The Influence of Gender on Students' Perception of Risk in Portugal
Bruno Martins
1
a
and Adélia Nunes
2
b
1
Department of Geography and Tourism, CEGOT (Centre of Studies on Geography and Spatial Planning),
University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal
2
Department of Geography and Tourism, CEGOT (Centre of Studies on Geography and Spatial Planning),
University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal
Keywords: Risk Perception; Students; Gender; Education; Portugal.
Abstract: This study analyses the perception that students at the end of the 3rd cycle, in Portugal, have of a set of natural
and environmental risks, considering their manifestation both nationally and in the area of residence. It also
sets out to understand how students perceive the risks, taking into account the causes, the future trend, and
support from public authorities, as well as the willingness to change attitudes regarding risk mitigation and
reduction. The results suggest that students have a relatively low-moderate perception of risk. The risks of
forest fires, heat waves, air and water pollution, and flooding are the ones they single out as most likely to
occur, mainly as a consequence of climate change. Gender proved to be an important variable in perception,
particularly in terms of manifestation and personal perception of risk. These results can influence the strategies
and resources to be applied in the educational context, so that there is less reason to educate the youngest
children about the need to prevent risk, and to reduce the impact of disasters and strengthen the resilience of
the community in general.
1 INTRODUCTION
Understanding how the general public perceive
natural and environmental risks is crucial for a better
definition of communication and information
strategies on risks. However, it can lead to more
efficient risk management strategies, which will
somehow contribute to societies that are better able to
respond in crisis situations and that have greater
social resilience.
Research on risk perception depends on various
local/geographic and personal factors, including: the
location of the individual (Bera & Danek, 2018);
housing characteristics (Hung, 2009; Thistlethwaite
et al, 2018); the consequences of the risk
manifestation (Stojanov et al.; 2015); the impacts of
the crisis (Thistlethwaite et al, 2018); the socio-
economic and demographic profile (such as age,
education, gender, income) (Balog-Way et al.., 2020);
direct experience (Terpstra, 2009; Bera & Danek,
2018); race (Macias, 2016); the historical-cultural
context (Armas et al., 2015); and the political and
a
https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8681-2349
b
https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8665-4459
religious context (Bichard & Kazmierczak, 2012). Of
the multiple variables that can influence risk
perception, the gender difference has been described
in several studies as a relevant factor, noting that
women have higher levels of risk perception and
show greater concern than men (Lindell & Hwang,
2008; Poortinga et al. 2011; Martins et al., 2019).
However, other studies (Bradford et al., 2012) have
not reported a robust correlation between risk
perception and gender.
Education, particularly school, does seem to play
a very important role in risk reduction, however. In
general, individuals with a higher level of education
tend to develop more accurate levels of risk
perception, generally adopting more effective
preventive behaviours towards risk (Striessnig et al.,
2013; Muttarak & Lutz, 2014).
Based on the application of questionnaires to
students living in several regions of Portugal, we set
out to assess how students at the end of the 3rd cycle
of basic education, perceive: (i) the probability of
manifestation of natural and environmental risks,
taking into account a number of natural and
Martins, B. and Nunes, A.
The Influence of Gender on Studentsâ
˘
A
´
Z Perception of Risk in Portugal.
DOI: 10.5220/0011921500003536
In Proceedings of the 3rd International Symposium on Water, Ecology and Environment (ISWEE 2022), pages 111-116
ISBN: 978-989-758-639-2; ISSN: 2975-9439
Copyright
c
2023 by SCITEPRESS – Science and Technology Publications, Lda. Under CC license (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0)
111
environmental hazards affecting the national territory
and their area of residence; (ii) the triggering factors;
(iii) the support of public authorities in the event of
crisis; (iv) the future trends regarding their
manifestation; (v) and the willingness to change their
attitude regarding the mitigation and reduction of the
respective impacts. The issue as to whether students
of different gender have different perceptions on the
previous questions is also examined.
2 MATERIALS AND METHODS
2.1 Background to the Study Area
Mainland Portugal is located in southwestern Europe,
between latitude 36º57' N and 42º9' N, and longitude
12' W and 30' W. It occupies approximately
92,212 km
2
and has 10.3 million inhabitants. The
insular part corresponds to the archipelagos of
Madeira and Azores, located in the Atlantic Ocean.
Several risks affect the mainland territory. In the
North and Centre regions of Portugal the risk of forest
fires is dominant, less so in the south, with the
exception of the Algarve mountains. In the south,
drought and desertification are the most important
risks, especially in eastern Alentejo. The risk of
flooding is greater in the lower, estuarine, courses of
the main Portuguese rivers, especially in the North
and Centre regions, as is the case of the Douro, Vouga
and Mondego rivers, and, further south, the Tagus.
2.2 Questionnaire and Statistical
Analysis
In order to assess the perception that end-of-school
students have regarding a set of natural and
environmental risks, a survey questionnaire was
applied to 376 students living in mainland Portugal.
The average age of the respondents is 15 years old;
about 47% were female and 53% male.
The questionnaire is structured in six parts. In the
first part ‘respondents are characterised’ and in the
second part students are asked to ‘Rank the risks
according to the probability of their occurrence’, at
national and municipal level. Fifteen natural and
environmental risks were listed and a qualitative
Likert scale was used to rank them, ranging from 1 -
nil/minimum; 2 - low; 3 - moderate; 4 - high; 5 -
maximum. The lowest value (nil or minimum) is
therefore linked to a very low risk perception in
relation to the probability of occurrence of the risk, as
opposed to the highest value (maximum), correlated
with a very high probability of manifestation.
The third part of the questionnaire considers
questions aimed at analysing the respondents’
perception of risk, such as: (i) whether risks tend to
increase in the future; (ii) whether they arouse fear;
(iii) whether individual actions influence risk; (iv)
whether they are concerned about the consequences
of risks; (v) whether they are willing to change
individual behaviour. The fourth part is aimed at
analysing the understanding of causes. This part of
the questionnaire considered: (i) whether risks result
from anthropic action; (ii) whether they result from
climate change; (iii) whether they result from poor
planning and land use planning; (iv) whether they
result from divine punishment; (v) whether they are
unpredictable natural events. The fifth part,
‘Channels of information on risk’, was intended to
examine the means of communication considered
most effective in communicating and informing about
risks, considering the role of education, the media and
the Internet. Finally, the sixth part of the
questionnaire looked at students' perceptions of state
support in the event of a crisis.
To assess whether there are statistically
significant differences between female and male
students in the different components of risk
perception, the independent t-test was used to
compare the mean difference between genders.
3 RESULTS
In general, the perception of the risks considered
according to their manifestation is low to moderate,
mainly at the local scale (fig. 1).
Figure 1: Distribution of the relative frequencies (%) by
class, according to the occurrence of different risks at
national level (A) and Madeira (B).
ISWEE 2022 - International Symposium on Water, Ecology and Environment
112
The risks with the highest perception values are
forest fires (average 3.87) and heat waves (average
3.12). These are followed by water pollution (average
3.08), air pollution (average 3.06) and coastal erosion
(average 2.72). The lowest values of risk perception
were the geophysical risks, in particular the risk of
volcanism (average 1.63) and tsunami (average 1.85),
based on the national scale.
With regard to the area of residence, the risks with
the highest perception are forest fires (average 2.77),
water pollution (average 2.53), cold spells (average
2.46), and floods (average 2.41). With lower
perception we find the volcanic risk (average 1.18),
tsunamis (average 1.32), storms (average 1.46), and
desertification (average 1.70).
A
B
Figure 2: Overall risk rating (mean) for females and males
(A National level, B Municipal) (Likert scale: 1 -
nil/minimum; 2 - low; 3 - moderate; 4 - high; 5 - maximum).
The results indicate statistically significant
differences (Levene’s test for equality of variances/ t-
test for equality of means) in the perception of risks,
taking gender into account. This fact is seen more
clearly at the local scale, and for the risks of flooding
(sig. 0.005), water (sig. 0.000) and air pollution (sig.
0.000), desertification (sig. 0.009), and soil
degradation (sig. 0.009). Female students have a
higher perception of risks than male students,
particularly when it comes to the risk of air and water
pollution, floods, forest fires and coastal erosion.
(Fig. 2).
Most students understand that the considered
risks, especially forest fires, tsunamis, storms and
earthquakes, can cause material and human losses.
Similarly, they consider that the occurrence of the
risks will tend to increase in the future, especially the
risks of air and water pollution, forest fires, and heat
waves.
The results also very clearly suggest the
importance of anthropic action as a risk amplifying
factor, especially regarding the risk of water (mean
4.16; n=162) and air pollution (mean 4.18), and the
risk of forest fire (mean 4.04) (fig. 3).
Figure 3: Comparing perceptions on likelihood and
anthropic action as a risk-increasing factor.
Climate change is seen as a cause of increased risk of
heat waves (mean 4.09), droughts (mean 4.02), cold
spells (mean 3.83) and forest fires (mean 3.76). Once
again, gender establishes statistically significant
correlations with the fear of risks (sig. 0.000), with
individual actions as a risk-influencing factor (sig.
0.004), with the concern about risks (sig. 0.000), and
with behavioural change (0.003) towards risk
mitigation (Table 1). It is the female students who are
more concerned about the consequences of the
manifestation of risks; they are more afraid and
believe that individual action contributes
significantly to risk reduction. They also tend to
ascribe the causes of an increase in the occurrence of
disasters to poor land use planning policies and are
The Influence of Gender on Studentsâ
˘
A
´
Z Perception of Risk in Portugal
113
more willing to change their behaviour in order to
moderate the impacts of risk manifestation.
Table 1: Levene’s and t-test for risk perception.
Note: Asterisk, significant relationships between groups (p-value <
0.05); n= 376.
Regarding channels of communication and
information on risks, the students surveyed believe
that the Internet and school are effective channels of
communication. Most students assigned a very
important role to education and the media as vectors
of communication (mean 4.19) and information
(mean 4.07) about risks. However, the Internet is the
most effective vehicle in the process of
communication and information about risks (average
4.07).
In crisis situations, students feel supported by
government bodies.
4 DISCUSSION
School education is very important in risk perception,
serving not only to increase knowledge of the
different potentially dangerous physical processes,
but also to raise awareness of practices aimed at
improving safety. In this study, the students'
perception of the risks considered is low to moderate,
both at national level and, more especially, in the
municipalities where respondents live. These results
corroborate some works that suggest a lower
perception of risk in the municipalities of residence
than at the national scale (Martins et al., 2019; Nunes
& Martins, 2019). This perception seems to be related
to direct experience with crisis situations (Wachinger
et al., 2013) or even to result from the influence of the
media. In fact, the media have proven to be an
important vehicle of information by influencing the
perception very strongly (Biernacki et al., 2009).
News of disasters elsewhere thus seems to influence
perceptions, suggesting that risk events tend to occur
outside the area of residence. However, the greater
attention given to a certain risk is responsible for
creating an indirect experience that influences
perception and that can, in some way, explain the
valuation of a certain risk (in the Portuguese instance,
forest fires) compared to other risks (Siegrist &
Gutscher, 2005).
In general, risks are perceived as potentially
dangerous because they can lead to loss of life and
damage to property. Forest fires and tsunamis are
prominent in this group. Disasters are also seen as
phenomena that will tend to increase in the future,
especially those related to water and air pollution, and
meteorological risks, such as heat waves and cold
spells. In fact, there are several works that point to
atmospheric risks as having the highest perceived
tendency to increase in the future (Garschagen et al.
2020), especially in relation to climate change and
extreme hydrometeorological phenomena (Eck et al.,
2020), and pollution (Altunoğlu et al., 2017).
The results suggest statistically significant
differences with respect to gender. Female students
showed a higher perception than male students,
especially regarding the risk of air and water
pollution, soil degradation and coastal erosion risks,
both at national level and in the municipality where
they lived. These results are in line with some works
that identify gender as a factor in risk perception
(Lindell & Hwang, 2008; Poortinga et al. 2011).
Female students tend to value the consequences of
disasters, too, raising more fear in them. This result is
similar to other studies that suggest that women feel
more fear and concern about the consequences of risk
manifestation than men (Lujala et al., 2015). They
also tend to give greater consideration to their actions
in influencing risk, and are more willing to change
their behaviours to lessen the likelihood that
catastrophic events might occur.
Regarding causes, climate change stands out as
the most important factor in increasing risk. The
strong media coverage of the topic and its relationship
with some risks, particularly those associated with
hydroclimatic extremes, could justify this perception,
in line with several studies that indicate identical
results (Wamsler et al., 2012; Muttarak & Lutz,
2014).
Although education, especially the role of school,
is seen as an important means of information and
communication vis-à-vis knowledge of the physical
phenomena associated with the different risks, social
communication is also emphasized, thus validating
some works that reach the same conclusions
(Biernacki et al., 2009). However, the internet is the
most effective form of knowledge (Roth &
Brönnimann, 2013). Nevertheless, although the
internet contributes very effectively to a wider
Levene's
test for equality
of variances
t-test for equality
of means
Sig. Sig. (2extremities)
I'm worried about the risks* 0.130 0.000
Willing to change* 0.667 0.000
Divine punishment 0.103 0.186
Nature's revenge 0.104 0.178
Planning Policies 0.868 0.177
My actions can lessen the risk* 0.465 0.006
ISWEE 2022 - International Symposium on Water, Ecology and Environment
114
dissemination of information, this does not mean that
it contributes to greater technical knowledge
(Krimsky, 2007).
The results also suggest that the causes of
occurrence, and the severity of the risks are related to
poor land use planning policies. They also suggest
that individual actions influence the risk, often
aggravating it, and claim that female students are
especially willing to change behaviours so as to
mitigate risks. Several studies (Zaalberg et al., 2009;
Terpstra, 2011) have similarly demonstrated a
positive relationship between emotional elements
such as fear or worry and willingness to implement
measures aimed at mitigating the impacts of risk
occurrence.
5 CONCLUSIONS
Risk perception is inherently personal and subjective,
and results from a combination of knowledge and
judgment associated with social, psychological,
cultural and political factors. Given this multiplicity
of factors, the analysis in this study is somewhat
limited and focuses mainly on gender and the socio-
cultural background of the students surveyed.
Gender, indeed, proved to be a variable with
significant influence on perception, particularly in
terms of personal risk perception. Female students
were more concerned about risks, as they are more
afraid because they think that risks will be more
frequent in the future and have increasingly
significant impacts. Female students also have a
higher perception of risk depending on its
manifestation. However, further work is needed in
order to consolidate this conclusion by including
more variables.
In fact, in Portugal the subject ‘Risks and
Disasters’ has only recently been introduced in the
school curriculum. Therefore, there are still no studies
on the contribution of this content to the perception of
risk by students. Thus, it is essential to carry out
studies, with a larger sample and over a longer period
of time, aiming at acquiring sounder knowledge about
how education, and school in particular, influences
students' risk perception. It will then be possible to
benefit equally from a more correct approach to
teaching methods and from the quality of educational
materials and resources used in the teaching-learning
process, thereby deepening knowledge and raising
students' awareness.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This research received support from the Centre of
Studies in Geography and Spatial Planning
(CEGOT), funded by national funds through the
Foundation for Science and Technology (FCT) under
the reference UIDB/04084/2020.
REFERENCES
Altunoğlu, B., Atav, E., Sönmez, S., 2017. The
Investigation of Environmental Risk Perception and
Attitudes Towards the Environment in Secondary
School Students, The Turkish Online Journal of
Educational Technology, pp. 446-477.
Armas, I., Ionescu, R., Posner, C.N., 2015. Flood risk
perception along the Lower Danube river, Romania.
Nat Hazards (79), 1913-1931.
Balog-Way, D., McComas, K. and Besley, J., 2020. The
Evolving Field of Risk Communication. Risk Analysis
(40), 2240-2262. https://doi.org/10.1111/risa.13615
Bera, M. and Danek, P., 2018. The perception of risk in the
flood-prone area: a case study from the Czech
municipality. Disaster Prev Manag 27, (1), 2-14.
Bichard, E. and Kazmierczak, A., 2012. Are homeowners
willing to adapt to and mitigate the effects of climate
change? Clim Chang (112), 633-654.
Biernacki, W., Działek, J., Janas, K., Padło, T., 2008.
Community attitudes towards extreme phenomena
relative to place of residence and previous experience.
In Liszewski S (ed). The influence of extreme
phenomena on the natural environment and human
living conditions (pp. 207-237).
Bradford, R., O'Sullivan, J., van der Craats, I., Krywkow,
J.; Rotko, P., Aaltonen, J., Bonaiuto, M., De Dominicis,
S., Waylen, K., Schelfaut, K. 2012. Risk perception-
Issues for flood management in Europe. Nat. Hazards
Earth Syst. Sci., 12, 2299-2309. doi:10.5194/nhess-12-
2299-2012.
Eck, Christel & Mulder, Bob & van der Linden, Sander
2020. Climate Change Risk Perceptions of Audiences
in the Climate Change Blogosphere. Sustainability. 12.
7990. 10.3390/su12197990.
Garschagen, M., Wood, S. L. R., Garard, J., Ivanova, M., &
Luers, A., 2020. Too big to ignore: Global risk
perception gaps between scientists and business
leaders. Earth's Future, 8, e2020EF001498.
https://doi.org/10.1029/2020EF001498
Hung, H., 2009. The attitude towards flood insurance
purchase when respondents' preferences are uncertain:
a fuzzy approach. J Risk Res 12(2):239-258
Krimsky, S. 2007. Risk communication in the internet age:
The rise of disorganized skepticism, Volume 7, Issue 2,
p.157-164.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envhaz.2007.05.006
Lindell, K., Hwang, S., 2008. 'Households' perceived
personal risk and responses in a multi-hazard
The Influence of Gender on Studentsâ
˘
A
´
Z Perception of Risk in Portugal
115
environment', Risk Anal. 28 (2), 539-556,
https://doi.org/ 10.1111/j.1539-6924.2008.01032.x.
Lujala, P., Lein, H., Rød, J., 2015. Climate change, natural
hazards, and risk perception: the role of proximity and
personal experience, Local Environ. 20 (4), 489-509,
https://doi.org/10.1080/13549839.2014.887666.
Macias, T., 2016. Environmental risk perception among
race and ethnic groups in the United States. Ethnicities,
Vol. 16 (1) 111-129,
https://doi.org/10.1177/1468796815575382
Martins, B., Nunes, A., Lourenço, L., Castro, F., 2019.
Flash Flood Risk Perception by the Population of
Mindelo, S. Vicente (Cape Verde), Water, Volume 11,
Issue 9, p.15. https://doi.org/10.3390/w11091895
Muttarak, R. and Lutz, W., 2014. Is education a key to
reducing vulnerability to natural disasters and hence
unavoidable climate change? Ecology and Society
19(1): 42. http://dx.doi.org/10.5751/ES-06476-190142
Nunes, A. & Martins, B. 2019. Exploring the spatial
perception of risk in Portugal by students of Geography,
Journal of Geography, vol.119, Issue 5
https://doi.org/10.1080/00221341.2020.1801803
Poortinga, W., Spence, A., Whitmarsh, L., Capstick, S.,
Nick F. Pidgeon,N. F., 2011. Uncertain climate: An
investigation into public scepticism about
anthropogenic climate change, Global Environmental
Change, 21(3), 1015-1024,
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2011.03.001.
Roth, Florian and Brönnimann, Gabriel, 2013. Using the
Internet for Public Risk Communication. Risk and
Resilience Reports.
Siegrist, M., Gutscher, H., Earle, T. C., 2005. Perception of
risk: The influence of general trust, and general
confidence. Journal of Risk Research, 8(2), 145-156.
https://doi.org/10.1080/1366987032000105315
Stojanov, R., Duz, B., Danek, T., Nemec, D., Prochàzka,
D., 2015. Adaptation to the impacts of climate extremes
in central Europe: a case study in a rural area in the
Czech Republic. Sustain 7(9):12758-12786
Striessnig, E., W. Lutz, and Patt, A., 2013. Effects of
educational attainment on climate risk vulnerability.
Ecology and Society 18(1): 16.
http://dx.doi.org/10.5751/ES-05252-180116
Terpstra, T., 2009. Flood preparedness: thoughts, feelings
and intentions of the Dutch public. Thesis, University
of Twente, Twente.
Thistlethwaite, J., Henstra, D., Brown, C., Scott, D., 2018.
How flood experience and risk perception influences
protective actions and behaviours among Canadian
homeowners. Environ Manag (61),197-208
Wachinger, G., Renn, O., Begg, C., Kuhlicke, C. 2013. The
Risk Perception Paradox - Implications for Governance
and Communication of Natural Hazards. Risk Analysis,
(33), 1049-1065.
Wamsler, C., Brink, E., Rentala, O., 2012. Climate change,
adaptation, and formal education: the role of schooling
for increasing societies' adaptive capacities in El
Salvador and Bazil. Ecology and Society 17(2):
2.http://dx.doi.org/10.5751/ES-04645-170202
Zaalberg, R., Midden, C., Meijnders, A., McCalley, T.,
2009. Prevention, adaptation, and threat denial:
flooding experiences in the Netherlands. Risk Anal
(29),1759-1778
ISWEE 2022 - International Symposium on Water, Ecology and Environment
116