0.36** -
5. Gende
r
a
0.40 0.49 0.26** 0.06 0.09 0.21** -
6. Or
g
anization T
yp
e
b
0.49 0.50 0.02 -0.08 0.06 -0.05 0.04 -
7. Em
p
lo
y
ee Status
c
0.60 0.50 0.07 -0.12 0.05 0.13 0.13 -0.1 -
8. Tenure
d
1.06 1.27 0.10 -0.01 0.11 -0.15* 0.01 0.67 0.25 -
Note: N=201. *p<0.05, **p<0.01.
a
0=Female, 1=Male.
b
0=Non-Government Organization, 1=Government Organization.
c
0=Non-Permanent, 1=Permanent.
d
0=0-2 years, 1=3-5 years, 2=6-10 years, 3=Above 10 years
Figure 2: Slope Analysis.
Felt accountability is a significant moderator in the
effect of conscientiousness on attitude towards time
theft (β = -0.15, p <0.01). Based on the slope
analysis in Figure 2, it can be seen that conscientious
employees with high felt accountability have more
negative attitudes towards time theft (against time
theft) than conscientious employees with low felt
accountability (H3 is supported). Lastly, procedural
justice negatively affects attitudes towards time theft
(β = -0.33, p < 0.01) (H4 is supported).
BINUS-JIC 2018 - BINUS Joint International Conference
618
4 DISCUSSION
First, this research successfully built and tested
hypotheses about individual and organizational
factors influencing attitude towards time theft. The
result shows that there are two factors that
independently affect attitude towards time theft
(conscientiousness and procedural justice),
suggesting that organizations need to pay attention
to both factors to prevent time theft. We extended
the results of a research in Malaysia that showed
negative effects of conscientiousness on time theft
among undergraduate students. The current research
demonstrates that this negative effect is also
prevalent in workers (not only students) (Hafidz,
2012). Further, differing from other research
(Hafidz, 2012) that used a survey questionnaire, the
current research employed a case-scenario
questionnaire. Given the consistency of the finding,
there is a strong indication that the negative effect of
conscientiousness on attitude towards time theft is
stable across cultures, types of participants (students
and employees),as well as types of instruments (case
scenario and usual survey). Moreover, the
significant relationship between procedural justice
and attitude towards time theft aligns with the result
of a survey carried out in Canada on 424 employed
students, which examined the relationship between
types of injustice and general counterproductive
work behavior (such as time theft, and property
theft) (Jones, 2009). Taken together these
consistency indicates the importance of individual
and organizational factors in counterproductive work
behaviour. However, further research is needed to
confirm this assumption.
Second, this research contributes to the current
understanding of factors influencing attitude towards
time theft by showing that felt accountability acts as
a significant moderator in the negative effect of
conscientiousness on attitude towards time theft.
With high levels of felt accountability, conscientious
employees’ tendency to commit time theft is lower
because interaction between felt accountability and
conscientiousness increases employee’s personal
sense of power. Individuals with high sense of
personal power tend to act in in socially appropriate
ways (Keltner, Gruenfeld and Anderson, 2003), such
as not doing time theft.
Table 2: Moderated Regression Analysis.
Step 1 Step 2
B SE Β
T
BSE
β
T
Gende
0.36 0.12 0.21** 3.06 0.48 0.11 0.28** 4.46
Felt accountabilit
y
(FA)
-0.04 0.09 -0.03 -0.42
Conscientiousness (C)
-0.29 0.06 -0.29** -4.53
Procedural
j
ustice (PJ)
-0.23 0.05 -0.33** -4.83
C x FA
-0.12 0.05 -0.15** -2.28
F
F
(1,199) =9.33
F
(5,195) = 15.21
F
chan
g
e Δ
F
(1,199) = 9.33 Δ
F
(4,195) = 15.97
R
2
0.05 0.38
R
2
chan
g
e 0.05 0.24
Note: Significance at: *p<0.05, **p<0.01
Third, this research highlights the importance of
gender in attitude towards time theft. Analysis
shows that male participants tend to have a more
positive attitude towards time theft compared to their
female counterparts. This result is consistent with a
survey (Henle, 2005) conducted in USA on 272
psychology and business undergraduate students,
that examines some antecedents of general
counterproductive work behaviortoward other
people (such as bullying) and toward organization
(such as withdrawal, and sabotage). It found that
male participants are more frequent to do
counterproductive work behavior. More research is
needed to uncover why and when gender affect
counterproductive behavior.
Our study leads to several practical implications.
First, companies should hire employees with high
conscientiousness to fill positions in which
committing time theft is a big possibility. Highly
conscientious employees have strong inhibitive
tendencies that deter destructive behavior. Their
thoughtful and planned nature drives them to
consider the consequences of deviant behavior, and
their concern for moral obligations preclude them
from engaging in behaviors that violate their
integrity (Mawritz, Dust and Resick 2014). Second,
it is recommended that companies attempt to
Stealing Time?: The Effect of Conscientiousness, Procedural Justice, and Felt Accountability on Attitude towards Time Theft
619
increase employees' perceptions of justice to reduce
time theft because according to the social exchange
theory, employees will ‘return’ the justice offered by
the organization by avoiding time theft (Cropanzano,
Prehar and Chen, 2002). Third, companies should
consider introducing an intervention to increase felt
accountability, such as through peer and supervisor
reviews on employees' time theft behavior and
applying sanctions based on the reviews. Those
three practices may support sustainable development
goals by facilitating employees to use their time
effectively and contribute more to general economic
growth.
4.1 Conclusion
This study examines the effect of conscientiousness,
felt accountability, procedural justice, and the
interaction effect of conscientiousness and felt
accountability on attitude towards time theft. The
results show that conscientiousness and procedural
justice negatively affect attitude towards time theft,
and the interaction effect of accountability weakens
the effect of conscientiousness on attitude towards
time theft. The current research advances literature
on counterproductive behavior by showing that felt
accountability acts as a powerful moderator to
increase productive work behavior. Thus, this
research contributes to the sustainable development
goal, especially the 8
th
goal on economic growth, by
suggesting that companies should be attention on
variables such as conscientiousness, procedural
justice and felt accountability. By controlling such
variables, employees are expected to avoid time
theft and have more time to be productive and
innovative.
REFERENCES
Bolton, L.R., Becker, L.K. and Barber, L.K. (2010) ‘Big
Five trait predictors of differential counterproductive
work behavior dimensions’, Personality and
Individual Differences, vol. 49, pp. 537-541.
Bowling, N.A., Burns, G.N., Stewart, S.M. and Gruys,
M.L. (2011) ‘Conscientiousness and agreeableness as
moderators of the relationship between neuroticism
and counterproductive work behaviors: A constructive
replication’, International Journal of Selection and
Assessment, vol. 19, pp. 320-330.
Chang, K. and Smithikrai, C. (2010) ‘Counterproductive
behaviour at work: an investigation into reduction
strategies’, The International Journal of Human
Resource Management’ vol. 21, pp. 1272-1288.
Cohen, A. (2016) ‘Are they among us? A conceptual
framework of the relationship between the dark triad
personality and counterproductive work behaviors
(CWBs)’, Human Resource Management Review, vol.
26, pp. 69-85.
Cropanzano, R., Prehar, C.A. and Chen, P.Y. (2002)
‘Using social exchange theory to distinguish
procedural from interactional justice’, Group &
Organization Management¸ vol. 27, pp. 324-351.
Cropanzano, R. and Mitchell, M.S. (2005) ‘Social
exchange theory: An interdisciplinary
review’, Journal of Management, vol. 31, pp. 874-900.
Devonish, D. and Greenidge, D. (2010) ‘The effect of
organizational justice on contextual performance,
counterproductive work behaviors, and task
performance: Investigating the moderating role of
ability-based emotional intelligence’, International
Journal of Selection and Assessment, vol. 18, pp. 75-
86.
Eschleman, K.J., Bowling, N.A. and LaHuis, D. (2015)
‘The moderating effects of personality on the
relationship between change in work stressors and
change in counterproductive work
behaviours’. Journal of Occupational and
Organizational Psychology, vol. 88, pp. 656-678.
Feist, J. and Feist, G.J. (2008) Theories of Personality.
New York: McGraw-Hill.
Frink, D.D. and Ferris, G.R. (1999) ‘The moderating
effects of accountability on the conscientiousness-
performance relationship’, Journal of Business and
Psychology, vol. 13, pp. 515-524.
Fuchs, S. (2011) ‘The impact of manager and top
management identification on the relationship between
perceived organizational justice and change-oriented
behavior’, Leadership & Organization Development
Journal, vol. 32, pp. 555-583.
Ganzach, Y. (1998) ‘Intelligence and job
satisfaction’, Academy of Management Journal, vol.
41, pp. 526-539.
Greenberg, J. (1990) ‘Employee theft as a reaction to
underpayment inequity: The hidden cost of pay cuts’,
Journal of Applied Psychology, vol. 75, pp. 561-568.
Hafidz, S.W.M. (2012) ‘Individual differences as
antecedents of counterproductive work
behaviour’, Asian Social Science, vol. 8, pp. 220-226.
Henle, C.A., Reeve, C.L. and Pitts, V.E. (2010) ‘Stealing
time at work: Attitudes, social pressure, and perceived
control as predictors of time theft’, Journal of
Business Ethics, vol. 94, pp. 53-67.
Henle, C.A. (2005) Predicting workplace deviance from
the interaction between organizational justice and
personality’, Journal of Managerial Issues, vol. 17,
pp. 247-263.
Henseler, J., Hubona, G. and Ray, P.A. (2016) ‘Using PLS
path modeling in new technology research: updated
guidelines’, Industrial Management & Data Systems,
vol. 116, pp. 2-20.
Hochwarter, W., Ferris, G., Gavin, M., Perrewé, P., Hall,
A. and Frink, D. (2007) ‘Political skill as a moderator
of the felt accountability–job performance
relationship: Longitudinal convergence of mediated
BINUS-JIC 2018 - BINUS Joint International Conference
620
moderation results’, Organizational Behavior and
Human Decision Processes, vol. 102, pp. 226-239.
Jones, D.A. (2009) ‘Getting even with one's supervisor
and one's organization: Relationships among types of
injustice, desires for revenge, and counterproductive
work behaviors’, Journal of Organizational Behavior:
The International Journal of Industrial, Occupational
and Organizational Psychology and Behavior, vol. 30,
pp. 525-542.
Judge, T.A. and Ilies, R. (2002) ‘Relationship of
personality to performance motivation: A meta-
analytic review’, Journal of Applied Psychology, vol.
87, pp. 797–807.
Kaplan, R.M. and Saccuzzo, D.P. (2005) Psychological
Testing : Principles, Application and Issue. Belmont:
Wadsworth.
Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D.H. and Anderson, C. (2003)
‘Power, approach, and inhibition’, Psychological
Review, vol. 110, pp. 265-284.
Krischer, M.M., Penney, L.M. and Hunter, E.M. (2010)
‘Can counterproductive work behaviors be
productive? CWB as emotion-focused
coping’, Journal of Occupational Health Psychology,
vol. 15, pp. 154-166.
Kulas, J.T., McInnerney, J.E., DeMuth, R.F. and
Jadwinski, V. (2007) ‘Employee satisfaction and theft:
Testing climate perceptions as a mediator’, The
Journal of Psychology, vol. 141, pp. 389-402.
Le, H., Oh, I.S., Robbins, S.B., Ilies, R., Holland, E. and
Westrick, P. (2011) ‘Too much of a good thing:
Curvilinear relationships between personality traits
and job performance’, Journal of Applied Psychology,
vol. 96, pp. 113-133.
Lerner, J.S. and Tetlock, P.E. (1999) ‘Accounting for the
effects of accountability’, Psychological Bulletin, vol.
125, pp. 255-275.
Liu, Y. and Berry, C.M. (2013) ‘Identity, moral, and
equity perspectives on the relationship between
experienced injustice and time theft’, Journal of
Business Ethics, vol. 118, pp. 73-83.
Lorinkova, N.M. and Perry, S.J. (2017) ‘When is
empowerment effective? The role of leader-leader
exchange in empowering leadership, cynicism, and
time theft’, Journal of Management, vol. 43, pp. 1631-
1654.
Martin, L.E., Brock, M.E., Buckley, M.R. and Ketchen,
D.J. (2010) ‘Time banditry: Examining the purloining
of time in organizations’, Human Resource
Management Review, vol. 20, pp. 26–34.
Mawritz, M.B., Dust, S.B. and Resick, C.J. (2014)
‘Hostile climate, abusive supervision, and employee
coping: Does conscientiousness matter?’, Journal of
Applied Psychology, vol. 99, pp. 737-747.
Stafford, L. (2008) Engaging theories in interpersonal
communication: Multiple perspectives in Baxter, L.A.
and Braithwaite, D.O. (eds.) California: Sage
Publication.
Syakina, D. (2018) The Relationship between
Consciencetiousness, Self Control, Perceived
Supervisor Behavioral Integrity and Attitude toward
Time Theft Moderated by Social Cohesion,
Postgraduate, Univeristy of Indonesia.
Stealing Time?: The Effect of Conscientiousness, Procedural Justice, and Felt Accountability on Attitude towards Time Theft
621