Difference of Self-disclosure on Social Media Users with Anonymous
and Non-anonymous Identity
Meidiana and Intan Dewi Kumala
Department of Psychology, Faculty of Medicine, Universitas Syiah Kuala
Keywords: Self-disclosure, Social Network, Anonymous, Non-anonymous, Online.
Abstract: Social networks facilitate individuals to express personal opinions and feelings through status,
photos/videos, or comments called self-disclosure. Users also have the freedom of choice to use both
anonymous and non-anonymous identities on their accounts so that any information disclosed has minimal
consequences and risks. The purpose of this research is to determine the differences of self-disclosure on
social network users with anonymous and non-anonymous identities using quantitative approach with
comparative research method. A total of 150 social network users with anonymous identities and 150 users
with non-anonymous identity (N=300) aged 18-34 years were sampled and selected using non-probability
sampling method through purposive sampling technique. The research data were collected using the
Revised Self-Disclosure Scale (RSDS) developed by Wheeless in 1978. The result of the analysis using
independent sample t-test showed p=0,034 (p<0,05), so it can be concluded that there is the differences of
self-disclosure on social media users with anonymous and non-anonymous identities, in which anonymous
identity users more disclose than non-anonymous identity users and female subjects with anonymous
identity have higher self-disclosure than males. In addition, younger social network users reveal more
personal information than older users.
1 INTRODUCTION
Survey by Indonesia Internet Service Association
(APJII, 2016) showed that there were 132.7 million
of active internet users, 97.4% of whom (129.2
million users) were the most-frequently-internet
accessing users. Social media is website-based
service on internet in which users can make public
content or semi-public content and build a
relationship with other users on the same website
(Boyd & Ellison, 2008). At the present, Facebook is
the most demanded social media, followed by
Instagram, Youtube, Google, Twitter, and LinkedIn
(APJII, 2016).
Besides the most demanded social media,
Facebook and Instagram are the media which could
stimulate self-disclosure (Schouten, 2007) because
both may emerge interaction with people known in
real life or online platform. An individual could
connect with other users, create a community, send
personal or open messages through status,
comments, likes, or uploaded photo/video which are
called self-disclosure.
Self-disclosure is that all information about
oneself is communicated with other people
(Wheeless & Grotz, 1976), including personal
information, ideas, feelings, and experiences
(Derlega, Metts, Petronio, & Margulis, dalam Knoll
& Bronstein, 2013). Wheeless and Grotz (1976)
state five dimensions of self-disclosure i.e. intent to
disclose, amount of disclosure, positive-negative
nature of disclosure, general depth-control of
disclosure), and honesty-accuracy of disclosure.
People could release their feelings publicly through
internet, so that they are more encouraged to do self-
disclosure (Christopherson, 2006).
Internet may make an individual use identified
identity (non-anonymous) or unidentified identity
(anonymous) (Marx, 1999). Non-anoymous is a
condition in which an individual put their real name
in a message that they send or one’s identity can be
known through the name (McKenna & Bargh,
2000), meanwhile anonymous is a condition in
which an individual cannot be identified (Marx,
1999). Morio and Buchholz (2009) mention three
levels of anonymity, namely (a) visual anonymity in
which no physical appearance attached to an
Meidiana, . and Kumala, I.
Difference of Self-disclosure on Social Media Users with Anonymous and Non-anonymous Identity.
DOI: 10.5220/0009820603630367
In Proceedings of the 1st International Conference on Psychology (ICPsy 2019), pages 363-367
ISBN: 978-989-758-448-0
Copyright
c
2020 by SCITEPRESS Science and Technology Publications, Lda. All rights reserved
363
individual’s message; (b) disassociation of identity
is an unmatched identity between online identity and
real identity; (c) lack of identifiability which is no
information referring to message sender. However,
anonymous or non-anonymous action does not
depend on technology, it depends on users’ will and
preference (Kang & Yang, 2004).
A survey conducted in 68 countries, including
Indonesia, found that 59% of media social users
became more open to express themselves and say
anything they want as anonymous users (Childnet
International, 2013). Suler (2004) states that in
anonymity condition, an individual tends to reveal
more personal information than one does in face-to-
face condition. An individual tends to do self-
revealing when using anonymous identity in order to
avoid social risk such as mockery or rejection from
other people (Kang, Dabbish, & Sutton, 2016).
Although almost 95% of online users refuse to
reveal their personal information on website
(Hoffman, Novak, & Peralta, 1999), many
individuals prefer to keep using real name and not
afraid to reveal one’s personal information on social
media, the cause of which is because of personality
factor in which an introvert individual hardly does
self-revealing which is in contrary with an extrovert
one who willingly reveals about oneself by using
real identity (DeVito, 2013). In addition, the need of
existence and obtaining an acknowledgement from
other people make it possible to be braver to reveal
personal identity on social media like sharing certain
contents through image, video, or article uploaded
on their personal accounts.
Both anonymous and non-anonymous users have
common chances and wishes to do self-disclosure,
while social media only acts as a facilitator of an
individual’s social needs to do self-disclosure, so
that hypothesis of this study was there is difference
of self-disclosure of anonymous and non-anonymous
social media users.
2 RESEARCH METHOD
This study used quantitative approach with
comparative method. The subjects were selected
using non-probability sampling through purposive
sampling. The number of samples was 300 subjects
which were selected based on several characteristics
i.e. age ranged 18-34-year-old, accessing social
media Instagram or Facebook once a day with
anonymous or non-anonymous identity, living in
Banda Aceh.
This study used Revised Self-Disclosure Scale
(RSDS) developed by Wheeless in 1978 based on
five dimensions of self-disclosure namely intent to
disclose, amount of disclosure, positive-negative
nature of disclosure, general depth-control of
disclosure), honesty-accuracy of disclosure, the
instrument consists of 31 items favourable and
unfavourable.
This study starts with preparing the instrument,
translating and modifying it into Bahasa Indonesia,
trying it out to gain Cronbach’s Alpha score at 0.843
with item discrimination index at approximately -
0.373-0.606, so that 6 items were aborted. Data were
collected using online platform by distributing
RSDS scale which consisted 25 items to 300
samples, therefore Cronbach’s Alpha score was
0.856. The collected data were analysed by using
independent sample t-test in order to see the
difference of self-disclosure on anonymous or non-
anonymous social media users.
3 RESULT
Table 1: Analysis of Independent Sample T-Test
Self-disclosure N Mean Si
g
Non-Anonim 150 95.27 0.034
Anonim 150 99.65
Statistical analysis of independent sample t-test
showed significance score at 0.034 (p<0.05) so that
it proved that the hypothesis was accepted meaning
that there was significant difference of self-
disclosure on anonymous and non-anonymous social
media users. In addition, social media users with
anonymous identity had higher mean score (99.65)
than those with non-anonymous identity did (95.27).
It showed that users with anonymous identity had
higher self-disclosure than those with non-
anonymous identity.
A survey on social media users was conducted in
68 countries showed that 59% of social media users
felt more open to express themselves and say
anything they want when they used anonymous
identity (Childnet International, 2013). The survey is
in accordance with Joinson, Woodley, and Reips
(2007) that found that individuals tended to reveal
more thoughts and feelings when their interlocutors
did not know their identity. Anonymous presence
makes individuals not responsible for online
communication that they does, so that they could
reveal more intimate information than they do face-
to-face (Suler, 2004).
ICPsy 2019 - International Conference on Psychology
364
Table 2: Self-disclosure Categorization of the Subjects
Category
Non-anon
y
mous Anon
y
mous
n % n %
High
61 40,7 79 52,7
Moderate
88 58,7 70 46,7
Low
1 0,7 1 0,7
The study resulted that of 300 subjects, 79
(52.7%) were from anonymous group with high self-
disclosure. Barak and Gluck-Ofri (2007) explain that
high self-disclosure means revealing personal
information which contains about oneself and the
closer companions, from physical appearance (body
weight, acne, hair, body shape, scar, cheek and so
on) to psychological condition (addiction, bulimia,
anorexia, alcoholic, drugs and so on). Individuals
also reveal their thoughts that are related to their
personal characteristics, health, or secret idea and
dream on social media. Moreover, there is a deep
expressed feeling including insult, misery, anxiety,
depression, fear, pain and so on.
Non-anonymous subjects were dominantly at
moderate category of self-disclosure for 88 subjects
(58.7%). It means that they reveal general
information about oneself like name, birthday,
location, contact, occupation, education, and family
(Bak, Lin, & Oh, 2014). Individuals reveal personal
thought related to personal experience, past event or
future plan. Furthermore, individuals also express
some light feeling like confusing, inconvenience,
common worry, or weakness (Barak & Gluck-Ofri,
2007).
Table 3: Categorization of Anonymous Subject based on
Sex
Catego
r
y
Male % Female %
Hi
g
h 19 12.7 60 40
Moderate 13 8.7 57 38
Low 1 0.7 0 0
Table 4: Categorization of Non-Anonymous Subject
Based on Sex
Categor
Male % Female %
High 17 11.3 44 29.3
Moderate 28 18.7 60 40
Low 0 0 1 0.7
60 subjects (40%) in anonymous group who
were at high category of self-disclosure were
females and 19 (12.7%) were males. Meanwhile, in
non-anonymous category, 60 female subjects (40%)
were at moderate category of self-disclosure, only
28 male subjects (18.7%) were at moderate.
Different level of high self-disclosure in anonymous
group and non-anonymous group can be explained
through several factors like sex, culture, age,
personality, and interlocutor. DeVito (2013) explains
that it occurs because woman tends to be more open
than man that woman commonly reveal their
romantic relationship, friendship, the biggest fear,
and unpleasant things from partners.
Table 5: Categorization of Anonymous Subject age 18-26-
Year-Old.
Cate
g
o
r
y
Male % Female %
High
77 51,3 55 36,7
Moderate
68 45,3 85 56,7
Low
1 0,7 1 0,7
77 subjects (51.3%) using social media with
anonymous identity at 18 to 26-year-old were at high
category of self-disclosure and 68 subjects (45.3%) were
at moderate category of self-disclosure. Not really
different, 55 subjects (36.7%) from non-anonymous group
were at high category of s and 85 subjects (56.7%) were at
moderate. It is in accordance with study by Goodstein
(2007) finding that the younger adult revealed more
personal matters on social media because they were less
cautious and became more convenient to do online
communication.
Table 6: Categorization of Anonymous Subjects age 27 to
34-Year-Old.
Cate
g
o
r
y
Male % Female %
High
3 2 5 3,3
Moderate
1 0.7 4 2,7
Low
0 0 0 0
27 to 34-year-old subjects in anonymous group
who were at high category of self-disclosure were 3
subjects (2%) and 1 subject (0.7%) was at moderate.
Similarly, 5 subjects (3.3%) at 27 to 34-year-old in
non-anonymous group were at high category of self-
disclosure 4 subjects (2.7%) were at moderate.
Kisilevich, Ang, and Last (2012) in a research they
did to Facebook users found that the higher the age
was, the less information was revealed on Social
media. The cause was that the elder rarely used
technology and felt uncomfortable to interact on
online platform (Bucur, Renold, & Henke, 1999).
Besides the factors explained above, social-
cultural and neighborhood factor, Aceh Province,
caused self-disclosure. Sharia implementation
indirectly makes Aceh people have limitation in
behaving, especially for some things considered as
culture and norm-violating. Therefore, to avoid risk
Difference of Self-disclosure on Social Media Users with Anonymous and Non-anonymous Identity
365
and consequence, subjects preferred to be an
anonymous.
DeVito (2013) states that personality is a factor
that could influence self-disclosure. An introvert
individual tends to be difficult to do self-disclosure,
in contrary with the extrovert one who does not
mind disclosing oneself despite using real identity.
In addition, an individual’s self-disclosure behaviour
is also affected by interlocutors, so that one tends to
do self-disclosure to those one likes, trusts or loves.
Table 7: subjects’ intent to use social media.
Intents Anonymous
Non-
anonymous
Entertainment 108 102
Communication 62 76
Accessing Information 97 94
online shop 27 17
Others 5 7
108 subjects (72%) from anonymous group and
102 subjects (68%) from non-anonymous group in
this study used social media for entertainment.
Accordingly, a respondent in research by Lee, Im,
and Taylor (2008) stated that self-disclosure
sometimes could create self-pleasure, thus an
individual enjoyed what they did and considered
social media as a recreation facility.
In addition, 97 subjects (64.7%) from
anonymous and 94 subjects (62.7%) from non-
anonymous also utilized social media to access
information. It is in accordance with a survey by
APJII (2016) that 129.3 million (97.5%) individuals
used social media to share information, it was
because social media could facilitate information-
sharing rapidly. An individual is also motivated to
do so because one has a psychological need to share
information or knowledge to others on social media
(Lee et al., 2008).
62 subjects (41.3%) in anonymous group used
social media as media to communicate, and 76
subjects (50.7%) from non-anonymous did so.
Derlega and Grzelak (in Yang & Tan, 2012) explain
several reasons why an individual do self-disclosure
on social media, one of which is creating closeness
and communication in interpersonal relationship
either with old friends or with new friends. Lee et al.
(2008) add that developing and maintaining a
relationship with the close people becomes a
motivation for an individual to do self-disclosure.
Other intents are for online shopping for 27
subjects (18%) from anonymous group and 17
subjects (11.3%) from non-anonymous groups.
Accordingly, a survey by APJII (2016) found
that 125.5 million (94.6%) people used social
media to trade and 82.2 million (62%) of internet
users often visited online shop. Mulawarman and
Nurfitri (2017) add that at first, social media like
Facebook and Instagram was friendship and
exchanging information site, but today they
become places for marketing for a company or
small-scale online shop.
There were only few subjects using social
media for certain intents such as a platform to
show their hobbies to others, to do promotion, to
stalk, to update new information, a media to
support job and task, learning facility, a platform
to reveal a feeling or to find information, or just
following others.
4 CONCLUSION
This study aimed to know the difference of self-
disclosure on social media users with anonymous
and non-anonymous identity. The result showed that
there was difference of self-disclosure on social
media users with anonymous and non-anonymous
identity that those with anonymous identity had
more self-disclosure than those with non-anonymous
identity and female subjects had higher self-
disclosure than male subjects did. Moreover, the
younger users revealed more personal information
on social media than the elder did.
For this research subjects who are social media
users, either those with anonymous or non-
anonymous, it is important to be more selective and
cautious before revealing information on social
media, particularly personal-related information. For
those who use social media with anonymous
identity, it is expected to understand that a decision
and preference has a positive or negative impact,
including law consequence. Therefore, it is better to
not use social media account to share hate speech
and other negative actions.
Future research can categorize required
demographic data using rating system in order to
ease data analysis, so that result does not broaden
and can be quantified. Moreover, future research can
use qualitative approach to deepen result, especially
about activity and content that are revealed by users
on social media.
REFERENCES
Asosiasi Penyelenggara Jasa Internet Indonesia (APJII).
(2016). Penetrasi & Perilaku Pengguna Internet
Indonesia. Jakarta: Penerbit Polling Indonesia.
ICPsy 2019 - International Conference on Psychology
366
Bak, J. Y., Lin, C. Y., & Oh, A. (2014). Self-disclosure
topic model for Twitter conversations. Proceedings of
the Joint Workshop on Social Dynamics and Personal
Attributes in Social Media, 42-49.
Barak, A., & Gluck-Ofri, O. (2007). Degree and
reciprocity of self-disclosure in online forums.
CyberPsychology & Behavior, 10(3), 407-417. DOI:
10.1089/cpb.2006.9938.
Boyd, D. M., & Ellison, N. B. (2008). Social network
sites: definition, history, and scholarship. Journal of
Computer-Mediated Communication, 13(1), 210-230.
DOI:10.1111/j.1083-6101.2007.00393.x.
Bucur, A., Renold, C., & Henke, M. (1999). How Do
Older Netcitizens Compare With Their Younger
Counterparts?. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 2(6),
505-513.
Childnet International. (2013). Global perspectives on
online anonymity. Youth IGF Project–Childnet
International, 1-9.
Christopherson, K. M. (2006). The positive and negative
implications of anonymity in internet social
interactions: “on the internet, nobody knows you’re a
dog”. Elsevier, 23(1), 3038-3056.
DOI:10.1016/j.chb.2006.09.001.
DeVito, J. A. (2013). The Interpersonal Communication
Book 13
th
Ed. New York: Pearson.
Goodstein, A. (2007). Totally wired: what teens and
tweens are really doing online. New York: St. Martin's
Griffin.
Hoffman, D. L., Novak, T. P., & Peralta, M. (1999).
Building consumer trust online. Communications of
the ACM, 42(4), 80-85.
Joinson, A. N., Woodley, A., & Reips, U. D. (2007).
Personalization, authentication and self-disclosure in
self-administered internet surveys. Computers in
Human Behavior, 23(2), 275–285. DOI:
10.1016/j.chb.2004.10.012.
Kang, H. S., & Yang, H. D. (2004). The effect of
anonymity on the usage of avatar: comparison of
internet relay chat and instant messenger. Proceedings
of the Tenth Americas Conference on Information
Systems, 2734-2743.
Kang, R., Dabbish, L., & Sutton, K. (2016). Strangers on
your phone: why people use anonymous
communication applications. In Proceedings of the
19th ACM Conference on Computer-Supported
Cooperative Work & Social Computing, 359-370.
DOI: 10.1145/2818048.2820081.
Kisilevich, S., Ang, C. S., & Last, M. (2012). Large-scale
analysis of self-disclosure patterns among online
social networks users: a Russian context. Knowledge
and Information Systems, 32(3), 609-628.
Knoll, M., & Bronstein, J. (2013). An examination of the
information disclosure behavior of infertility
bloggers—patterns of self-disclosure and anonymity.
Emerald Group Publishing Limited, 66(2), 175-201.
DOI: 10.1108/AJIM-06-2013-0055.
Lee, D. H., Im, S., & Taylor, C. R. (2008). Voluntary self-
disclosure of information on the internet: A
multimethod study of the motivations and
consequences of disclosing information on blogs.
Psychology & Marketing, 25(7), 692-710. DOI:
10.1002/mar.20232.
Marx, G. T. (1999). What’s in a name? some reflections
on the sociology of anonymity. The Information
Society: An International Journal, 15(2), 99-112. DOI:
10.1080/019722499128565.
McKenna, K. Y. A., & Bargh, J. A. (2000). Plan 9 from
cyberspace: the implications of the internet for
personality and social psychology. Personality and
Social Psychology Review, 4(1), 57-75. DOI:
10.1207/S15327957PSPR0401_6.
Morio, H., & Buchholz, C. (2009). How anonymous are
you online? Examining online social behaviors from a
cross-cultural perspective. AI & Soc, 23(1), 297-307.
DOI: 10.1007/s00146-007-0143-0.
Mulawarman, & Nurfitri, A. D. (2017). Perilaku pengguna
media sosial beserta implikasinya ditinjau dari
perspektif psikologi sosial terapan. Buletin Psikologi,
25(1), 36-44.
Schouten, A. P. (2007). Adolescents’ online self-disclosure
and self-presentation. Unpublished Doctoral
Dissertation. University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam.
Suler, J. (2004). The online disinhibition effect. Cyber
Psychology & Behavior, 7(3), 321-326.
Wheeless, L. R., & Grotz, J. (1976). Conceptualization
and measurement of reported self-disclosure. Human
Communication Reasearch, 2(4), 338-346.
Yang, L. & Tan, B. C. Y. (2012). Self-disclosure on online
social networks: motives, context feature, and media
capabilities. Human-Computer Interactions, 1-11.
Difference of Self-disclosure on Social Media Users with Anonymous and Non-anonymous Identity
367