Gratitude Journal, Expressive Writing, and Mindfulness
Ka Yan
a
and Priska Analya
b
Faculty of Psychology, Maranatha Christian University, Jl. Surya Sumantri No. 65, Bandung, Indonesia
Keywords: Gratitude Journal, Expressive Writing, Mindfulness.
Abstract: This study, divided into two sub-studies, was aimed at determining the influence of writing on mindfulness.
The first substudy investigated the influence of gratitude journals on mindfulness, while the second substudy
sought for the influence of expressive writing on mindfulness. The participants were 72 students (first
substudy) and 23 students (second substudy). Both substudies involved students grouped in the experimental
group (who received treatment writing) and the control group who did not receive the treatment. The results
of statistical tests in the first substudy showed that there was no effect of writing gratitude journal on
mindfulness, while the results of statistical tests in the second substudy indicated that the score of the group
with expressive writing treatment was higher than that without the treatment. There was no significant
influence of gratitude journal on mindfulness. Similarly, expressive writing showed no significant influence
on mindfulness. However, there was a moderate effect size in participants who received expressive writing
treatment, which needed further study involving larger number of samples. Researchers can also then consider
the characteristics of the sample to be studied (not just students) so as to get a broader picture
1 INTRODUCTION
It is known that period of study is a stressful time,
where students often face personal, social, academic,
economic stress, and so on in various areas of life so
that they are prone to stress (Mutalik et al. 2016).
Hunt & Eisenberg, (2010) revealed that there is an
increase in the number of students at various colleges
experiencing mental health problems. This is because
students face both academic and non-academic
pressures during their college experience—similarly,
the students of psychology, who in their daily life can
not escape from various kinds of forces. Various
studies across the globe have emphasised that
students undertaking professional courses, are
subjected to higher stress (Waghachavare et al. 2013).
The students of the psychology faculty are
classified as psychology scientists who, after their
graduation, have a certain authority to provide
psychological services that aim to make efforts to
prevent, develop, and solve psychological problems
(HIMPSI 2010). The students of the faculty of
psychology are required to be able to help while
providing alternative problem solving for the people
a
https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3720-2258
b
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5660-1548
who need their expertise (Fitrianingrum 2009). One
of the skills required by psychology students is the
ability to manage their own stress first before
connecting with the client they are about to face. This
is because if the students of the faculty of psychology
have not solved their personal problems first, it will
be difficult for them to deal with other people's
problems.
Stress is a condition or feeling experienced when
an individual perceives that the demands exceed the
individual's personal abilities (Lazarus and Folkman
1984). When the stress of the individual is
experienced, the individual will have the urge to
reduce the perceived stress level. Each individual has
a different way of reducing the level of stress
experienced. Some individuals focus on problem-
solving coping to lower stress levels, but some others
focus on processing their emotions to reduce the level
of stress experienced.
One of the strategies that can be done to process
emotions by individuals is by overflowing the
emotions they bear through spoken or written words
when experiencing high stress. Susilowati & Hasanat
(2015) stated that writing emotional experiences can
448
Yan, K. and Analya, P.
Gratitude Journal, Expressive Writing, and Mindfulness.
DOI: 10.5220/0010753600003112
In Proceedings of the 1st International Conference on Emerging Issues in Humanity Studies and Social Sciences (ICE-HUMS 2021), pages 448-452
ISBN: 978-989-758-604-0
Copyright
c
2022 by SCITEPRESS Science and Technology Publications, Lda. All rights reserved
reduce negative emotions possessed by individuals. It
is also in line with a research conducted by
Pennebaker & Chung (2007) that there was a decrease
in negative emotions in students who were asked to
write down their emotional experiences. Writing this
emotional experience is also known as expressive
writing. In addition to expressive writing, one way to
cope with stress is to create a gratitude journal.
Gratitude is defined as a distinctive pleasant feeling—
in the form of gratitude, or gratitude—that arises
when receiving kindness (kindness, compassion,
love), benefits, or altruistic help from others—
especially things that are not actually worthy of
acceptance, which are things that are not caused by
self-effort (Emmons and McCullough 2004). In
recent years, research has shown that gratitude is
strongly associated with some aspects of well-being
and mental health (Watkins, Uhder, and Pichinevskiy
2015), and clinical interventions have been developed
to improve gratitude as a way to foster positive
function, improve well-being, and decrease
psychological disorders (Emmons and McCullough
2003).
In addition, some studies have found gratitude
practices, such as creating gratitude journals that can
improve one's happiness and positive mood.
Gratitude journal or counting blessings is an
intervention designed to improve gratitude (Emmons
and McCullough 2003). This activity involves
writing about five things to be grateful for every day
or every week. In recent years, many studies have
tested the benefits of gratitude in children and
adolescents, for example, older teens will be more
interested in and satisfied with their school life, more
generous and helpful, and more socially integrated.
Other studies suggested that gratitude journaling in
the classroom changed the mood of the students. It is
said that by improving students' subjective well-
being, accompanied by an increase in positive
emotions, students are expected to feel happier and
not feel depressed in their college life (Christanto
2017).
Although many studies stated that gratitude can
improve a person's quality of life, it can not always
improve life satisfaction because when a person is
unaware of an emotional state, then he can show the
thought and action of prototypes caused by a
particular emotional state (Lambie and Marcel 2002).
This is because their awareness of themselves is not
fully activated (Bargh and Chartrand 1999). On the
contrary, expressive writing gives a person the
opportunity to be more 'open' to his emotional state.
Bishop et al. (2004) describes mindfulness as a
process to bring quality attention at any time at a time
from an experience. There are two components in
operational mindfulness, namely (1) maintaining
attention to the current experience and (2) being an
attitude of curiosity, openness, and acceptance of
one's experience. Mindfulness is an approach to raise
awareness and respond to mental processes that
contribute to emotional distress and maladaptive
behavior. Based on this, mindfulness can bridge
relationships related to gratitude and expressive
writing, and life satisfaction (Poon, Alvin; Danoff-
Burg 2011). However, as far as the researchers know,
there has not been much research on these subjects,
so the researchers are interested in researching more
on gratitude journal, expressive writing, and
mindfulness.
2 METHODS (AND MATERIALS)
The research question of this study was whether
gratitude journaling and expressive writing
influenced mindfulness. The study consisted of 2
substudies using experimental methods. Participants
were students, who joined those substudies
voluntarily. Participants in this study were students
who experienced stresses with moderate or high
degrees. The first study, involving 72 students who
were grouped into experimental and control groups
(38 students per group, using randomization for
categorizing to each group). The students in the
experimental group were asked to write down the
positive things that happened in their life (gratitude
journal), namely: 1) what to be grateful for that day,
2) what to expect for the next day, and 3) random
questions on positive things in their life (questions
varied). Journaling was committed for two weeks,
with each week having 3 times of the practice. This
refers to a study from (Lyubomirsky & Layous 2013)
saying that writing a gratitude journal would be more
effectively done once a week than done every day.
The second substudy, involving 23 students who
were also grouped into experimental and control
groups (with 12 students included into experimental
and 11 students included into control group). Students
in the experimental group were asked to write
emotional experiences for 25-30 minutes a day for 4x.
In this writing, students did not need to pay attention
to writing structure but were expected to expand and
explore their deepest experiences and emotions. The
concern of the researchers was the use of words
containing negative as well as positive emotional
content. In addition, in the first and second study, the
participants from both groups (experiments and
controls) should fill out a State Mindfulness
Gratitude Journal, Expressive Writing, and Mindfulness
449
Questionnaire (SMS) at the end of the session to find
out how their mindfulness condition after getting the
treatment (for the experimental group).
SMS is a standard questionnaire compiled by
Tanay & Bernstein (2020), consisting of 21 items,
with five answer options: not at all, slightly
describing, quite describing, illustrating a lot, and
strongly describing the participants. This measuring
instrument has two factors, namely state mindfulness
of mind with Pearson validity value ranging from
0.499-0.815 and state mindfulness of body with
Pearson validity value ranging from 0.621-0.839. The
reliability obtained with alpha Cronbach is 0.755.
The study results were processed using JASP
software (Jeffreys's Amazing Statistics Program), a
free statistical analysis software (open-source)
created by the Department of Psychological Methods,
University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
Table 1: Substudy 1. Experimental group.
Gender Percentage (amount)
Male 10,53% (4)
Female 89,47 (34)
Table 2: Substudy 1. Control group.
Gender Percentage (amount)
Male 5,89% (2)
Female 94,12% (32)
Based on the description in the table 1 and 2, in
the study 1, majority of participants were female
(89.4% from the experimental group and 94.12%
from the control group).
Table 3: Substudy 2. Experimental group.
Gender Percentage (amount)
Male 25% (3)
Female 75% (9)
Table 4: Substudy 2. Control group.
Gender Percentage (amount)
Male 0% (0)
Female 100% (11)
Based on the description in the table 3 and 4, in
the substudy 2 majority of participants were female
(75% experimental group and 100% control group).
3 RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
Table 5: Substudy 1. Descriptive statistics.
Mindfulness
Experimental Control
Valid 38 34
Mean 67.921 69.294
Std. Deviation 5.697 11.074
Minimum 55.000 49.000
Maximum 80.000 91.000
Table 6: Independent samples T-test.
T df P Cohen’s
d
mindfulness -0.650 48.073 0.519 -0.156
Table 7: Test of normality (Shapiro-Wilk).
W P
Mindfulness Experimental 0.984 0.841
Control 0.977 0.685
Note. Significant results suggest a deviation from
normality.
Table 8: Test of equality of variance (Lavene’s).
F df P
Mindfulness 13.838 1 < .001
The tables 7 and 8 show that the research data in study
1 are normal (p > 0.05) but not of equal invariance (p
< 0.001), therefore independent sample t-test (welch's
t-test) is used Ahad & Yahaya (2014). As a result,
there is no effect of gratitude journaling on
mindfulness. This is in line with the mean between
experimental groups and controls, which are not
significantly different.
Table 9: Substudy 2. Descriptive statistics.
Mindfulness
Experimental Control
Valid 12 11
Mean 78.167 72.091
Std. Deviation 10.760 5.319
Minimum 56.000 62.000
Maximum 98.000 82.000
Table 10: Independent samples T-test.
t Df p Cohen’s d
Score 1.691 1 0.106 0.706
ICE-HUMS 2021 - International Conference on Emerging Issues in Humanity Studies and Social Sciences
450
Table 11: Test of normality (Shapiro-Wilk).
W p
Score Experimental
Control
0.975
0.947
0.955
0.610
Note. Significant results suggest a deviation from
normality.
Table 12: Test of equality of variances (Lavene’s).
F f p
Score 2.873 1 0.105
The tables 11 and 12 show that the research data
in study 2 are classified as normal (p > 0.05) and of
equal invariance (p > 0.05), therefore an independent
sample t-test (student t-test) is used (Ahad and
Yahaya 2014). As a result, there is no expressive
writing effect on mindfulness. However, the effect
size indicates 0.706 (cohen's d), which falls into the
moderate category. This is in line with the mean of a
larger experimental group than the control group
(78,167 > 72,091).
Study 1 did not show the effect of gratitude on
mindfulness. Both the experimental and control
groups got relatively similar averages. This is in line
with the exposure put forward by Lyubomirsky &
Layous (2013), which stated that gratitude
intervention has an unstable effect. Research Cregg &
Cheavens (2021) revealed that even if there is a
change in well-being by using gratitude intervention,
it is just a placebo effect. Cregg & Cheavens (2021)
conducted a meta-analysis and stated that gratitude
intervention only has a small effect on symptoms of
depression and anxiety. Gratitude is associated with
positively interpreting several stimuli and life events,
which is contrary to selective attention aimed
negatively at the self, the world, and the future, which
is a character of depression and anxiety. Although the
researchers did not conduct research related to
depression and anxiety, the results of some of the
above researchers need to be a record of the
effectiveness of gratitude journal.
Study 2 found almost the same result, stating that
expressive writing had no effect on mindfulness.
Nonetheless, it was interesting that the average
mindfulness of the experimental group was higher
than that of the control group and the effect size test
(Cohen's d) stated that there were moderate effects.
Ericson (2015) stated that interventions that can be
administered over the internet at this low cost show
that it can prevent and reduce physical and emotional
symptoms. Expressive writing may provide benefits
if participants experience a lower negative affectivity
after making their writing. This is in line with several
studies which stated that expressive writing can
improve the people’s well being (Merz, Fox, and
Malcarne 2014).
Mindfulness accepts thoughts, emotions, and
sensations that arise from consciousness in the
present without giving judgment (Neff and Dahm
2015). In other words, according to the researchers,
mindfulness encourages a person to accept reality for
what it is, both negative and positive. This is in line
with expressive writing, where participants are asked
to write down what they feel, which is not always a
form of negative emotion. Thus, expressive writing
encourages participants to see reality as it is, and by
releasing those emotions, participants avoid
rumination and increased mindfulness. Rumination is
defined as focusing attention on a negative emotional
state, such as intrusive thinking of distressing events,
looking for the meaning of negative things, and
thinking about how to change the negative.
Mindfulness can 'solve' that thought, which
emphasizes the awareness of the present moment
(Deyo et al. 2009).
According to this finding, there is a difference in
focus between expressive writing and gratitude
journal, that has an impact on the difference in the
results between substudy 1 and substudy 2. When
expressive writing emphasizes the disclosure of what
is felt, both negative and positive, gratitude journal
emphasizes more on the disclosure of positive affect
(grateful). This may make the negative affect that
participants actually feel unexpressed, which
ultimately is not resolved. In this case, the
participants did not 'learn' how to receive this
negative affect. This is in line with Pennebaker &
Chung (2007), who stated that expressive writing is
tought to be helpful to express suppressed thoughts,
emotions and give an opportunity for exposure to
unpleasent emotions. The process of expressive
writing also involves labeling emotions related to an
unpleasant event which allows one to assign the event
meaning and accommodate it into one’s existing
cognitive scheme, and thus let go of unresolved
negative emotions. This is different from gratitude
journal which is focusing on positive aspects of lives
rather than allowing to be overwhelmed by negative
events. This makes the mindfulness of the
participants not improve. Nonetheless, this argument
still needs further study. The expressive writing
participants in this study are still very limited, so it
needs further study on how it effects on larger
samples.
Gratitude Journal, Expressive Writing, and Mindfulness
451
4 CONCLUSIONS
Writing in journals has no impact on increasing
mindfulness. However, there was a moderate effect
size in participants who received expresive writing
treatment, which needed further study involving
larger samples. Future studies can also consider the
characteristics of the sample to be studied (not just
students) so as to get a broader picture.
REFERENCES
Ahad, N. A., & Yahaya, S.S.S. (2014). “Sensitivity
Analysis of Welch’s t -Test.” AIP Conference
Proceedings 1605:888–93. doi: 10.1063/1.4887707.
Bargh, J.A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). “The Unbearable
Automaticity of Being.” American Psychologist
54(7):462–79. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.462.
Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson,
N. D., Carmody, J., Segal, Z. V., Abbey, S., Speca, M.,
Velting, D., & Devins, G. (2004). “Mindfulness: A
Proposed Operational Definition.” Clinical
Psychology: Science and Practice 11(3):230–41. doi:
10.1093/clipsy/bph077.
Christanto, S. A. (2017). “Studi Hubungan Anxiety Dan
Attentional Bias Dalam Emotional Stroop Task.”
Cregg, D. R., & Cheavens, J. S. (2021). “Gratitude
Interventions : Effective Self Help ? A Meta
Analysis of the Impact on Symptoms of Depression and
Anxiety.” Journal of Happiness Studies 22(1):413–45.
doi: 10.1007/s10902-020-00236-6.
Deyo, M., Wilson, K. A., Ong, J., & Koopman, C. (2009).
“Original Research Mindfulness and Rumination: Does
Mindfulness Training Lead to Reductions in the
Ruminative Thinking Associated with Depression?”
JSCH 5(5):265–71. doi: 10.1016/j.explore.2009.06.
005.
Emmons, R. A., & McCullough, M. E. (2003). “Counting
Blessings versus Burdens: An Experimental
Investigation of Gratitude and Subjective Well-Being
in Daily Life.” Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology 84(2):377–89. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.
2.377.
Emmons, R. A., & McCullough, M. E. (2004). The
Psychology of Gratitude. Oxford University Press.
Ericson, S. K. (2015). “The Effects of Positive Expressive
Writing on Postpartum Wellbeing: A Comparison of
Mindfulness and Self-Affirmation.”
Fitrianingrum, U. (2009). “Perilaku Koping Pada
Mahasiswa Psikologi Yang Mengalami Kecemasan
Komunikasi Interpersonal.”
HIMPSI. 2010. “Kode Etik Psikologi.” 132.
Hunt, J., & Eisenberg, D. (2010). “Mental Health Problems
and Help-Seeking Behavior among College Students.”
Journal of Adolescent Health 46(1):3–10. doi:
10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.08.008.
Lambie, J. A., & Marcel, A. J. (2002). “Consciousness and
the Varieties of Emotion Experience: A Theoretical
Framework.” Psychological Review 109(2):219–59.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.109.2.219.
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, Appraisal,
and Coping. New York: Springer Publishing Company.
Lyubomirsky, S., & Layous, K. (2013). “How Do Simple
Positive Activities Increase.” doi: 10.1177/09637214
12469809.
Merz, E. L, Fox, R.S., & Malcarne, V.L. (2014).
“Expressive Writing Interventions in Cancer Patients:
A Systematic Review.” Health Psychology Review
8(3):339–61. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2014.882007.
Mutalik, N. R., Moni, S., Choudhari, S. B., & Bhogale, G.
S. (2016). “Depression, Anxiety, Stress among College
Students in Bagalkot: A College Based Study.”
International Journal of Indian Psychology 3(4). doi:
10.25215/0304.210.
Neff, K. D., & Dahm, K. A. (2015). “Self-Compassion:
What It Is, What It Does, and How It Relates to
Mindfulness.” Handbook of Mindfulness and Self-
Regulation 121–40. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4939-2263-
5_10.
Pennebaker, J. W., & Chung, C. K. (2007). “Expressive
Writing, Emotional Upheavals, and Health.”
ResearchGate (January 2007):23–38. doi: 10.1007/
978-3-030-39903-0_1225.
Poon, A., & Sharon, D-B. (2011). “Mindfulness as a
Moderator in Expressive Writing.” Clinical Psychologu
67(9):881–95.
Susilowati, T. G., & Hasanat, N.U. (2015). “Pengaruh
Terapi Menulis Pengalaman Emosional Terhadap
Penurunan Depresi Pada Mahasiswa Tahun Pertama.”
Jurnal Psikologi 38(1):92 107–107. doi:
10.22146/jpsi.7669.
Tanay, G., & Bernstein, A. (2020). “State Mindfulness
Scale ( SMS ): Development and Initial Validation
State Mindfulness Scale ( SMS ).” (November). doi:
10.1037/a0034044.
Waghachavare, V. B., Dhumale, G.B., Kadam, Y. R., &
Gore, A. D. (2013). “A Study of Stress among Students
of Professional Colleges from an Urban Area in India.”
Sultan Qaboos University Medical Journal 13(3):422–
29. doi: 10.12816/0003266.
Watkins, P. C., Uhder, J., & Pichinevskiy, S. (2015).
“Grateful Recounting Enhances Subjective Well-
Being: The Importance of Grateful Processing.”
Journal of Positive Psychology 10(2):91–98. doi:
10.1080/17439760.2014.927909.
ICE-HUMS 2021 - International Conference on Emerging Issues in Humanity Studies and Social Sciences
452