Altruistic Behavior: Comparation of Men and Women in Indonesia
Andy Gunardi
1,2
1
Character Building Development Center, Information Systems Department, Bina Nusantara University, Jakarta 11480,
Indonesia.
2
PhD Student in Psychology, Atma Jaya Catholic University, Jakarta 12930, Indonesia
Keyword: Altruistic behavior, gender, Western
Abstract: Indonesia is a country having a lot of cultures, religions and well known as a tolerant and help each other.
They have an adage, that’s “berat sama dipikul dan ringan sama dijinjing” (Indonesian are living together
and helping each other). If the idea is correct, we might say Indonesian are altruistic people. Because of
such phenomena, researcher is interested in proving the idea. Researcher differentiates between women and
men to compare the two. According to the theory before in the West Countries, women are more altruistic
than such men, so that the question may arise is, “is that the same in Indonesia?”. To measure such
behavior, researcher finds a tool called Adapted Self Report Altruism Scale (ASRAS) in Indonesia by
Researcher. Researcher spreads ASRAS forms online to the university students in Bina Nusantara who are
seventeen to twenty-four years old and adults who are twenty-four and above years old. Having such
analyzed research, the results can explicitly between men and women. The main purpose of this research is
to have the level of altruistic scale for Indonesian and which one is more altruistic than the other.
1 INTRODUCTION
Indonesia is a country known as high social level of
population. Indonesian live together and build
connection each other as a community. It can be
seen from a low level of governmental structure,
they are rukun tetangga (bringing peace each other
in small area of residents) and rukun warga
(bringing peace each other in a few of rukun
tetangga). These kinds of structure cannot be found
in other countries. The task of rukun tetangga is to
help neighborhood if there are dispute or conflict
each other and also to help family if they have
problem. The task of rukun warga is to facilitate a
harmony living around rukun tetangga.
Through those phenomena, researcher is
interested in finding the level of altruistic behavior
of Indonesian, especially between men and women.
If the concept is so, Indonesian are high in altruistic
level and next question is that “which one is more
altruistic than the others; men or women?”. This
research is used as Adapted Self Report Altruism
Scale. The scale is adapted from SRAS by Rushton,
Chrisjohn and Fekken (1981).
Altruism is a topic that discussed in the context
of social behavior. People are questioning about
those who want to help and act sacrificing to others.
One theory said that such genetic is delivered from
one generation to the other generations (Hamilton,
1964). Richard Dawkins (1989) said that altruism
was derived from human being selfish behavior. He
quoted Hamilton theory that altruism is individuals
being selfish, make decision together to facilitate
and maintain selfish behavior among them. The
purpose was their personal interests (Dawkins,
1989).
The Dawkin’s theory is rejected by Gintis,
Bowles, Boyd and Fehr (2003). They, therefore, find
a theory about strong reciprocity. There is a
tendency doing good among people when they
experience the good act from the other people
(Gintis, Bowles, Boyd and Fehr, 2003). Long before
Gintis, Bowles, Boyd and Fehr (2003) found the
theory, Batson and Shaw mentioned there were
some people who were born naturally as altruistic
(Batson and Shaw, 1991). They have good
empathizing and willing to help others (Batson,
2011). Rosopa did a research about helping people
who actually did helping themselves (Rosopa,
Schroeder and Hatfield, 2013). This is different from
what Gintis said. Helping behavior was not caused
by reciprocity but by helping the others.
Gunardi, A.
Altruistic Behavior: Comparation of Men and Women in Indonesia.
DOI: 10.5220/0009999100002917
In Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on Social Sciences, Laws, Arts and Humanities (BINUS-JIC 2018), pages 11-15
ISBN: 978-989-758-515-9
Copyright
c
2022 by SCITEPRESS Science and Technology Publications, Lda. All rights reserved
11
Theory about altruism is developed in daily
context, such as what Jaffe did. He uses altruism in
punishment context. People who did wrongly are
punished by doing good for community. The
altruistic punishment can change norms in the
society (Jaffe, 2004). The other side, Saslow uses
altruism in the context of spirituality. She finds that
more spiritual the person, more altruism the person
is (Saslow, 2011). Through these theories, we can
say that prosocial behavior is not only moved by the
selfishness but also by the good heart of people and
by practice.
Women have higher level of altruistic than men
(Mills, Pedersen and Grusec, 1989; Fabes and
Eisenberg, 1998). Through their research, women
are easier to help because of the role of unifying
than men. Men are rather acting than unifying.
Women have tendency to be warm, social orientated
and sensitive to relation and men have tendency to
gain self-control and success (Eagly, 1997; Eagly
and Wood, 2012; Eagly and Wood, 2016; Bierhoff
H W 2002).
1.1 Definition of Altruism
There are various theories about altruism and in this
case, researcher will focus on some relevant to the
research. Batson defines altruism as an act of
helping others, even if doing so involves some cost
to them (Batson and Shaw, 1991). Simmons defines
altruism as a feeling or tendency to do good to
others, even at the expense of personal gain
(Simmons, 1991). He mentioned 4 factors, those are
willingness to do things to enhance the welfare of
others, not one’s own, voluntary, intentional
involving helping others and expecting no reward.
In the handout book of social psychology defines
altruism as a tendency to help even so doing that
will take personal cost (Aronson, Wilson, Akert and
Sommers, 2015).
Fuentes defined altruism as prosocial behavior
that satisfied the observable requirements of
altruistic behavior plus the following criteria: The
behavior benefits another person; It’s voluntary; the
beneficiary of the act is well defined; the performer
of the act does not expect to receive any external
benefit from the act (Fuentes, Lopez, Etxebarria,
Ledesma and Apocada, 2014).
Through those theories researcher defines
altruism as individual tendency doing good, freely,
to the others, even if it involves a cost to the helper
and the helper does not expect in return of what he
or she has done (Batson and Shaw, 1991; Simmons,
1991; Aronson, Wilson, Akert and Sommers, 2015)
.
1.2 ASRAS 2018
Adapted Self Report Altruism Scale is designed to
measure altruistic scale for Indonesian. This test is
adapted from Rushton, Chrisjohn and Fekken (1981)
who firstly found consistency of the altruism in all
field of life. The previous theory said there were no
consistency (Hartshorne, May and Maller, 1929). He
proved through correlation test, altruism in variety
fields was +.5 and +.6 (Rushton, Chrisjohn, and
Fekken, 1981). The Self Report Scale was tested to
Western Ontario University students and the results
were significant. He compared the test with the other
instruments such as Educational Testing Service
(ETS) and Nurturance Scale of the Personality
Research Form (PRF) (Jackson, 1974). The results
were the same. SRAS could be compared to the
other measurements.
In 1992 SRAS was developed in India. The
results was SRAS could be adapted to India culture
(Khanna, Singh and Rushton, 1993). In 2009 SRAS
adapted by Peter Witt and Chris Boleman in
America. The tool has been used by CYFAR Life
Skills Project, Youth Development Initiative and
Texas A&M University (Boleman and Witt, 2009).
2 METHOD
This research uses quantitative method and the
participants are from Bina Nusantara university
students and adults. They are strictly selected
because of according to Newman and Newman the
later adolescence (about 18-24) can be differentiate
gender, autonomy from their parents, and
internalized morality (Newman and Newman, 2008).
The further steps of life which are adulthood and old
age can be more mature to do altruistic act.
Participants from Bina Nusantara University are
eighty-one students and they are seventeen to
twenty-four old year old. The adults are 128 people
and they are twenty-five and above year old.
The SRAS by Rushton, Chrisjohn, and Fekken,
(1981) is adapted to Indonesian context by
researcher. Researcher used sample about 309
participants and find the tool was established. The
original items are twenty and after adaptation
become thirteen items. The process of adaptation
using experts to translate the language from English
to Indonesian and Indonesian to English and also
expert judgement. After that researcher used SPSS
24 and Amos 24 to find reliability, EFA and CFA.
The results are Cronbach Alpha .837; corrected
items .3 and above; communalities extraction .4 and
BINUS-JIC 2018 - BINUS Joint International Conference
12
above; RMSEA .56; AGFI .897; CFI .958; GFI .934.
The conclusion is this test can be used for
Indonesian.
The participants from Bina Nusantara university
students were asked to fill ASRAS questions using
their own cellular phone in the class. The adults
were asked to answer the questions using media
social (WhatsApp group). One person asked to send
to the other person using their own cellphone. The
total participants are 309 (Try out ASRAS: 50 men
and 50 women; Field ASRAS: 104 men and 105
women). There are no missing data in this research.
Data were collected using google form and
analyzed them using SPSS 24. This research is
applied research. The objective is comparative (to
compare score or value between two variables). The
purpose of this comparation is to capture the real
situation of altruistic behavior between men and
women.
The independent variables are gender: men and
women. While dependent variable is altruistic
behavior. According to ASRAS there are three
factors: they are helping with cost; care; and
empathy. To measure the dependent variable,
Researcher use ASRAS 2018. The method to
compare men and women, researcher use t-test from
SPSS 24
3 RESULTS
3.1 Interpretation
According to descriptive statistic using Kolmogorov
Smirnov the data are normal. They can be seen
through table 1.
Table 1: Test of normality.
Kolmogorov-Smirnov
a
Gender Statistic df Sig.
men .067 104 .200
*
women .071 105 .200
*
There are 209 participants and the data that can
be used are Kolmogorov-Smirnov which has
significant both for men and women α = .2 >.05.
After making sure that the data are normal,
researcher uses independent t test. The results can be
seen the following table.
Table 2: Test for equality.
Levene's
Test for
Equality of
Variances
t-test for Equality of
Means
F Sig t df Sig.
Equal
variances
assume
d
1.58 .21 -3.76 207 .000
Equal
variances not
assume
d
-3.76 204,80 .000
Base on Levene’s Test α = .210 > .01. It
indicates that the data is equal variances assumed. It
means t-test α = .00 < .01. The assumption that can
be used is there different level of altruistic behavior
between men and women.
Table 3: Group statistics.
Gender N Mean
Std.
Deviation
Std.
Error
Mean
Total
Men
104 41.07 9.216 .903
Women
105 45.66 8.388 .818
The table 3 shows us that women are more
altruistic than men. It can be seen through mean
value. Men are 41.07 and women 45.66.
3.2 Discussion
Indonesia is assumed as high level of altruistic
behavior. It can be observed through the low
governmental structures. They are rukun tetangga
and rukun warga. According to the results such
measurement using ASRAS 2018, we find data the
low altruistic level (score below 50) is 14.3%,
medium level (score between 50 to 70) is 40.7% and
high level altruistic behavior (70 and above) is 45%.
Table 4: Score of altruistic behavior.
N/score Under 50 50 to 70 70 to 100
30 14.3%
85 40.7 %
94 45%
According to the score, the image of high level
prosocial behavior, especially altruism is in stake.
Altruistic Behavior: Comparation of Men and Women in Indonesia
13
There is a threat that the tradition to help each other
could be reduced from time to time. If Indonesian
are altruist people the high score (70% and above)
must be more than 50%. This score is a warning for
people in Indonesia. If they want to keep as a nation
which is social people who want to help each other,
they need to keep the tradition and also develop the
pro social behavior.
The results of t-test indicate that women are
more altruistic than men. The theory in the west
about this, is the same (Mills, Pedersen and Grusec,
1989; Fabes and Eisenberg, 1998), the nature of
women which is unifying and more social person
give contribution to be more helpful. Women are
more communicable than men. The communication
reflects the level of emotional closeness (Palchykov,
Kaski, Kertész, Barabási and Dunbar, 2012; Kamas,
Preston and Baum, 2008).
In the context of participants, even women are
higher score than men the gap is not too high (men’s
mean is 41 and womens mean is 45). Perhaps the
role of women as defining by Mills and Grusec
(1989) bring the differences. Asih and Pratiwi did
the research about the pro social behavior and they
found there were no differences between two (Asih
and Pratiwi, 2010). The conclusion that can be taken
is even women are more altruist than men, the gap is
not too high.
3.3 Limitation
There are a lot of factors that can change Indonesian
from altruism to selfishness. The research of
altruism in Indonesia mentions that high level of
altruism is 45%. The factors that can decrease and
increase the altruistic behavior need to define. For
doing so, researcher is aware of the limitation of this
research. The future research is needed, especially
how to intervene people in Indonesia to raise the
altruism and to wake up the tradition of helping and
tolerancing each other.
REFERENCES
Aronson, E., Wilson, T.D., Akert, M.A. and Sommers,
S.R. (2015) Social Psychology. New Jersey: Pearson,
p. 345-6.
Asih, G.Y. and Pratiwi, M.M. (2010) ‘Perilaku Prososial
Ditinjau dari Empati dan kematangan Emosi’, Jurnal
Psikologi Universitas Muria Kudus, vol. 1, pp. 33-42.
Batson, C.D. (2011) Altruism in Humans. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Batson, C.D. and Shaw, L.L. (1991) Psychological
Inquiry, vol. 2, pp. 107–122.
Bierhoff, H.W. (2002) Prosocial Behaviour. Philadelphia:
Psychology Press.
Boleman, C. and Witt, P. (2009) Adapted Self-Report
Altruism Scale. Texas: A&M University.
Dawkins, R. (1989) The Selfish Gene . Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Eagly, A.H. (1997) ‘Sex differences in social behavior:
Comparing social role theory and evolutionary
psychology’, American Psychologist, vol. 52, pp.
1380–1383.
Eagly, A.H. and Wood, W. (2012) ‘Social role theory’,
Handbook of theories in social psychology. NewYork:
SAGE Publication Ltd, p. 458–476.
Eagly, A.H. and Wood, W. (2016) ‘Social Role Theory of
Sex Differences’, The Wiley Blackwell Encyclopedia
of Gender and Sexuality Studies. New Jersey: Wiley-
Blackwell, p. 1–3.
Fabes, R. and Eisenberg. (1998) Meta Analyses of age
and sex differences in children's and adloescents' pro
social behavior’, Age and Sex, pp. 1-29.
Fuentes, M.J., Lopez, F., Etxebarria, I., Ledesma, A.R.
and Apocada, M.J. (2014) ‘Empatía, Role-taking y
conceptode ser humano, como factoresasociados a la
conductaprosocial/altruista’, Infancia y Aprendizaje,
pp. 2–17.
Gintis, H., Bowles, S., Boyd, R and Fehr, E. (2003)
‘Explaining altruistic behavior in humans’, Evolution
and Human Behavior, vol. 24, pp. 153–172.
Hamilton, W.D. (1964) ‘The Genetical Evolution of Social
Behaviour’, Journal of Theoritical Biology, vol. 1, pp.
371–52.
Hartshorne, H., May, M.A. and Maller, J.B. (1929)
‘Studies in the Nature of Character’, Studies in Self-
Control, vol. 2. New York: Macmillan.
Jackson (1974) Personality Research Form Manual. Port
Huron: Research Psychologists Press.
Jaffe, K. (2004) ‘Altruism, Altruistic Punishment and
Social Investment’, ActaBiotheoretica, pp. 155–172.
Kamas, L., Preston, A. and Baum, S. (2008) ‘Altruism in
individual and joint-giving decisions: What’s gender
got to do with it?’, Feminist Economics, pp. 1423–50.
Khanna, R., Singh, P. and Rushton, J.P. (1993)
‘Development of the Hindi version of a Self-Report
Altruism Scale’, Personality and Individual
Differences, vol. 14, pp. 267–270.
Mills, R., Pedersen, J. and Grusec, J.E. (1989) ‘Sex
differences in Reasoning and emotion altruism’, Sex
Roles, vol. 20, pp. 603-621.
Newman, B.M. and Newman, P.R. (2008) Development
Through Life. New York: Wadsworth Cengage
Learning.
Palchykov, V., Kaski, K., Kertész, J., Barabási, A.L. and
Dunbar, R.I. (2012) ‘Sex differences in intimate
relationships’, Scientific Reports, vol. 2, p. 370.
Rosopa, P., Schroeder, A.N. and Hatfield, A.L. (2013)
‘Helping yourself by helping others: examining
personality perceptions’, Journal of Managerial
Psychology, vol. 28, pp. 147–163.
Rushton, J.P., Chrisjohn, R.D. and Fekken, G.C. (1981)
‘The Altruistic Personality and The Self-Report
BINUS-JIC 2018 - BINUS Joint International Conference
14
Altruism Scale’, Personality and Individual
Differences, vol. 2, pp. 293–302.
Saslow, L.R. (2011) The Social Significance of
Spirituality: New Perspectives on the Compassion-
Altruism Relationship. Los Angeles: University of
California.
Simmons, R. (1991) ‘Presidential Address on Altruism
and Sociology’, The Sociological Quarterly, vol. 32,
pp. 1-22.
Altruistic Behavior: Comparation of Men and Women in Indonesia
15