(EOGh: U = 11754, p=.32, r=-.12; EOGv: U= 2486,
p=<.001, r=.76). Vertical, but not horizontal,
saccades were much faster during gaze aversion than
visual saccades. The velocity did not differ
significantly between the two memory conditions
(EOGh: U = 218, p=.04, r=.34; EOGv: U =405,
p=.17, r=-.23).
4 DISCUSSION
From these preliminary results, it seems that the EOG
signal associated with eye saccades initiating a gaze
aversion during attentional switch differs from the
EOG signal associated with visual eye saccades that
people do when they explore their environment.
Saccades initiating gaze aversion were faster and had
larger amplitude than visual saccades. As expected,
aversions seemed to be more distinguishable from the
visual saccades on the vertical rather than on the
horizontal EOG.
However, the present study is far from real-time
detection of attentional switches. We are facing many
limits. First, we show that even on a small sample of
participants, there is huge individual variability in the
gaze aversion behaviour. The ERP analysis shows
that the pattern of the aversion differs between
participants. To be applicable, an individual personal
calibration of the system would be necessary. Here,
we used a small sample size because we aimed to
determine the potential interest of the method, but a
validation of the method would require a higher
sample. Second, to refine the results, future studies
should include recordings of head movements and a
better calibration to infer gaze angle more accurately.
Here, we show that gaze aversions induce a large gaze
angle. However, the linear relation with EOG is not
true for gaze at high eccentricities (e.g., Hládek et al.,
2018). Interestingly however, the results for gaze
aversion during autobiographical memory do not
differ significantly from the one during semantic
memory. It seems therefore linked to internal
attention in general, and maybe not specific to
autobiographical memory.
Despite these limits, we want to emphasize the
importance of studying such behaviour in
aeronautics. Although here we have focused on gaze
aversion occurring during memory retrieval for the
sake of developing an experimental paradigm, in our
view such behaviour is similar to what occurs during
mind-wandering and in fact any behaviour requiring
access to internal thoughts including when one is
speaking to an interlocutor. Monotonous tasks
generate higher rates of mind-wandering, which is a
problem given the increasing automation in the
cockpits (Gouraud et al., 2018). Therefore, we
urgently need an objective marker allowing real-time
detection of internal thoughts switching to monitor
attention in critical situations. In this context, our
work aims to open discussions and perspectives.
5 CONCLUSION
Although our results are preliminary, they are
encouraging. First, we propose a protocol to trigger
attentional switches in the lab. Second, we show that
these switches are associated with gaze aversions.
Third, since we observe that gaze aversion have
different EOG features compared to visual saccades,
we think that EOG could be a potential method to
study and detect attentional switch. In an early
laboratory phase, EOG could be coupled with
augmented reality helmets to characterize gaze
aversion better before reaching a reliable detection
using EOG only.
REFERENCES
Benedek, M., Stoiser, R., Walcher, S., & Körner, C. (2017).
Eye behavior associated with internally versus
externally directed cognition. Frontiers in Psychology,
8, 1092. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01092
Casner, S. M., & Schooler, J. W. (2014). Thoughts in flight:
Automation use and pilots
’task-related and task-
unrelated thought. Human Factors, 56(3), 433–442.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0018720813501550
Conway, M. A. (2001). Sensory-perceptual episodic
memory and its context: Autobiographical memory.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of
London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 356(1413),
1375–1384. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2001.0940
Doherty-Sneddon, G., & Phelps, F. G. (2005). Gaze
aversion: A response to cognitive or social difficulty?
Memory and Cognition, 33(4), 727–733.
https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03195338
Dorronsoro, G., Álvarez, J. R. & Soto, J. M. (2019).
"Nonlinear analysis of electrooculogram signals using
higher-order statistics." IEEE Transactions on
Biomedical Engineering, 47, 5, 674-680, 2000.
https://doi.org/10.1109/RBME.2019.2951328
Favre-Félix, A., Graversen, C., Dau, T., & Lunner, T.
(2017). Real-time estimation of eye gaze by in-ear
electrodes. 39th Annual International Conference of the
IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society
(EMBC), 2017, 4086–4089. https://doi.org/10.1109/
EMBC.2017.8037754
Fernandes, M. A., & Moscovitch, M. (2000). Divided
attention and memory: Evidence of substantial