The Cultural Role on Emotional Competence Development in Early
Childhood
Yettie Wandansari
1,2
, Dewi Retno Suminar
2
, and Tina Afiatin
3
1
Faculty of Psychology, Widya Mandala Surabaya Catholic University, Surabaya - Indonesia
2
Faculty of Psychology, Universitas Airlangga, Surabaya - Indonesia
3
Faculty of Psychology, Gajah Mada University, Yogyakarta - Indonesia
Keywords: Culture, Emotional Competence, Early Childhood.
Abstract: Emotional competence in early childhood is a multi-dimension construct consists of emotion knowledge,
emotion expression, and emotion regulation. Previous research findings consistently reported emotional
competence as the predictor of the child's school and social competence. One of the factors that are
influencing the development of emotional competence is the cultural context. Culture does not only affect
the way children recognizes his/her own emotion and the emotion of others, but also determine how an
individual expresses emotion and regulates emotion in socially acceptable ways. This literature review aims
to explore previous research findings and theoretical approach of cultural roles on emotional competence,
which can be used as a foundation to develop a culture-based theory of emotional competence and culture-
based interventions to optimize early childhood emotional competence. A number of research articles that
specifically examine the relationship between culture and emotional competence were obtained through
electronic journal articles databases (Proquest, Science Direct, and Google Scholar) in span of time
publication between 2008-2017, with keywords culture and emotional competence, emotional
understanding, emotional expression, and emotional regulation. The findings suggest the cultural role on
emotional competence in early childhood as a developmental context through parental emotion
socialization. Recommendations for further research in methods of assessment dan culture-specific
emotions are discussed.
1 INTRODUCTION
Emotional competence in the context of early
childhood is defined as the ability to express
emotions, regulate emotional expressions and
emotional experiences, and understand the emotions
of oneself and the emotions of others (Denham
1998). Emotionally competence preschoolers are
able to express a variety of emotions, which
increasingly fit into the social-cultural context
(Denham et al., 2016). At that age, they also
experience a decrease in anger, frustration, and
tantrum behavior, as well as learn to direct and
monitor their behavior, express their feelings
verbally, and use language to influence others to
meet their needs and goals, which indicate the
existence of emotional regulation development (La
Freniere, 2000). Preschoolers are also capable of
labelling various feelings of self and others,
demonstrating an understanding of emotional
experiences, and demonstrating the ability to predict
emotional reactions (LaFreniere, 2000).
Emotional competence will help the child's
success in fulfilling the developmental task of
preschool age (Denham et al., 2016). Some
developmental tasks of preschool age are building
positive relationships with the social environment,
managing emotional impulses in social interaction,
staying connected to adults while shifting to peers,
as well as learning skills such as concentrating and
following teacher direction (Denham et al., 2016).
The importance of emotional competence for
children is also supported by some previous studies.
Emotional competence correlated with children's
social competence (Carlo et al., 2012; Denham et al.,
2015; Farina and Belacchi, 2014; Herts, et al., 2012;
McLaughlin et al., 2011). Besides, emotional
competence is also correlated with the child's
academic competence (Denham et al., 2013;
Herndon et al., 2013; Magdalena, 2013).
82
Wandansari, Y., Suminar, D. and Afiatin, T.
The Cultural Role on Emotional Competence Development in Early Childhood.
DOI: 10.5220/0008585600820090
In Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on Psychology in Health, Educational, Social, and Organizational Settings (ICP-HESOS 2018) - Improving Mental Health and Harmony in
Global Community, pages 82-90
ISBN: 978-989-758-435-0
Copyright
c
2020 by SCITEPRESS – Science and Technology Publications, Lda. All rights reserved
One of the factors that influence the development
of children's emotional competence is the cultural
context. Unlike the emotional intelligence that is
innate potential, emotional competence is a number
of skills acquired or developed through cultural and
contextual influences (Lau and Wu, 2012). This
article aims to explore previous research findings
and theoretical approach of cultural roles on
emotional competence and its impact on emotional
competence development of early childhood. It is
expected to stimulate further research to develop a
cultural-sensitive framework of emotional
competence and intervention to optimize emotional
competence in early childhood.
1.1 Emotional Competence
Emotional competence is a construct that
encompasses three dimensions, namely
understanding emotions, expressing emotions, and
regulating emotions (Denham, 2016). The first
dimension is emotion knowledge, which includes an
understanding of basic emotions, as well as
emotional expressions, situations, causes, and
consequences, an understanding of more complex
emotions, and using of display rules, mixed
emotions, and more complex emotions such as
shame and guilt (Denham et al., 2007). The second
dimension is emotion expression, which refers to the
specific emotions showed by the child with varying
purposes, and the overall expression of emotions
(Denham et al., 2007). The third dimension is
emotion regulation, which is done by the child when
the child's emotional experience is too severe, or the
child's emotional expression is inconsistent with the
expectations of others, by using physical, cognitive
or behavioral strategies to overcome internal
emotional experiences or external emotional
expression (Denham et al., 2007).
1.2 The Individualistic-versus-
Collectivistic Cultural Framework
Culture values does not only affect the way each
ethnic group identifies emotions, but also how they
express and manage emotions in socially acceptable
ways. Culture would restrict and regulate to whom,
when and where any person may express and
conceal certain emotions, as well as in the way to
express emotions through nonverbal behaviors and
facial expressions (Kurniawan and Hasanat, 2007).
According to Matsumoto et al. (2008a), the role of
culture can be explained by one of dimensions of
Hofstede's cultural values of individualism-
collectivism (IC). IC dimensions have four
attributes, namely self, purpose, relationships, and
determinants of behavior. Individualistic culture
supports independent self-development, emphasizes
personal goals, encourages rationality and
interpersonal relations, and places attitudes as a
determinant of behavior. Free emotional expression
confirms the importance of individuals more than
groups. On the contrary, collectivistic culture
supports an interdependent self, emphasizes group
goals, encourages communal relations, and places
norms as determinants of behavior. In addition, there
is a within-culture diversity, namely interindividual
variability or individual differences in norms of
expression in various contexts and emotions, and
intraindividual variability or variation in individuals
in various contexts and emotions (Matsumoto et al.,
2008a ).
Furthermore, Halberstadt and Lozada (2011)
describe that the culture of collectivism emphasizes
interdependence, group identity, interpersonal
harmony, and the achievement of shared goals;
emotions are characterized by self-control to
promote interpersonal harmony and group goals. In
contrast, the culture of individualism emphasizes
independence, individual identity, personal
assertion, and achievement of personal goals;
emotions are perceived to be unique to the
individual, emotion expressiveness is encouraged,
and emotions are identified on the basis of
subjective experience. Parents with collectivism
cultural backgrounds tend to support the children
individually (see Halberstadt and Lozada, 2011).
Matsumoto et al. (2008) also emphasized that the
cultural display rules are an important concept to
explain cultural differences in emotional expression.
In Indonesia particularly, children would be
expected to control the overt expression of negative
emotions and emotionally driven negative behaviors,
so that they behave in a manner that promotes group
harmony and avoids interpersonal conflict
(Eisenberg , Pidada, and Liew, 2001).
2 METHOD
A number of research articles that examine the
relationship between culture and emotional
competence were obtained through some search
systems for electronic journal articles databases,
such as Proquest, Science Direct, and Google
Scholar in span of time publication between 2008-
2017, with keywords culture and emotional
competence, emotional understanding, emotional
The Cultural Role on Emotional Competence Development in Early Childhood
83
expression, and emotional regulation. The articles
selection criteria are specifically examining the
relationship between culture and emotional
competence.
3 RESULTS
3.1 Culture and Emotion Recognition
or Emotion Understanding
There is limited amount of cross-cultural research on
emotion recognition or emotion understanding. One
of them is a study of 120 participants from
Germany, Rome, and Indonesia by Jurgens et al.
(2013). They reported that emotion recognition is
influenced by the complex interactions between
universal and cultural factors. They used
conversations that contain angry, sad, happy, and
frightened emotions from German radio. These
conversations are played back by professional
actors, consisting of spontaneous emotional
conversations and acted conversations. The result
indicates that participants from Germany showed
better emotion recognition, but there is no difference
between Romanian and Indonesian in the overall
emotion recognition. In short, these findings confirm
that the cultural-specific differences in emotion
recognition or understanding.
3.2 Culture and Emotion Expression
The results of cross-cultural studies show that there
are cultural-specific differences in emotion
expression. Wilson et al. (2012) compared the
emotional expressions of 180 children aged 6-9
years old from city India, suburban India, and
suburban United States. Compared to US Children,
Indian children are less likely to express their anger,
sadness, and pain. They are also less likely to exhibit
verbal expressions directly. The Indian child states
the reason for controlling angry and sad emotions is
to keep the social norm, while the U.S. child
perceives that express all emotions is to
communicate perceived emotions.
Similarly, Lewis, Takai-Kawakami, and Sullivan
(2010) asserted cultural-specific differences in
emotional expression of preschoolers. They
investigated the emotional responses to success and
failure of 149 Japanese, African American, and
White American mixed European preschoolers. The
results indicate that Japanese children express less
shame, pride, and sadness, but more exposure and
evaluative embarrassment. American children
indicate more evaluative embarrassment. This
finding confirmed that success and failure are
interpreted differently by Japanese preschoolers. The
low amount of sadness and shame expression, and
the limited range of numbers of different expressions
observed in the Japanese children and the children of
East Asian infants and young children differ from
Western infants and children primarily in the display
of negative expressions. These results demonstrate
that cultural differences influence how children
respond to achievement situations.
In addition, Adrianson and Rhamdani (2014)
described the cultural-specific experiences of envy
in two cultures, namely Indonesia and Sweden. The
research findings show that in the Indonesian
language there is a broader envy meaning and
emotional vocabulary that is rarely found in
Swedish. The Swedish's description of envy relates
to a malicious (ill will) meaning, while the Javanese
use the concept of envy without ill will. Jealousy
and envy seem to overlap each other more in Bahasa
Indonesia than in the Swedish. Swedish had
schadenfreude that was lacking in Bahasa Indonesia.
According to the Swedish respondents, the central
feature of envy is wanting to have what other person
possesses, such as prosperity or competence. On the
other hand, the Javanese respondents stress more on
relationships, achievements and personal
characteristics as primary causes for envy.
However, there are also some findings that
indicate inter-cultural similarities as well as culture-
specific differences in emotional expression.
Matsumoto et al. (2008b) involved more than 5,000
respondents in 32 countries completed the Display
Rule Inventory Assessment. They reported that there
are some universal effects, including greater
expression toward in-groups, and an overall
regulation effect. Respondents with a background of
individualistic and collectivistic cultures differed on
expressivity and norms concerning specific emotions
in group situations.
Also, Tsai et al., (2016) examined the effect of
positive and negative emotions on interpersonal and
intrapersonal function at 155 Asian American and 74
European American college students. The results
reported no cultural differences in the tendency to
express positive and negative emotions, or in the
relationship between positive emotional expression
and intrapersonal function. Nevertheless, there are
differences in ethnic groups in the relationship
between the expression of negative emotions and
their functions. In contrast to European American
respondents, Asian Americans respondents with
negative emotional expression show lower
ICP-HESOS 2018 - International Conference on Psychology in Health, Educational, Social, and Organizational Settings
84
intrapersonal functions. Individuals who have a high
level of interdependence have a positive emotional
expression that correlates with a higher positive
mood, lower interpersonal problems, and lower
depressive symptoms.
Likewise, Novin, Riefe, and Mo (2010) examine
the role of individualistic versus collectivistic goals
and the presence of an authority figure (father)
versus an equal status figure (peer) on 24 Dutch and
23 South Korean children's negative emotions
experience and emotion expression motives. The
results indicate the cultural similarity of the
situational goals and audience and the cultural
differences in their emotional expression. More
specifically, cultural differences in how negative
emotions would be expressed in ‘father’ situations
but not in 'peer' situations. There is also a cultural
difference in children's motives for emotion
expression in situations with collectivistic goals.
Dutch children's emotion expressions are more
context-sensitive than South Korean children.
Also, Safdar et al., (2009) compares emotional
display rules of 835 Canadians, US Americans, and
Japanese university students across as well as within
cultures. The results indicate that Japanese display
rules permit the expression of anger, contempt, and
disgust significantly less than the North American.
Japanese also think that they should express
happiness and surprise significantly less than the
Canadian.
In brief, these results indicate that cultural
context partly explained cultural differences in
emotional expression.
3.3 Culture and Emotion Regulation
Research findings of emotion regulation among
cultures also indicated the differences and
similarities. The cultural differences in emotion
regulation are reported by Arens, Balkir, and
Barnow (2012). They investigated the emotion
regulation of 26 healthy and 25 depressed German
women and 28 healthy and 29 depressed Turkish
immigrants living in Germany. Healthy Turkish
immigrants reported higher balance infrequently
using of suppression and frequently using of
reappraisal, which was associated with more positive
outcomes of expressive suppression. Both of patient
samples showed great use of emotional suppression
than cognitive reappraisal. Results suggest that the
mediating cultural role between emotional
suppression and well-being is associated with
greater emotion regulation balance in healthy
Turkish.
Moreover, Matsumoto, Nakagawa and Yoo
(2008a) confirmed differences of emotion regulation
process across 23 countries. Cultures that
emphasized the maintenance of social order tended
to have higher scores on suppression, and
reappraisal and suppression tended to be positively
correlated. In contrast, cultures that minimized the
maintenance of social order and valued individual
affective autonomy and egalitarianism tended to
have lower scores on suppression, and reappraisal
and suppression tended to be negatively correlated.
Miyamoto, Ma and Petermann (2014) examined
whether there are cultural differences in emotional
regulation based on Eastern belief about negative
emotions after experiencing a negative event on 72
Asian American undergraduates and 41 Asian and
European American undergraduates. By assessing
online reactions of hedonic emotion regulation (i.e.
up-regulation of positive emotions and down-
regulation of negative emotions) to a recent negative
event, the study found that European Americans
were more motivated to engage in hedonic emotion
regulation. The cultural differences in hedonic
emotion regulation is mediated by cultural
differences in beliefs about motivational and
cognitive utility of negative emotions, but not by
self-efficacy beliefs.
Furthermore, Stupar, van de Vijver and Fontaine
(2015) reported a study of how emotion regulation
strategies can be predicted by emotion valence and
intensity in 389 Dutch majority members and
members of five immigrant groups, i.e. Turkish and
Moroccan, Antillean and Surinamese, Indonesian,
Western and other non-Western immigrants. The
research results confirmed that emotion regulation
strategies were significantly and similarly correlated
to emotional valence and intensity across the groups.
Negative emotions were more reappraised and
suppressed than positive emotions. Emotional
intensity was positively related to social sharing and
negatively related to reappraisal and suppression.
The Dutch majority group scored higher on emotion
valence than Turkish and Moroccan immigrants.
Also, the Dutch majority group scored lower on
reappraisal than all non-Western groups, and lower
on suppression than Turkish and Moroccan
immigrants.
To summarize, these findings demonstrate that
cultural context affect emotion regulation.
3.4 Culture and Emotion Socialization
The ability to regulate emotion is learned within a
developmental context, such as family situations and
The Cultural Role on Emotional Competence Development in Early Childhood
85
relationship (Matsumoto et al., 2008a). The
socialization process takes place throughout
children's development, even since the infant stage,
through the way parents communicate and calm
down their infants, which further develop the infant's
ability to recognize and to respond the facial
emotional expression (Halberstadt and Lozada,
2011).
There are three possible mechanisms of emotion
socialization mechanisms stated by Halberstadt (in
Denham et al, 1997). They are modeling,
contingency, and coaching. The first mechanism
according to Halberstadt is modeling, states that the
ways parents express emotions implicitly teach
children about emotions that are expected and
acceptable in the family, as well as how to express
and regulate emotions. The second mechanism
according to Halberstadt is contingency, stating that
the response of parental support will help children to
maximize the expression of positive emotions, to
minimize the expression of negative emotions, and
to distinguish among emotions. The third
mechanism according to Halberstadt is coaching,
stating that the ways parents teaches emotions
contribute to the child's emotional expression and
emotional reaction to their peers.
The results of cross-cultural studies show the
differences among cultures in emotion socialization.
Camras et al., (2009) compared emotional
expression within 40 European American, 39
Chinese American and 36 Mainland Chinese
mothers of 3-year-old. European American mothers
reported more positive emotional expression than
Mainland Chinese mothers. There are no significant
differences for negative emotional expressions.
Results of this study confirm the cultural differences
in mothers' emotional expressivity.
Further, Chen et al. (2014) examined the
emotional expression of parents in the family in 252
foreign-born Chinese American immigrant parents
by using self-reported emotional expressivity and
observed emotional expression during a parent-child
interaction task. Results showed that parents’ self-
reported expressivity was only related to their own
reports of children’s regulation, however parents’
observed emotional expression was related to both
parents’ and teachers’ reports of children’s
regulation. These results suggest that self-reported
expressivity and observed emotional expression
reflect different constructs and have differential
relations to parents’ cultural orientations and
children’s regulation.
In addition, Keller and Otto (2009) examine
families in two environments represent different
sociodemographic contexts, i.e. an ethnic group of
rural Cameroon villagers in Northwest Cameroon
and German urban middle-class families in Southern
Germany. The emotional socialization strategy of
the Nso aims for early emotional control with
emphasis on the negative emotion suppression,
while German urban middle-class parents emphasize
the expression of emotions, especially instantiating
and maintaining positive emotions. Emotional
control from an early age is considered as part of a
self-definition and relatedness is regarded as the
developmental organizer; early emotional
expressiveness is acknowledged as part of a self-
definition and autonomy is examined as the
developmental organizer. Related with family
context, Gao and Han (2016) examined the effect of
family expressiveness on children's emotional
development in the context of familial risks.
Participants were 178 school-aged children and their
parents. The results show that the familial risk index
is related positively to emotional dysregulation and
negatively to adaptive emotion regulation, mediated
by positive family expressiveness.
In summary, these findings confirm that cultural
context contribute to cultural differences in
emotional socialization.
4 DISCUSSION
Based on previous studies on culture and emotional
competence, it can be known that there are
differences among cultures in emotional recognition
or emotion understanding, emotion expression, and
emotion regulation. Those findings support the
cultural model of interdependence and the cultural
model of independence (Trommsdorff and
Rothbaum, 2008). They stated that in a cultural
model of interdependence, emotion regulation
centers on socially engaging emotions, on the well-
being of other persons, and on maintaining group
harmony. In contrast, in a cultural model of
independence, emotion regulation centers on
socially disengaging emotions, on self-esteem, and
on individuals' well-being. The development of
emotion regulation in non-Western cultures is
related to empathy, interpersonal accommodation,
and norm orientation. The development of emotion
regulation in Western cultures is related to autonomy
and self-expression.
Besides, those previous findings are in line with
the relational-cultural model and the autonomous
cultural model (Keller and Otto, 2009). A relational
cultural model views the individual as part of a
ICP-HESOS 2018 - International Conference on Psychology in Health, Educational, Social, and Organizational Settings
86
social system, in particular the family, maintains a
harmonious relationship, accepts hierarchy primarily
of age and gender, upholds cooperation and
conformity, and identifies social roles. On the other
hand, an autonomous cultural model views
individual as competitive, unique, self-reliant,
assertive, and have personal opinions. Furthermore,
individualistic or collectivistic cultural values related
to emotion are transmitted to children through
parental emotion socialization. Parents play a
significant role in the development of the emotional
competence of children through the process of
emotion socialization.
However, there are still some inconsistencies in
cross-cultural research findings on emotional
competence. Many works of literature confirm the
existence of inter-cultural similarities in recognition
of basic emotions and situational antecedents of
emotion, and cultural variations in the way people
conceptualize, control and communicate emotions as
well (Wilson et al., 2012). In addition, culture is
dynamic and can change over time, so that the
effects of globalization can cause a shift in the
cultural dimension, so that individuals from a culture
can absorb values and thought patterns from other
cultures, which then lead to changes in the origin
culture or variations in culture (Hashim, 2016).
Also, one thing that seems related to the
inconsistencies of research results is assessment
issues. There are rich variations of instruments used
to measure emotional competence. To measure
emotion recognition, researchers used the test
(Jurgens et al., 2013). Emotional expression was
measured using self-report on questionnaire
(Adrianson and Rhamdani, 2014), daily diary
surveys (Tsai et al., 2016), Emotional
Expressiveness Questionnaire (Tsai et al., 2006),
child emotion interviews with hypothetical vignettes
(Novin, Riefe, and Mo, 2010; Wilson et al., 2012),
games and observations (Lewis, Takai-Kawakami
and Sullivan, 2010), and self-report on the Display
Rule Assessment Inventory (Matsumoto et al.,
2008b). The instruments used to measure emotion
regulation are Emotion Regulation Questionnaire
(Matsumoto, Nakagawa, and Yoo, 2008a; Arens,
Balkir, and Barnow, 2012; Stupar, van de Vijver,
and Fontaine, 2015), and assessment of online
reaction to a recent negative event and retrospective
memory of reactions to a past negative event
(Miyamoto, Ma, and Petermann, 2014). While
emotion socialization is measured by a self-report on
Self-Expressiveness in the Family Questionnaire
(Camras et al., 2009; Chen et al., 2014), interview
(Keller and Otto, 2009), self-report on Family
Expressiveness Questionnaire (Gao and Han, 2016),
self-report on Display Rule Assessment Inventory
(Safdar et al., 2009), parents' observed emotional
expression (Chen et al., 2014), and Child Behavior
Questionnaire (Chen et al., 2014).
However, as a multi-dimension construct,
emotional competence should be assessed with
battery-tests (Denham et al., 2016). Each dimension
should be assessed with multi-methods as well, such
as self-report, teacher report, and observation as
well. Further research is needed to obtain more
consistent results.
One of the limitations of this literature review is
including only a few research articles with young
children participants. Further longitudinal and cross-
sectional research is needed to further clarify the
cultural context of emotional competence throughout
the lifespan from early childhood to adulthood. Next
research can also explore the culture-specific
emotion vocabularies to bring a more
comprehensive perspective on the cultural context of
emotional competence development. For example,
Suswandi et al. (2017) examine sad emotion in
Javanese. In the Javanese language, there are rich
variations in the use of sad emotion vocabulary, and
each vocabulary contains slight meaningful
difference. In terms of lexical semantics, it was
found 15 Javanese vocabularies of sad emotions.
Some of them are karantaranta means to grieve, be
in a sorrowful; ngĕrĕs means 'so sad'; nglangut
means 'so sad', nlangsa means 'heartbroken, crushed
with grief or hardship'; ngĕnĕs means 'so sad';
rudatin means 'sad, sorrowful, worried'. Therefore,
indigenous research related to emotional competence
and emotion socialization need to be explored.
5 CONCLUSIONS AND
RECOMMENDATIONS
In conclusion, the cultural role on emotional
competence in early childhood is as a developmental
context. Through parental emotion socialization,
cultural values are transmitted to children from an
early age, which is the foundation for the
development of emotion expressions, emotion
understanding, and emotion regulation of children.
Most of the previous findings support the
relationship of cultural models of interdependence
and the cultural model of independence with
emotional competence dimensions. However, there
are still inconsistencies in the results of cross-
cultural research on emotional competence, which
The Cultural Role on Emotional Competence Development in Early Childhood
87
may be related to the dynamics of culture and
assessment issues.
Some recommendations for further research are
to use multi-methods of assessment, to extend the
age range of participants from early childhood to
adulthood through a combination of longitudinal and
cross-sectional methods, and to explore the culture-
specific emotion vocabularies to bring a more
comprehensive perspective on the cultural context of
emotional competence development.
REFERENCES
Adrianson, L. and Ramdhani, N., 2014. Why you and not
me? Expressions of envy in Sweden and Indonesia.
International Journal of Research Studies in
Psychology, pp. 1-23, doi: 10.5861/ijrsp.2014.743.
Arens, E.A., Balkir, N, and Barnow, S., 2012. Ethnic
variation in emotion regulation: Do cultural
differences end where psychopathology begins?.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 44(3), pp. 335-
351, doi: 10.1177/0022022112453314.
Ashiabi, G.S., 2000. Promoting the emotional
development of preschoolers. Early Childhood
Education Journal, 28 (2), pp.79-84.
Camras, L, Kolmodin, K, and Chen, Y., 2008. Mothers’
self-reported emotional expression in Mainland
Chinese, Chinese American and European American
families. International Journal of Behavioral
Development, 32(5), pp. 459463.
Carlo, G, Mestre, M.V., McGinley, M.M., Samper, P, Tur,
A, and Sandman, D., 2012. The interplay of emotional
instability, empathy, and coping on prosocial and
aggressive behaviors. Personality and Individual
Differences, 53, pp. 675-680, doi: 10.1016/j.paid.
2012.05.022.
Chaplin, T.M., and Aldao, A., 2013. Gender differences in
emotion expression in children: A meta-analytic
review. Psychological Bulletin, 139(4), pp. 735-765,
doi:10.1037/a0030737.
Chen, S.H., Zhou, Q, Main, A, and Lee, E.H., 2014.
Chinese American immigrant parents’ emotional
expression in the family: Relations with parents’
cultural orientations and children’s emotion-related
regulation. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority
Psychology. Advance online publication.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/cdp0000013.
Cole, P.M., Tamang, B.L., and Shresta, S., 2006. Cultural
variations in the socialization of young children’s
anger and shame. Child Development, 77 (5), pp.
1237-1251.
Cole, P.M., Dennis, T.A., Smith-Simon, K.E., and Cohen,
L.H., 2009. Preschoolers’ emotion regulation strategy
understanding: Relations with emotion socialization
and child self-regulation. Social Development, 18 (2),
pp. 324-352.
Denham, S.A., 1998. Emotional development in young
children. New York: Guilford Press.
Denham, S.A., Bassett, H.H., Brown, C., Way, E., and
Steed, J., 2015. I know how you feel: Preschoolers
emotion knowledge contributes to early school
success. Journal of Early Childhood Research, 13(3),
pp. 252-262, doi: 10.1177/1476718X13497354.
Denham, S.A., Basset, H.H., Sirotkin, Y.S., and Zinsser,
K., 2013. Head Start preschoolers’ emotional
positivity and emotion regulation predict their social-
emotion behavior, classroom adjustment, and early
school success. Dialog, 16(2), pp. 18-33.
Denham, S.A., Ferrier, D.E., Howarth, G.Z., Herndon,
K.J., and Bassett, H.H., 2016. Key considerations in
assessing young children’s emotional competence.
Cambridge Journal of Education, http://dx.doi.org/
10.1080/ 0305764X.2016.1146659.
Denham, S.A., Mason, T, Kochanoff, A, Neal, K, and
Hamada, H., 2003. Emotional development. In: D.
Cavalieri (Ed.), International Encyclopedia of
Marriage and Family Relationships. 2
nd
ed. New
York: McMillan.
Denham, S.A., Mitchell-Coppeland, J, Stranberg, K,
Auerbach, S, and Blair, K., 1997. Parental
contributions to preschoolers’ emotional competence:
Direct and indirect effects. Motivation and Emotion,
21(1), pp. 65-86.
Eisenberg, N, Pidada, S, and Liew, J., 2001. The relations
of regulation and negative emotionality to Indonesian
children’s social functioning. Child Development, 72,
(6), pp. 1747-1763.
Farina, E, and Belacchi, C., 2014. The relationship
between emotional competence and hostile/prosocial
behavior in Albanian preschoolers: An exploratory
study. School Psychology International, 35(5), pp.
475-484, doi: 10.1177/0143034313511011.
Gao, M, and Han, Z.R., 2015. Family expressiveness
mediates the relation between cumulative family risks
and children’s emotion regulation in a Chinese
sample. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 25, pp.
15701580, doi: 10.1007/s10826-015-0335-z
Halberstadt, A.G., and Lozada, F.T., 2011. Emotion
development in infancy through the lens of culture,
Emotion Review, 3(2), pp. 158-168.
Herndon, K.J., Bailey, C.S., Shewark, E., Denham, S.A.,
and Bassett, H.H., 2013. Preschoolers’ emotion
expression and regulation: Relations with school
adjustment. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 174, pp.
642-663.
Herts, K.L., McLaughlin, K.A., and Hatzenbuehler, M.L.,
2012. Emotion dysregulation as a mechanism linking
stress exposure to adolescent aggressive behavior.
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40(7),
pp.1111-1122, doi: 10.1007/s10802-012-9629-4.
Jurgen, R, Drolet, M, Pirow, R, Scheiner, E, and Fischer,
J., 2013. Encoding conditions affect recognition of
vocally expressed emotions across cultures. Frontiers
in Psychology, 4, pp. 1-10. Doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.
00111.
ICP-HESOS 2018 - International Conference on Psychology in Health, Educational, Social, and Organizational Settings
88
Keller, H, and Otto, H., 2009. The cultural socialization of
emotion regulation during infancy. Journal of Cross-
Cultural Psychology, 40(6).
Kurniawan, A.P., and Hasanat, N.U., 2007. Perbedaan
ekspresi emosi pada beberapa tingkat generasi suku
Jawa di Yogyakarta. Jurnal Psikologi, 34(1), pp. 1-17.
LaFreniere, P., 2000. Emotional development, A biosocial
perspective. Belmont, California: Wadsworth/Thompson
Learning.
Lewis, M, Takai-Kawakami, K, and Sullivan, M.W., 2010.
Cultural differences in emotional responses to success
and failure. International Journal of Behavior and
Development, 34(1), pp. 5361, doi: 10.1177/
0165025409348559.
Magdalena, S.M., 2013. Social and emotional competence,
predictors of school adjustment. Procedia Social and
Behavioral Sciences, 76, pp. 29-33.
Markus, H.R., and Kitayama, S., 1994. A collective fear
of the collective: Implications for selves and theories
of selves. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 20, pp. 568579.
Matsumoto, D., 2009. Culture and emotion expression.
In: R.S. Wyer, C. Chiu, and Y. Hong (Eds.).
Understanding culture: Theory, research and
application. New York: Taylor and Francis Group.
Matsumoto, D, Nakagawa, S, and Yoo, S.H., 2008a.
Culture, emotion regulation and adjustment. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(6), pp. 925
937, doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.6.925
Matsumoto, D, Yoo, S.H., Fontaine, J, Anguas-Wong,
A.M., Arriola, M, Ataca, B, Bond, M.H., Boratav,
H.B., Breugelmans, S.M., Cabecinhas, R, Chae, J,
Chin, W.H., Comunian, A.L., Degere, D.N., Djunaidi,
A., Fok, H.K., Friedlmeier, W., Ghosh, A.,
Glamcevski, M., Granskaya, J.V., Groenvynck, H.,
Harb, C., Haron, F., Joshi, R., Kakai, H., Kashima, E.,
Khan, W., Kurman, J., Kwantes, C.T., Mahmud, S.H.,
Mandaric, M., Nizharadze, G., Odusanya, J.O.,
Ostrosky-Solis, F., Palaniappan, A.K., Papastylianou,
D., Safdar, S., Setiono, K., Shigemasu, E., Singelis,
T.M., Iva, P.S., Spiess, E., Sterkowicz, S., Sunar, D.,
Szarota, P., Vishnivetz, B., Vohra, N., Ward, C.,
Wong, S., Wu, R., Zebian, S., Zengeya, A., Altarriba,
J., Bauer, L.M., Mogaji, A., Siddiqui, R.N., Fulop, M.,
Bley, L.D.M.G., Alexandre, J., Garcia, F.B., and
Grossi, E., 2008b. Mapping expressive differences
around the world: The relationship between emotional
display rules and individualism versus collectivism.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39, pp. 55-74,
doi: 10.1177/0022022107311854.
McLaughlin, K.A., Hatzenbuehler, M.L., Mennin, D.S.,
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., 2011. Emotion dysregulation
and adolescent psychopathology: A prospective study.
Behavior Research and Therapy, 49, 544554.
Mesquita, B., and Karasawa, M., 2004. Self-conscious
emotions as dynamic cultural processes.
Psychological
Inquiry, 15, pp. 161166.
Miyamoto, Y., Ma, X., Petermann, A.G., 2014. Cultural
differences in hedonic emotion regulation after a
negative event. Emotion, 14(4), pp. 804815, doi:
10.1037/a0036257.
Novin, S., Rieffe, C., and Mo, T.J.H., 2010. The role of
situational goals and audience on self-reported
emotion experience and expression: Dutch and South
Korean children compared. Infant and Child
Development, 19, pp. 406421, doi: 10.1002/icd.671.
Perlman, S.B., Camras, L.A., and Pelphrey, K.A., 2008.
Physiology and functioning: Parents’ vagal tone,
emotion socialization, and children’s emotion
knowledge. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 100, pp. 308-315.
Safdar, S., Friedlmeier, W., Matsumoto, D., Yoo, S.H,,
Kwantes, C.T., Kakai, H., and Shigemasu, E., 2009.
Variations of emotional display rules within and
across cultures: A comparison between Canada, USA,
and Japan. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science,
41(1), pp. 110, doi: 10.1037/a0014387.
Spangler, G., and Zimmermann, P., 2014. Emotional and
adrenocortical regulation in early adolescence:
Prediction by attachment security and disorganization
in infancy. International Journal of Behavior
Development, 38(2), pp. 142-154, doi:
10.1177/0165025414520808.
Stupar, S., van de Vijver, F.J.R., and Fontaine, J.R.J.,
2015. Emotion valence, intensity and emotion
regulation in immigrants and majority members in the
Netherlands. International Journal of Psychology,
50(4), pp. 312318, doi: 10.1002/ijop.12091.
Suseno, F.M., 2001. Etika Jawa: Sebuah analisa falsafi
tentang kebijaksanaan hidup Jawa. Jakarta: Gramedia
Pustaka Utama.
Suswandi, I., Widhyasmaramurti, Tharik WS, A., 2017.
Sad emotion in javanese language: An analysis of
meaning component and relation. PEOPLE
International Journal of Social Sciences, 3(2), pp.
2318-2336.
Tao, A., Zhou, Q., and Wang, Y., 2010. Parental reactions
to children’s negative emotions: Prospective relations
to Chinese children’s psychological adjustment.
Journal of Family Psychology, 24(2), pp. 135-144.
Trommsdorff, G., and Rothbaum, F., 2008. Development
of emotion regulation in cultural context. In: M.
Vandekerckhove, C. von Scheve, S. Ismer, S. Jung,
and S. Kronast (Eds.). Regulating emotions: Culture,
social necessity, and biological inheritance. Malden:
Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 85-120.
Tsai, W., Sun, M., Wang, S., and Lau, A.S., 2016.
Implications of emotion expressivity for daily and trait
interpersonal and intrapersonal functioning across
ethnic groups. Asian American Journal of Psychology,
7(1), pp. 5263. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037/
aap0000043.
Wang, Q., 2001. Did you have fun?: American and
Chinese mother-child conversations about shared
emotional experiences. Cognitive Development, 16,
pp. 693715.
Wang, Q., and Fivush, R., 2005. Mother-child
conversations of emotionally salient events: Exploring
the functions of emotional reminiscing in European-
The Cultural Role on Emotional Competence Development in Early Childhood
89
American and Chinese families. Social Development,
14(3), pp. 473495.
Weinfeld, N.S., Sroufe, L.A., Egeland, B., and Carlson, E.,
2008. Individual differences in infant-caregiver
attachment: Conceptual and empirical aspects of
security. In: J. Cassidy and PR Shaver (Eds.),
Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and
clinical application. 2
nd
ed. New York: The Guildford
Press.
Wilson, S.L., Raval, V.V., Salvina, J., Raval, P.H., and
Panchal, I.N., 2012. Emotional expression and control
in school-age children in India and the United States.
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 58(1), pp. 5076.
Zimmer-Gembeck, M.J., Webb, H.J., Pepping, C.A.,
Swan, K., Merlo, O., Skinner, E.A., Avdagic, E., and
Dunbar, M., 2015. Review: Is parentchild attachment
a correlate of children’s emotion regulation and
coping?. International Journal of Behavioral
Development, pp. 120, doi: 10.1177/
0165025415618276.
ICP-HESOS 2018 - International Conference on Psychology in Health, Educational, Social, and Organizational Settings
90