The Analysis of Test-Taking Anxiety from a Sample of Chinese
Students Studying Abroad and in the Home Country
Rihua Min
1,*,+ a
, Tianyu Han
2,+ b
and Zhihan Yang
3,+ c
1
College of Arts and Sciences, Syracuse University, 331 Chinook Dr, Syracuse, U.S.A.
2
Nanjing No.9 Middle School, No.51 Beiting Alley, Nanjing, China
3
Changzhou senior high school of Jiangsu Province, No.8 Luohan Road, Changzhou, China
*
Corresponding author
These authors contributed equally
Keywords: Test-Taking Anxiety, Chinese Students, The United States.
Abstract: In light of the high prevalence of anxiety among students and severe test-taking anxiety among Chinese
students, one of our main purposes is to discuss whether studying abroad in the United States could relieve
test-taking anxiety, and we thus merely recruited participants who were Chinese to test hypotheses.
Participants in our experiment were N = 44 Chinese students (age range: 16-24 years, 33 females and 11
males) recruited from multiple high schools and universities across China and the United States. After a
comprehensive and comparative analysis, we found that Chinese students studying abroad had less test-taking
anxiety than those studying in their home country China.
Test forms (test with or without rewards) had no
relationship with test-taking anxiety and no interaction with countries for study. The relationship between
test-taking anxiety and test scores was largely negative. According to our findings, however, there was no
relationship between gender and test-taking anxiety.
1 INTRODUCTION
Mental illness, as a prevalent issue among students,
has a negative relationship with academic
performance. Of college students, 25 % have been
diagnosed with or been treated for mental health
illnesses (Posselt, & Lipson, 2016). Posselt and
Lipson (2016) also claimed that anxiety, a form of
mental illness, is one of the top factors that impair the
academic achievements of college students.
Similarly, of middle and high school students in
Shanghai, 28.3% were tested as abnormally anxious
(Gu, Gong, & Zhang, 2009). Taking tests is a process
every student will experience in their school lives, but
what is its relationship with anxiety needs to be
explored. Contemplating multiple causes that give
rise to anxiety is imprudent and complicated, thus
focusing on test-taking concerning anxiety is our aim.
China and the United States, because of their
different cultural and educational patterns, bring
a
https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0064-7544
b
https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0720-0251
c
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8619-3565
about discrepant influences on students. However, the
students in both countries might have more or less
pressure and anxiety caused by tests in common.
Test-taking anxiety of Chinese students has been a
hot issue in Chinese society for many years and even
attracted the attention of the world because of the
severe pressure of study and examination brought by
the exam-oriented education system. The prevalence
of anxiety symptoms among middle and high school
students in China reaches 9.89% (Xu, Mao, Wei, Liu,
Fan, Wang, Wang, Lou, Lin, Wang, & Wu, 2021).
One of our main purposes is to discuss whether
studying abroad in the United States could relieve
test-taking anxiety, and we thus merely recruited
participants who were Chinese to test hypotheses.
Owing to the deficiency of research on test-taking
anxiety, we list a bunch of hypotheses and try to
discover the potential factor connected to test-taking
anxiety, including gender, test forms, test scores, and
the goal of the test. Our main method for
394
Min, R., Han, T. and Yang, Z.
The Analysis of Test-Taking Anxiety from a Sample of Chinese Students Studying Abroad and in the Home Country.
DOI: 10.5220/0011912700003613
In Proceedings of the 2nd International Conference on New Media Development and Modernized Education (NMDME 2022), pages 394-402
ISBN: 978-989-758-630-9
Copyright
c
2023 by SCITEPRESS Science and Technology Publications, Lda. Under CC license (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0)
measurement was Form Y-1 from State-Trait Anxiety
Inventory (STAI) (Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene,
Vagg, & Jacobs, 1983). Our participants in the
experiment were all Chinese students, recruited from
multiple high schools and universities across China
and the United States, aged 16-24.
2 LITERATURE REVIEW
Stowell and Bennett (2010) found that test-taking
anxiety could be reflected by its “affective (physical
arousal, emotionality), cognitive (worry), and
behavioral (procrastination, avoidance)
components”, together impairing the academic
achievement of college students. Zeidner (1998) also
referred to test-taking anxiety as “the set of
phenomenological, physiological, and behavioral
responses that accompany concern about possible
negative consequences of failure on an exam or
similar evaluative situation”. We conceptualize the
reason for test-taking anxiety among Chinese
students in terms of the contemporary goal
classification theory (Elliot, 1999; Pintrich, 2000) and
make hypotheses based on prior research findings.
2.1 Contemporary Goal Classification
Theory
According to the contemporary goal classification
theory proposed by Elliot (1999) and Pintrich (2000),
learning goals can be divided into 4 major types:
mastery approach goal -- the purpose of personal
participation in activities is seeing their progress and
improvement; mastery avoidance goal -- the purpose
of personal participation is to avoid their
shortcomings actively; performance-approach goal --
the purpose of personal participation in activities is to
expect a positive evaluation of the outside world;
performance-avoidance goal -- the purpose of
personal participation in activities is to avoid the
negative evaluation of the outside world. Concluded
from their later findings, Linnenbrink and Pintrich
(2000) associated learning goals with anxiety as
follows: mastery approach goal is associated with low
anxiety, mastery avoidance goal and performance-
approach goal are related to moderate anxiety, and the
performance-avoidance goal is correlated with high
anxiety.
Because the educational system in China is exam-
oriented, every test is highly valued by Chinese
students as an assessment of their self-esteem. As a
result, test-taking anxiety escalates in the preparation
of the test. Given the previous investigation in high
schools, test-taking anxiety was a common problem
among Chinese students. Of the students who were
anxious about tests, 74.8% had moderate or severe
test-taking anxiety, and showed strong performance-
avoidance goals and performance-approach goals; the
rest (25.2%) with mild anxiety mostly showed
mastery avoidance goals, and a small number of them
showed the other three goals (Cui, Liu, & Gao, 2008).
In addition, a study on Chinese college students
revealed that the anxiety level of college students was
concentrated in the low and middle levels, lower than
that of middle school students. Their learning goals
were mainly embodied as mastery approach goal,
mastery avoidance goal, and performance-approach
goal, which are associated with low and moderate
anxiety (Wang, 2013).
There are some similarities and differences in test-
taking anxiety between Chinese and American
students, as evidenced by previous studies. For both
Chinese and American students, taking the foreign
language test in Chinese and American colleges
students, for example, the learning goals leading to
their test-taking anxiety are comparable. In terms of
learning goals, American students experience test-
taking anxiety because they are concerned about the
failure of the application of examinations to life and
the negative evaluation from others, whereas the test-
taking anxiety of Chinese students is only reflected in
the fear of negative evaluation from others (Tang,
2012). However, as per one research on test-taking
anxiety of high school students in China and the
United States, the learning goals leading to test-taking
anxiety among students in the two countries are
different. Chinese students are more worried about
the negative evaluation from peers, parents, teachers,
and people from the outside world if they fail the
exam, while American students are merely concerned
about their test performance not being able to be
employed in real life pragmatically and the test failure
may harm their chance of further study (Huang,
Huang, Xing, Sanche, & Ye, 2005).
2.2 Competition
Posselt and Lipson (2016) argued the sense of
competition highly increases the incidence of test-
taking anxiety among students. They are afraid of bad
test performance and its subsequent negative
outcome, which brings about tremendous anxiety.
2.3 Gender
In terms of gender, female students had higher
anxiety levels than male students because of greater
The Analysis of Test-Taking Anxiety from a Sample of Chinese Students Studying Abroad and in the Home Country
395
emotional fluctuations (Li, & Liu, 1999). According
to a survey of high school students (Huang, 2003), the
degree of test-taking anxiety of girls was significantly
higher than that of boys, and the number of girls who
had severe and moderate anxiety is much more than
boys. Surprisingly, in another middle school, the
degree of anxiety of female students is almost the
same as (slightly higher than) that of male students.
The anxiety of boys comes primarily from parents’
high expectations, while that of the girls is produced
mostly from the negative self-cognition caused by
examinations and the pressure from school and
society (Wang, Lu, Chen, & Xia, 2005).
2.4 Test Performance
Many research findings have demonstrated that test-
taking anxiety greatly lowers test performance
(Chapell, Blanding, Silverstein, Takahashi, Newman,
Gubi, & McCann, 2005; Barrows, Dunn, & Lloyd,
2013; Posselt, & Lipson, 2016; Gharib, Phillips, &
Mathew, 2012; Stowell, & Bennett, 2010; Cassady, &
Johnson, 2002; Zeidner, 1998). In a study with 5551
participants who completed STAI (Spielberger et al.,
1983) and reported their cumulative GPA and grades,
there was “a one-third letter grade difference between
undergraduates with high test anxiety and lower test
anxiety” (Chapell et al., 2005). Likewise, Barrows et
al. (2013) pointed out that 10 million primary and
secondary students performed badly in tests due to
their test-taking anxiety.
2.5 Psychopathology
Anxiety disorder increasingly happens to students. In
the study at Uludag University, among 4850 students,
29.6% and 36.7% obtained the anxiety scores from
the evaluations of STAI Form Y-1 (state anxiety) and
Y-2 (trait anxiety), respectively (Spielberger et al.,
1983), higher than the cut-off point of
psychopathology, which means they might have an
anxiety disorder (Ozen, Ercan, Irgil, & Sigirli, 2010).
2.6 Hypotheses
Given the contemporary goal classification theory
and previous literature, we make the following
hypotheses:
H1: There would be some differences in test-
taking anxiety between Chinese students studying
abroad in the United States and the home country
when taking a test with or without rewards (the
reward might somehow provoke a sense of
competition).
H2: Female students would have higher test-
taking anxiety than male students.
H3: There would be some differences in test
scores between Chinese students studying abroad in
the United States and the home country when taking
a test with or without rewards.
H4: Test-taking anxiety would have a negative
relationship with test scores.
Besides, we would not only analyze the
relationship between the goal of the test and test-
taking anxiety but compare the incidence of anxiety
disorder before and after the awareness of the
existence of a test. We only focus on Chinese students
as our participants to control variables and observe
whether studying abroad in the United States could
alleviate test-taking anxiety, thus further proving the
success of the American educational pattern in terms
of the alleviation of test-taking anxiety.
3 METHOD
3.1 Participants and procedure
Participants in our experiment were N = 44 Chinese
students (age range: 16-24 years, 33 females and 11
males) recruited from multiple high schools and
universities across China and the United States. Of
them, N = 24 had been studying abroad in the United
States for at least 2 years and N = 20 studied in their
home country China (they had never been to foreign
countries including the United States). They all
signed a consent form before being asked to
participate. To ensure those Chinese students who
studied abroad had assimilated American cultural and
educational patterns, we set the threshold 2 years in
the United States; likewise, Chinese students who
studied in the home country were set the threshold as
having never been to foreign countries to prevent
them from contacting foreign cultural and educational
patterns. While they were found fit for the
requirements, we contacted them through either email
or WeChat (a Chinese social media) in favor of their
preference. Then the participants were divided into
four groups: students who studied abroad took a test
with a reward; students who studied abroad took a test
without a reward; students who studied in the home
country took a test with a reward; students who
studied in the home country took a test without
reward. To simulate a real test environment, we
arrange all participants in each group within one
setting.
Our experiment consists of two STAI (Form Y-1)
(Spielberger et al., 1983) questionnaires, one in the
NMDME 2022 - The International Conference on New Media Development and Modernized Education
396
very beginning (without knowing the existence of a
test) and the other one just before taking the test
(knowing the existence of a test), a test asking basic
questions (the difficulty is set the same as that of
middle school in China) that costs 7 minutes, and a
questionnaire for debriefing. The experiment was
conducted on Zoom, an online meeting app, thus it
was conducted in a video-conferencing format. All
participants attended the meeting punctually and
opened cameras throughout the whole experiment. N
= 8 participants wore a mask in the process. The
experiment on average lasted for 25 minutes for each
group.
3.2 Instruments
Two STAI questionnaires (created on Qualtrics) were
assessed using the same but randomized 20 items
from Form Y-1 to measure state anxiety (Spielberger
et al., 1983). To avoid the language barrier, a
Chinese-version STAI was utilized by attaching the
Chinese translation to each item and prompt (Wang,
Wang, & Ma, 1999). The items were administrated
with a four-point rating scale, ranging from not at all
(1) to very much so (4) for the negative term (e.g., I
feel tense). The order of points for each item was
reversed for the positive term (e.g., I feel secure). We
subtracted the first state anxiety score (one in the very
beginning) from the second one (one just before
taking the test) to measure how much anxiety induced
by a test (i.e., test-taking anxiety equaled the second
state anxiety score minus the first state anxiety score).
The rewards for the test were random delicate gifts.
The test (created by Wenjuanxing, a Chinese
survey maker) in the experiment included 13
questions with the full marks as 100 -- 10 multiple-
choice questions (each was worth 6 points), 2
multiple-answer questions (each was worth 13 points,
and partial credits, 6 points, were allowed only if
getting one choice wrong), and 1 short-answer
question that was worth 14 points. Questions were
interdisciplinary, given from tests of middle school
(age 14) in China. There were four test papers for four
groups of participants and participants in each group
would do the same test paper.
Test Examples:
Multiple-choice question: In a hexagon, the
sum of degrees of interior angles is …
a. 360
b. 720
c. 860
d. 1080
Multiple-answer question: Which of the
following is not able to conduct electricity?
a. Iron
b. Pure water
c. Graphite
d. Human bodies
e. Silicon
f. Air
Short-answer question: Why do women
annually earn less money than men on average?
The questionnaire (also created on Wenjuanxing)
for debriefing asked participants to be debriefed on
their feelings before and after the test. In addition,
they were requested to indicate their goal of the test.
Questions were all set open-ended.
3.3 Analyses
Regarding the first and third hypotheses, we
computed a 2×2 Between-Subjects ANOVA for each.
Concerning the second hypothesis, we computed an
independent-samples t-test. While for the fourth
hypothesis, we made a correlation analysis. To reach
coherence and consistency in analyses, we set the
critical region in all tests α = .05. Then for the fifth
hypothesis, we compared the descriptive statistics,
based on test-taking anxiety scores and debriefings,
and made an analysis. Lastly, for the sixth hypothesis,
we computed the anxiety scores of Chinese students
before and after knowing the existence of the test and
then compared them with the cut-off point of 40 to
find the probable number of Chinese students who
could be diagnosed with anxiety disorder arising from
the test (Womble, Jennings, Schatz, & Elbin, 2021).
Our data was all collected in Microsoft Excel and
evaluated in JASP.
4 RESULTS
4.1 Test-Taking Anxiety VS Countries
for Study VS Test Forms
Descriptive Statistics are shown in Table 1. A 2×2
Between-Subjects ANOVA revealed a statistically
significant main effect of countries for study. Chinese
students who study abroad (N = 24) experienced less
test-taking anxiety (M = -.42, SD = 8.36) than those
who study in the home country (N = 20, M = 10.05,
SD = 15.93), F
(1, 40)
= 7.63, p < .01, η
2
p
= .16.
Regarding test forms, analyses revealed that the main
effect was not statistically significant, F
(1, 40)
< .01, p
= .97, η
2
p
< .01. With respect to the interaction
between countries for study and test forms, analyses
revealed a not significant interaction effect, F
(1, 40)
=
1.03, p = .32, η
2
p
= .32. For further analyses of the
The Analysis of Test-Taking Anxiety from a Sample of Chinese Students Studying Abroad and in the Home Country
397
relationship between countries for study and test-
taking anxiety among Chinese students, an
independent-samples t-test indicated that Chinese
students who study abroad had lower test-taking
anxiety than those who study in the home country, t
(42)
= -2.79, p = .01 (see Figure 1).
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of test-taking anxiety for four groups of Chinese students
Countries for study Test Forms M SD N
Studying abroad Test with reward 1.583 5.282 12
Test without reward -2.417 10.457 12
Studying in the home country Test with reward 8.200 14.748 10
Test without reward 11.900 17.629 10
Note. Descriptive statistics of test-taking anxiety based on STAI (Form Y-1) index for four groups of Chinese students
aged 16-24.
Figure 1. The relationship between countries for study and test-taking anxiety among Chinese students aged 16-24 was
established by an independent-samples t-test. The horizontal axis indicated countries for study, which used “number 1” to
denote Chinese students who study abroad and “number 2” to denote those who study in the home country.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics of test scores for four groups of Chinese students
Countries for Study Test Forms M SD N
Studying abroad Test with reward 48.000 16.564 12
Test without reward 48.083 16.714 12
Studying in the home country Test with reward 40.900 15.242 10
Test without reward 42.000 15.362 10
Note. Descriptive statistics of test scores based on four test papers, with interdisciplinary questions within the time limit
of 7 minutes for each one, for four groups of Chinese students aged 16-24.
NMDME 2022 - The International Conference on New Media Development and Modernized Education
398
Figure 2. Correlation between test-taking anxiety and test scores among Chinese students aged 16-24 (N = 44).
4.2 Test-Taking Anxiety VS Gender
An independent-samples t-test revealed that there was
no relationship between gender (33 females and 11
males) and test-taking anxiety, t
(42)
= -.55, p = .58. The
effect size was relatively small, Cohen’s d = -.19.
4.3 Test Scores VS Countries for Study
VS Test Forms
Table 2 shows the descriptive statistics mainly
focused on test scores. A 2×2 Between-Subjects
ANOVA revealed that in regard to countries for
study, there was no statistically significant main
effect, F
(1,40)
= 1.84, p = .18, η
2
p
= .04. Likewise, the
main effect of test forms was not statistically
significant, F
(1,40)
= .02, p = .90, η
2
p
< .01. There was
also no significant interaction between countries for
study and test forms, F
(1,40)
= .01, p = .92, η
2
p
< .01
.
4.4 Test-Taking Anxiety VS Test
Scores
In the sample of 44 Chinese students, the relationship
between test-taking anxiety and test scores was
Pearson’s coefficient r
(42)
= -.49, p < .01. This
negative relationship was nearly large. Results
showed that 24% of the variability in test scores was
determined by test-taking anxiety, r
2
= .24 (see Figure
2).
Table 3. Descriptive statistics of goal types of the test for four groups of Chinese students
Levels of anxiety
Goal types
Studying in the home
country
Studying abroad Total
M SD N M SD N M SD N
Mastery approach goal
35.17 7.06 6 35.22 7.67 9 35.20 7.43 15
Mastery avoidance goal
43.57 10.94 7 41.50 7.50 4 42.82 9.88 11
Performance approach goal
58.00 12.75 4 45.25 8.93 8 49.50 11.98 12
Performance avoidance goal
69.00 4.97 3 67.00 6.38 3 68.00 5.80 6
Note. Descriptive statistics of test-taking anxiety based on STAI (Form Y-1) index and debriefings on Wenjuanxing for
four groups of Chinese students aged 16-24.
4.5 Test-Taking Anxiety VS Goal of the
Test
Through the comparison of research data, the analysis
revealed that the mastery approach goal would cause
the lowest test-taking anxiety among Chinese
students (N
total
= 15, M
total
= 35.20, SD
total
= 7.43),
while the performance-avoidance goal would lead to
their highest test-taking anxiety (N
total
= 6, M
total
=
68.00, SD
total
= 5.80) (see Table 3).
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
-40-30-20-100 102030405060
Test Scores
Test-Taking Anxiety
The Analysis of Test-Taking Anxiety from a Sample of Chinese Students Studying Abroad and in the Home Country
399
4.6 Anxiety Disorder
At the beginning of the experiment (without knowing
there would be a test), 18 out of 44 (40.9%) Chinese
students obtained the anxiety scores from STAI
(Form Y-1) above the cut-off point of
psychopathology, which meant they could be
diagnosed with anxiety disorder in a clinical setting
(Womble et al., 2021). Yet just before taking the test
(after knowing there would be a test), 6 more Chinese
students (N = 24 in total, 54.5%) were tested above
the cut-off point of psychopathology.
5 DISCUSSION
Given the results, our first and fourth hypotheses were
validated whereas the second and third were falsified.
More specifically, the first hypothesis was partly
validated: the only found difference was that Chinese
students studying abroad had less test-taking anxiety
than those studying in their home country China. Test
forms, however, had no relationship with test-taking
anxiety and no interaction with countries for study.
We assume one reason is our participants were
somewhat immune to physical rewards because of
their high socioeconomic status (Chen, & Hou, 2014).
Another reason could be they were motivated by the
physical rewards, contrary to our supposition, to
compete more passionately, lowering test-taking
anxiety (Chen, & Hou, 2014). The result might have
been altered if we either enhanced the price of
physical rewards or converted the form of rewards to
the psychological.
There was no relationship between gender and
test-taking anxiety and no difference in test scores
between Chinese students studying abroad in the
United States and in the home country when taking a
test with or without rewards. Our findings were
inconsistent with any other previous research
findings. One possible reason is the size of our sample
was not sufficiently large.
The relationship between test-taking anxiety and
test scores was largely negative, which was consistent
with other research findings and perfectly verified our
fourth hypothesis.
Our result also confirmed the findings of
Linnenbrink and Pintrich (2000): mastery approach
goal will cause the lowest level of anxiety, mastery
avoidance goal and performance-approach goal will
induce moderate anxiety level, while performance-
avoidance goal will lead to the highest level of
anxiety among Chinese students.
When Chinese
students studying abroad in the United States and
studying in their home country faced exams, all four
types of goals were involved. Among them, the
number of students with the type of mastery approach
goal was the largest. The fewest number of students
had the performance-avoidance goal. This was not
consistent with the result of the previous study on
Chinese high school students, (Cui et al., 2008) but
similar to that of a previous investigation of Chinese
college students (Wang, 2013). The reason might be
the test in the experiment did not have a significant
impact on student's personal development. Students
thus would not focus too much on test scores and not
worry about negative results and evaluations and
would turn to improve their abilities instead.
Further, the result revealed that in any case from
the contemporary goal classification theory, the test-
taking anxiety of Chinese students studying abroad in
the United States was slightly lower than that of those
studying in the home country, and in the case of
performance-approach goals, the test-taking anxiety
of Chinese students who studied abroad to the United
States (M = 45.25) was significantly lower than that
of those who studied in the home country (M =
58.00). It was assumed that when both two cohorts
wanted to be positively evaluated by the outside
world because American culture emphasizes the
cultivation of self-confidence more than Chinese
culture, Chinese students who studied abroad in the
United States could be more likely to draw upon self-
confidence to cope with test-taking anxiety.
In previous studies, mastery avoidance goals and
performance-approach goals were associated with
moderate anxiety. However, these studies had not
indicated the specific difference between the levels of
anxiety arising from the two goals. Following our
experiment, the difference between the anxiety levels
triggered by these two goals was offered the anxiety
caused by performance-approach goal was higher
than that caused by mastery avoidance goal. This
point was rarely mentioned in previous studies. The
result of our experiment could fill the gap in previous
studies.
Not surprisingly, the number of students probably
having anxiety disorder after the awareness of the
existence of a test was more than before. However,
the proportion of students diagnosed with anxiety
disorder (40.9% before and 54.5% after), according
to the scale of anxiety scores from STAI Form Y-1
(Spielberger et al., 1983) and the cut-off point of
psychopathology as 40 (Womble et al., 2021), was
much higher than our expectations and previous
research findings. Our explanation for it is that the
online setting and COVID-19 pandemic complicates
NMDME 2022 - The International Conference on New Media Development and Modernized Education
400
their mental state, which would be mentioned in the
following section.
6 LIMITATIONS
There are some limitations worth taken into
consideration when interpreting our results. First, the
online setting might affect test-taking anxiety levels
of certain students. Test-taking anxiety is not
uncommon among students in online exam
environment (Huang, 2014). Online learning and tests
could bring students visual fatigue, mental
depression, loneliness, and many other negative
experiences (Li, & Fu, 2013). Stowell and Bennett
(2010) found that experiencing less anxiety in one
form of test entails higher anxiety in another form of
test; in other words, those who feel comfortable in
classroom settings would feel anxious when taking
online tests and the inverse is also correct. The online
setting could be one factor elevating test-taking
anxiety of some students to the overestimated value,
otherwise it lowered test-taking anxiety of several
students who were in favor of online tests.
In the midst of the period of pandemic COVID-
19, the score of acute psychological stress among
international students were higher than general
population. Because international students had to
undergo the inconvenience of living in isolation,
away from family and home, cultural differences,
academic delays, and visa issues, their anxiety levels
were elevated above normal amid the period of
COVID-19 (Zhao, 2022). Despite these troubles, the
cohort in our experiment – Chinese students studying
abroad to the United States experienced
significantly less test-taking anxiety than Chinese
students studying in the home country China.
Whether COVID-19 was involved in the result or not
was unknown and further studies should be taken into
account.
Also, two years as a threshold to distinguish
Chinese students studying abroad to the United States
from those studying in the home country China might
be arbitrary. Whether adjusting the threshold would
have changed the result remains unclear. Lastly, as
said, our sample size was not large enough, so some
of our results might be affected.
7 CONCLUSIONS
Concluded that Chinese students studying abroad had
less test-taking anxiety than those studying in the
home country China; test forms (test with or without
rewards) had no relationship with test-taking anxiety
and no interaction with countries for study; there was
no relationship between gender and test-taking
anxiety; there was no difference of test scores
between Chinese students studying abroad to the
United States and in the home country when taking a
test with or without rewards; the relationship between
test-taking anxiety and test scores was largely
negative. Moreover, we also found that in any case
from the contemporary goal classification theory, the
test-taking anxiety of Chinese students studying
abroad in the United States was slightly lower than
that of those studying in the home country, and in the
case of performance-approach goals, the test-taking
anxiety of Chinese students who studied abroad to the
United States was significantly lower than that of
those who studied in the home country.
By our experiment in the video-conferencing
form, anxiety probably caused by COVID-19,
relatively small sample size, and arbitrary selection of
participants, our results might be affected. Therefore,
further studies that prudently consider these
conditions are highly needed and we would do some
of them in the future.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We are thankful to the precious advice given by
Hanna, a Chinese teacher, when we formulated ideas
of this research and the commitment of Ge Gao, a
Sophomore at Syracuse University, to the data
collection. We appreciate their contributions to the
research.
REFERENCES
Barrows, J., Dunn, S., & Lloyd, C. A. (2013). Anxiety, self-
efficacy, and college exam grades. Universal Journal of
Educational Research, 1(3), 204-208.
Cassady, J. C., & Johnson, R. E. (2002). Cognitive test
anxiety and academic performance. Contemporary
Educational Psychology. 27(2), 270–295.
Chapell, M. S., Blanding, Z. B., Silverstein, M. E.,
Takahashi, M., Newman, B., Gubi, A., & McCann, N.
(2005). Test anxiety and academic performance in
undergraduate and graduate students. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 97(2), 268-274.
Chen, P., & Hou, C. (2014). Simple analysis of the
complementary motivations induced by physical and
spiritual rewards among university students. Journal of
Yangtze University (Social Sciences), 37(12), 153-155.
The Analysis of Test-Taking Anxiety from a Sample of Chinese Students Studying Abroad and in the Home Country
401
Cui, N., Liu, Y., & Gao, B. (2008). The relationship
between achievement goal, examining anxiety and
performance in senior students. China Journal of Health
Psychology, 16(4), 436-438.
Elliot, A. J. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation
and achievement goals. Educational Psychologist,
34(3), 169-189.
Gharib, A., Phillips, W., & Mathew, N. (2012). Cheat sheet
or open-book? a comparison of the effects of exam
types on performance, retention, and anxiety.
Psychology Research, 2(8), 469-478.
Gu, W., Gong, X., & Zhang, W. (2009). A survey of
depression and anxiety among high school students.
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, (6), 415-416.
Huang, G. (2003). Gender differences in test anxiety of
College Entrance Examination students. Health
Psychology Journal, 11(2), 98-99.
Huang, X. (2014). An empirical study of English computer-
based test anxiety in the network environment. Journal
of Kaifeng Institute of Education, 34(6), 77-79.
Huang, X., Huang, H., Xing, Li., Sanche, K., & Ye, R.
(2005). What do middle school students fear for lower
grades on important examinations: Comparisons of the
United States and China. Journal of Nantong University
(Education Sciences Edition), 21(1), 37-41.
Li, X., & Liu, H. (1999). Study on gender differences in test
anxiety of medical college students. Chinese Journal of
School Health, 21(3), 192-193.
Li, Y., & Fu, G. (2013). Research on factors influencing
college students' online learning anxiety. E-Education
Research, 5, 31-38.
Linnennbrink, E. A., & Pintrich, P. R. (2000). Multiple
pathways to learning and achievement: The role of goal
orientation in fostering adaptive motivation, affect, and
cognition. Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: The
search for optimal motivation and performance (pp.
195-227). Academic Press.
Pintrich, P. R. (2000). An achievement goal theory
perspective on issues in motivation terminology theory
and research. Contemporary Educational Psychology,
25, 92-104.
Posselt, J. R., & Lipson, S. K. (2016). Competition, anxiety,
and depression in the college classroom: Variations by
student identity and field of study. Journal of College
Student Development, 57(8), 973-989.
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P.
R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait
Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
Psychologists Press.
Stowell, J. R., & Bennett, D. (2010). Effects of online
testing on student exam performance and test anxiety.
J. Educational Computing Research, 42(2), 161-171.
Tang, J. (2012). A comparative study of foreign language
learning anxiety between Chinese and American
college students. English Teachers, 2, 31-36.
Wang, X., Wang, X., & Ma, H. (1999). Handbook of mental
health assessment scales (Enlarged ed., pp. 205-209).
Chinese Mental Health Magazine Press.
Wang, Y. (2013). The Relationship among college
students’ achievement goal, academic self-efficacy and
test anxiety [Unpublished master’s thesis]. Harbin
Engineering University.
Wang, Y., Lu, X., Chen, S., & Xia, W. (2005). Gender
differences in test anxiety among middle school
students. Chinese Journal of Clinical Rehabilitation,
9(40), 24-26.
Womble, M., Jennings, S., Schatz, P., & Elbin, R. J. (2021).
A-173 clinical cutoffs on the State-Trait Anxiety
Inventory for concussion. Archives of Clinical
Neuropsychology, 36(6), 1228.
Xu, Q., Mao, Z., Wei, D., Liu, P., Fan, K., Wang, J., Wang,
X., Lou, X., Lin, H., Wang, C., & Wu, C. (2021).
Prevalence and risk factors for anxiety symptoms
during the outbreak of covid-19: A large survey among
373216 junior and senior high school students in China.
Journal of Affective Disorders, 288(1), 17-22.
Zeidner, M. (1998). Test anxiety: The state of the art. New
York: Plenum Press.
Zhao, Q. (2022). A Study on acute psychological stress
reaction and coping style of Chinese overseas students
during the COVID-19 pandemic. Chinese Journal of
Frontier Health and Quarantine.
NMDME 2022 - The International Conference on New Media Development and Modernized Education
402