Corpus-driven Analysis on the Language of Children’s Literature
Evynurul Laily Zen
1
1
Universitas Negeri Malang
Keywords:
Corpus-driven; linguistic analysis, children’s literature.
Abstract: This current paper examined distinctive patterns of language that characterize children’s literature using a
corpus-driven approach. I built a limited corpus—CoCL (Corpus of Children’s Literature)—of 28 novels
and short stories that were published in the late 19
th
to the early 20
th
available on Project Gutenberg and
written by four prominent writers; Carlo Collodi, Lewis Carrol, Beatrix Potter and Hugh Lofting. With the
utilization of WMatrix and AntConc as the corpus tools, the 319.968 tokens of CoCL were further analyzed
and compared to the BNC Written Imaginative. The findings demonstrated several features distinguishing
the language of this particular genre to adult fictions including significant uses of noun and subjective
pronoun, explicit articulations of smallness, animals, and food, as well as cultivation of positive vibes,
joyful tones, and optimism. The paper attempted to enrich evidence on the effectiveness of corpora in both
linguistic and literary analysis that was, at the same time, seen to mark the advancement of digital world in
language research.
1 INTRODUCTION
A tradition of story writing for children and
probably also by children has dated back 250 years
ago. This explains that children’s literature has truly
taken thousand miles of development following the
changes of people and their cultures. Kennedy
(2017) points out that it was in the seventeenth
century where children’s literature emerged as an
independent genre stimulated by an increasing
awareness of repositioning children as the center of
agency as well as the point of interest. This was
clearly manifested in the emerging moral values
relevant for children through the portrayal of
adventures together with a massive growth of picture
books in the nineteenth century (Kennedy, 2017).
In the twentieth-century, children stories become
progressively diverse yet remain didactic as they are
written in an age-specific language (Coghlan, 2017;
Leland et al., 2013). In this way, scholars agreed that
children’s literature should be distinctive in a sense
that it should talk about children and use ‘child-
oriented’ language. Taking a child-centeredness as a
point of departure, I put forward a corpus-driven
analysis toward children stories with the aim of
figuring out distinctive features of this specific genre
in comparison to the adult’s literature.
Corpus analysis becomes a primary backdrop of
this paper as I refer to (Llaurado, et al., 2012)
argument stating that corpus linguistics enables
researchers to obtain samples of authentic language
uses in different contexts for various analytical
purposes ranging from capturing developmental
shifts in language use to encapsulating genre specific
features. The employment of corpus is also seen to
be able to build a connecting link between linguistic
analysis and literary interpretation (Hardstaff, 2015;
Cogo, and Dewey, 2012; Forceville, 2006). Through
a corpus-based study, Hardstaff herself carefully
examines child agency and character development
embedded within grammatical patterning in Roll of
Thunder.
In a specific context of children literature analysis,
Hardstaff’s study is influential as it not only
approaches a literary analysis from a different angle,
but also draws a bridging line between two sub-
disciplines. It, at the same time, is able to fill the gap
of previous studies that have repeatedly investigated
children’s literature from a very specific literary
issue in a single story, such as style shifting in Peter
Rabbit (Mackey, 1998; Rudman, 1995), boundaries
of properties in Beatrix Potter’s tales (Blomley,
Laily Zen, E.
Corpus-driven Analysis on the Language of Children’s Literature.
DOI: 10.5220/0009912700170022
In Proceedings of the 1st International Conference on Recent Innovations (ICRI 2018), pages 17-22
ISBN: 978-989-758-458-9
Copyright
c
2020 by SCITEPRESS Science and Technology Publications, Lda. All rights reserved
17
2004), animals’ right in Doctor Dolittle (Elick,
2007), and translating animals’ language in Doctor
Dolittle (Hague, 2007; Heine, Narrog, & Biber,
2015).
Following Hardstaff’s line of research, I
specifically work to find prominent linguistic
patterns and literary elements that characterize
children’s literature by making use of corpus data
and corpus tools. More explicitly, my study
replicates Thompson and Sealey's (2007)
comparative analysis of language patterns used in
three corpora: CLLIP (Corpus-based Learning about
Language in the Primary-school), adult fiction, and
newspapers to identify specific features of writing
aimed for child audiences compared to adult
audiences. Their findings demonstrate that the
language of CLLIP and adult fiction was much
similar than those in newspapers. However, in terms
of methodology, Thompson and Sealey (2007) limit
their corpus exploration only on POS (Part of
Speech) tagging analysis. It is therefore necessary to
broaden their investigation on semantic tagging and
concordance analysis to get a closer look at different
picture of linguistic patterns of children’s literature.
In addition, instead of using their corpus data that
they obtain from BNC (British National Corpus)
Imaginative, I build my own corpus that I will
elaborate in the following sub section.
2 METHOD
I employed a corpus-driven approach where the
corpus files were created before being investigated. I
primarily utilized WMatrix and AntConc to locate
keyness/keyword lists, POS tags, Semantic tags, and
concordance analysis by using BNC Written
Imaginative as a reference corpus to reveal regular
patterns of language within the frame of literary
works.
The corpus—I termed it CoCL (Corpus of
Children’s Literature)—was compiled from samples
of novels and short stories published during the late
19
th
to the early 20
th
century. Stories written by
Carlo Collodi and Lewis Carrol were taken to
represent the late 19
th
century, whereas Beatrix
Potter and Hugh Lofting were to represent the early
20
th
. This was the Golden Age of Children’s
Literature in Britain. Avoiding the use of random
sampling, I took those with everlasting international
popularity having been adapted into screen plays as
my corpus data which all are listed in Table 1.
Table1: Corpus of Children’s Literature
Authors Novels/Stories
Year
s Tokens
Carlo
Collodi
The Adventures of
Pinocchio
1881 -
1883
82.869
Pinocchio: the Tale of the
Puppe
t
Lewis
Carroll Alice’s adventures in
1865 -
1889
92.313
wonderland
Through a looking-glass
Beatrix
Potte
r
The Tale of Pete
r
Rabbi
t
1902 -
1930
42.510
The Tale of Squirrel
Nutkin
The Tailor of Gloucester
The Tale of Benjamin
Bunny
The Tale of Two Bad
Mice
The Tale of Mrs. Tiggy-
Winkle
The Tale of Samuel
Whiskers
The Tale of the Flopsy
Bunnies
The Tale of Ginger and
Pickles
The Tale of Mrs.
Tittlemouse
The Tale of Timmy
Tiptoes
The Tale of Mr. Tod
The Tale of the Pie and the
Patty-Pan
The Tale of Pigling Bland
The Tale of Mr. Jeremy
Fishe
r
Appley Dapply's Nursery
Rhymes
TheStoryofaFierce Bad
Rabbit
The Tale of Johnny Town-
Mouse
The Story of Miss Moppet
Cecily Parsley's Nursery
Rhymes
The Tale of Tom Kitten
The Tale of Little Pig
Robinson
The Tale of Jemima
Puddle-
Duck
The Tale of Kitty-in-Boots
Hugh
Loftin
g
The Story of Docto
r
Dolittle
1920
1921
102.272
The Voyages of Doctor
Dolittle
Total 319.968
ICRI 2018 - International Conference Recent Innovation
18
3 FINDINGS AND DISCUSSION
3.1 Linguistic Patterns
To find the most preferred linguistic items used in
children stories, I look at three features: keyness,
POS tags, and Semantic Tags. The keyness analysis
presented in Figure 1 shows that character names
(Alice, Pinocchio, doctor (Doctor Dolittle),
Polynesia) and grammatical bins (nt, the, and, im)
appeared most prominently. This is unsurprising as
these terms should generally appear in all fictional
texts. The word little (988 times), however, can be
claimed as a distinctive feature of CoCL due to its
high frequency compared to the reference corpus
(208 times). This is in line with the result of
semantic tag analysis partly shown in Figure 2 in
which the concept of small occurred more regularly
than big. In this context, little is used to portray
children’s ability in viewing the world and their
surroundings where everything they could see, hear,
taste, and touch must be in an equivalent ‘size’ to
them.
Figure 1: Keyness Analysis
Figure 2: Word Cloud on Semantic Category of Size
Further observation is carried out to find group of
words that collocates with the word little. The result
in Figure 3 indicates that they commonly denotes (1)
human and animals (e.g. man, donkey, boy, girl,
fairy, pig, dog, etc), (2) places (e.g. house, boat), and
(3) objects (e.g. way, door). This particular empirical
evidence has strengthen a claim that children stories
should be child-oriented in the way that they must
contain more concrete objects (either animate or
inanimate) rather than the abstract ones (McDowell,
2006).
POS tagging analysis provides another interesting
point especially when this CoCL is compared to
Thompson and Searley’s (2007) CoAL (Corpus of
Adult’s Literature). The result in Chart 1 illustrates
that the frequency of noun, article, preposition,
pronoun, and conjunction are higher in CoCL than in
CoAL. This significant use of noun and pronoun in
children’s literature has suggested an emphasis of
‘subject’ and ‘object’ in child’s point of view.
Figure 3: R1 Collocates of Little
Chart 1: Comparative POS Tags of CoCL and CoAL
A distinctive pattern of language is further
maintained by the overuse items of semantic
category in Figure 4 below.
Figure 4: Overuse Items of Semantic Category
The category of living creatures: animals in
Figure 4 appears as the key concept in CoCL. It
suggests that children’s stories were constantly
capturing animals. More importantly, the stories
featured animals as talking creatures that personify
human with their life experiences (e.g. said the crow,
said the four Rabbits, the judge was a monkey, etc.)
as seen in Figure 5.
Corpus-driven Analysis on the Language of Children’s Literature
19
Figure 5: Concordance of Living Creatures Category
A semantic field of food is fascinating in
particular association to animal. It is to say that
animals’ life is commonly centered around food
finding. A variety of food (e.g. blackberries,
cherries, beans) in Figure 6 is seen to depict a close
connection of animals to their habitat and
environment. The significant use of food in CoCL,
therefore, sustains the importance of it both in
animal and children’s life serving as the basic need
of all living creatures.
Figure 6: Concordance of Food Category
2.3 Literary Elements
Literary elements commonly include settings,
characters, plots, meaning, point of view, and style
(Temple, et al., 2002). In analyzing the outstanding
concepts appeared in children’s literature literary
elements, I look at characters, style and meaning.
To assess the first element, I analyze the use of
pronoun as a relevant POS to describe characters.
Chart 2 below illustrates the comparative use of
pronoun in CoCL and CoAL to pinpoint the findings
that all types of pronoun were exploited more
frequently in CoCL rather than in CoAL. This
finding articulates a critical role played by the
‘agent’ or ‘doer’ in children stories which, to a large
extent, uncovers children’s distinctive point of view
for it seems easier for them to understand who do
things before what things are done. Furthermore,
Chart 2 indicates the greater use of ‘subjective’
pronoun (e.g. I, you, she, he, we, they) and the lesser
use of ‘objective pronoun (e.g. me, us). It supports
the previous claim on the importance of ‘self’ in
childhood which I assume to be shifting in
adulthood.
A closer look at all characters in the CoCL, I find
that 16 out of 119 were human (e.g. Alice, Doctor
Dolittle, Mr. Jackson, etc), whereas 103 out of 119
were animals (e.g. Petter the rabbit, Polynesia the
parrot, Mr. John Dormouse the mouse, etc). This is
predictable as the semantic tag of living creatures:
animal is overused. What I consider interesting is
that these animal characters were mostly portrayed
as male (See Chart 3). This illustrates the focal point
of gender representation as key issues in children
world. With a more thorough examination on this
phenomenon, I believe further study will be able to
provide more elaborate explanation.
Chart 2: Pronoun in CoCL & CoAL
Chart 3: Representation of Gender in CoCL
Style is not stories the authors wanted to tell, but
how stories are delivered through words (Ibid,
2002). In this context, I put forward an idea that
style is to behave similarly to tone as both elements
are about manner of delivering a story to the targeted
audience. The depiction of tone in the delivery of
stories to child readers is of importance. Our general
assumption is that stories about children and adult
will not be delivered using the same tones. Using
this corpus analysis, I aim to find evidence on the
distinctive tone of children’s literature by looking at
the semantic field of emotion (Figure 7).
Figure 7: Semantic Category of Emotion
ICRI 2018 - International Conference Recent Innovation
20
Words within this field include verbs (e.g. cried,
laughed), adjectives (e.g. funny, glad), and adverbs
(e.g. angrily, anxiously). There seems to be a
tendency to use greater positive emotional
expressions (e.g. gently, joy) and lesser negative
emotional expressions (e.g. angry, frightened) in
CoCL. It strongly implies that children are similar to
adult in the way they experience both joy and
sorrow, but they are different in that joy and all
those enjoyable experiences were valued more.
Also, Figure 7 clearly indicates that words
expressing unpleasantness and sorrow were
underused, whereas pleasantness and joy were
overused. This corpus evidence is in particular
support to the argument stating that children’s books
tend to use language expressing optimism rather
than depression with a major purpose to entertain
children and provide moral values (McDowell,
2006; O’Sullivan and Whyte, 2017; Glynn, 2010;
Guo, 2015).
The meaning of a story can be broadly treated as
certain theme or value to share embedded within that
story. In this way, theme often defines the
segmentation of the readers. Thompson and Sealey
(2007) figured out that intimacy and sex was
frequently used as a theme in adult fiction, whereas
the CoCL demonstrates that nature-related issues
are noteworthy in defining children’s state of mind. I
refer my finding to the semantic categories of
geographical terms (ranked 15
th
), plants (ranked
27
th
), and farming and horticulture (ranked 28
th
). It
is then convincing to claim that nature is the
ultimate necessity that children need to know.
Geographical terms in Figure 8 below, for example,
is seen to function not only as places where daily
activities were conducted, but also things attached to
their daily life.
Figure 8: Concordances of Geographical Terms
In addition, words within the category of
plants (e.g. bushes, trees, flowered) in Figure 9
function more than only as supplementary elements,
but as a center of interests where stories are about.
The semantic field of farming and horticulture in
Figure 10 shows a similar tendency. These all come
to prove that nature is one of the distinctive themes
being valued as the key element of children’s stories.
This supports Pike's (2010) argument on the nature
of spaces in children’s perspective where
fairgrounds, amusement parks, and zoological
gardens are commonly successful in creating an
enjoyment and pleasure for children as they can
interact with the natural world.
Figure 9: Concordances of Plants
Figure 10: Concordances of Farming and Horticulture
However, nature does not seem to be the only
prominent themes in children stories as I figure out
that words reflecting a spirit of optimism are also
overused. The semantic field of psychological
actions, states, and processes (X) in Chart 4
provides us with the evidence. The overuse items of
this semantic category imply that children stories
have invested an equal probability of failure and
success, the importance of trying hard, and the value
of feeling interested/excited/energetic and
tough/strong to strive for life.
Chart 4: Reflection of ‘Optimism’ in CoCL and BNC
written image.
4 CONCLUSIONS
In conclusion, a corpus analysis I carried out has
facilitated me in discovering distinctive language
patterns as well as literary elements embodied within
the CoCL (Corpus of Children’s Literature).
Compared to the literary texts written for adults, the
language used in children’s literature tends to be
centered around the idea of smallness, animals, and
food through the significant uses of noun and
subjective pronoun. In terms of literary concepts,
children’s literature tends to cultivate the idea of
personifying animals as talking characters, elevating
positive vibes and joyful tones, making nature and
optimism the most preferable themes of the stories.
Corpus-driven Analysis on the Language of Children’s Literature
21
A corpus of this kind will impart a practical
implication of cross-sectional studies mainly for
pedagogical purposes where it provides teachers
with big data of children stories as well as typical
patterns of children’s language.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This paper was written upon a completion of a term-
paper of Corpus Linguistics Class 2017 at the
Department of English Language and Literature,
National University of Singapore with a special
reference and appreciation for Prof. Vincent Ooi for
his constructive feedback toward the earlier draft of
the paper.
REFERENCES
Blomley, N., 2004. The boundaries of property: lessons
from Beatrix Potter. The Canadian Geographer, 48(2),
91–100.
Coghlan, V., 2017. Picturing possibilities in children’s
book collections. In K. O’Sullivan & P. Whyte (Eds.),
Children’s Literature Collections: Approaches to
Research (pp. 221–240). New York.
Cogo, A. and Dewey, M., 2012. Analysing English as a
lingua franca: A corpus-driven investigation.
Bloomsbury Publishing.
Elick, C. L., 2007. Anxieties of an animal rights activist:
The pressures of modernity in Hugh Lofting’s Doctor
Dolittle series. Children’s Literature Association
Quarterly, 32(4), 323–339.
Forceville, C. 2006. Book Review: Corpus Approaches to
Critical Metaphor Analysis. Language and Literature,
15(4), 402-405. doi:10.1177/0963947006068661
Glynn, D. 2010. Corpus-driven Cognitive Semantics.
Introduction to the field. Quantitative Methods in
Cognitive Semantics: Corpus-Driven Approaches, 1-
42. doi:10.1515/9783110226423.1
Guo Gui-Hang & LI Dan. 2015. A Corpus-Driven
Analysis of Image Construction of BRIC Bank from
Mainstream Media’s Perspective—A Case Study of
China Daily. Journal of Literature and Art Studies,
5(7). doi:10.17265/2159-5836/2015.07.009
Hague, D. R., 2007. Fuzzy memories: Why narrators
forget they translate for animals. Translation and
Literature, 16(2), 178–192.
Hardstaff, S., 2015. ‘“Papa Said That One Day I Would
Understand”’: Examining child agency and character
development in Roll of Thunder , Hear My Cry using
critical corpus linguistics. Children’s Literature in
Education, 46, 226–241.
http://doi.org/10.1007/s10583-014-9231-1
Heine, B., Narrog, H. & Biber, D. 2015. Corpus-Based
and Corpus-Driven Analyses of Language Variation
and Use. The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Analysis.
doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199677078.013.0008
Kennedy, M., 2017. Instruction with delight: Evidence of
children as readers in eighteenth-century Ireland from
the collections of Dublin City Library and Archive. In
K. O’Sullivan & P. Whyte (Eds.), Children’s
Literature Collections: Approaches to Research (pp.
15–32). New York: Palgrave Macmilan.
Leland, C., Lewison, M., & Harste, J., 2013. Teaching
children’s literature: It’s critical! New York and
London: Routledge.
Llaurado, A., Marti, A., & Tolchinsky, L., 2012. Corpus
CesCa: Compiling a corpus of written Catalan
produced by school children. International Journal of
Corpus Linguistics, 17(3), 428–441.
http://doi.org/10.1075/ijcl.17.3.06lla
Mackey, M., 1998. The case of Peter Rabbit: Changing
conditions of literature for children. New York and
London: Garland Publishing, Inc.
McDowell, M., 2006. Fiction for children and adults:
Some essential differences. In P. Hunt (Ed.),
Children’s literature: Critical concepts in literary and
cultural studies (pp. 53–67). London and New York:
Routledge.
O’Sullivan, K., and Whyte, P. (Eds.)., 2017. Children’s
literature collections: Approaches to research. New
York: Springer.
Pike, D. L., 2010. Buried pleasure: Doctor Dolittle, Walter
Benjamin, and the nineteenth-century child.
Modernism/Modernity, 17(4), 857–875.
Rudman, M.K., 1995. Children's literature: An issues
approach. Addison-Wesley Longman, 1 Jacob Way,
Reading, MA 01867.
Temple, C., Martinez, M., Yokota, J., and Naylor, A.,
2002. Children’s books in children’s hands: An
introduction to their literature. Boston: Allyn &
Bacon: A Pearson Education Company.
Thompson, P., and Sealey, A., 2007. Through children’s
eyes? Corpus evidence of the features of children’s
literature. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics,
12(1), 1–23.
ICRI 2018 - International Conference Recent Innovation
22