Can We Use EOG to Identify When Attention Switches Away from
the Outside World to Focus on Our Mental Thoughts?
Anaïs Servais
1,2 a
, Raphaël Poveda
1
, Hanna Gerony
1
, Emmanuel J. Barbeau
1b
and Christophe Hurter
2c
1
Centre de Recherche Cerveau et Cognition (CerCo), CHU Purpan, Toulouse, France
2
École Nationale d’Aviation Civile (ENAC), Toulouse, France
Keywords: Attentional Switch, Mind-Wandering, Eye Movements, Electro-Oculography, Gaze Aversion.
Abstract: Aviation incidents resulting from attentional failures continue to occur. Attention is a limited resource and
perceptual decoupling occurs when the attention switches away from the outside world to focus on the inner
mental world. This phenomenon dramatically decreases visual perception, but is common and frequent since
human beings spend nearly half of their time immersed in their thoughts, implying numerous switches
between the outside and the inner world each day. We believe that detecting these attentional switches in air
traffic controllers could improve safety. We suggest gaze aversion as a potential behavioural objective marker
and therefore aim to find a method to measure gaze aversion in the lab. Our preliminary study tested EOG
and provided encouraging results since movements of gaze aversion differed significantly from visual
saccades in terms of amplitude and velocity, two characteristics measurable with the EOG signal. Gaze
aversions are faster and related to wider movements. This opens up great perspectives in aviation since the
EOG is a non-invasive method.
1 INTRODUCTION
Observe what’s going on in your mind while
answering the following question: "How did you
celebrate your last birthday?". Answering this
question required the creation of a temporary internal
mental space where your attention was focused
(Tulving, 2002). Thus, there was an attentional
switch from the external world to the internal world
— a change in mental state commonly known as
mind-wandering— a general term popularly used to
designate daydreaming and zoning-out (Smallwood
& Schooler, 2006). Indeed, since attention is a limited
resource, when it is focused on the internal world, the
external world vanishes (Fernandes & Moscovitch,
2000), a phenomenon known as perceptual
decoupling. Though useful for managing attentional
resources, this decoupling can have dramatic
consequences—impairing performance and safety in
operational tasks (such as driving a car or piloting a
plane) where attention must be directed to the
a
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0032-2953
b
https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0836-3538
c
https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4318-6717
external world (Gouraud et al., 2018; Smallwood et
al., 2011). Reports show that aviation accidents
related to attentional lapses occur (NTSB, 2014), but
the origin of these lapses remains unknown. Given
perceptual decoupling, mind-wandering could be a
potential cause but objective, behavioural, markers to
monitor attention remain elusive.
Here, we propose eye movements as one such
potential candidate. Mind-wandering is associated
with oculomotor features reducing visual processing:
more and longer blinks, fewer and longer fixations,
longer saccades durations, and so on (Benedek et al.,
2017). However, these eye behaviours cannot be used
as markers to detect mind-wandering in real-time
because they require posteriori averages. Instead,
here we suggest investigating another eye behaviour
gaze aversion — a very common behaviour while
answering memory questions. People routinely look
away as if they were searching for the answer on the
ceiling or “in the sky” (Doherty-Sneddon & Phelps,
2005; Glenberg et al., 1998).
24
Servais, A., Poveda, R., Gerony, H., Barbeau, E. and Hurter, C.
Can We Use EOG to Identify When Attention Switches Away from the Outside World to Focus on Our Mental Thoughts?.
DOI: 10.5220/0011950500003622
In Proceedings of the 1st International Conference on Cognitive Aircraft Systems (ICCAS 2022), pages 24-29
ISBN: 978-989-758-657-6
Copyright
c
2023 by SCITEPRESS Science and Technology Publications, Lda. Under CC license (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0)
So far, mind-wandering has seldomly been
studied in aeronautics, perhaps because it is a
spontaneous mental state and therefore difficult to
assess. Current methods are based on self-reporting,
which has several biases, including social desirability
for pilots (Casner & Schooler, 2014). To overcome
this problem, since people tend to retrieve events that
occurred in their past during 60% of their zoning out
time (Mildner & Tamir, 2019), we propose to trigger
attentional switches in the lab using questions like the
one you answered, i.e., autobiographical memory
questions (Conway, 2001) while recording eye
movements with electro-oculography (EOG). EOG
records the electrical potential between electrodes
placed on the muscles around the eyes and allows to
quantify vertical and horizontal eye movements. In
airplane cockpits, the EOG may provide a less
invasive alternative to infrared cameras, which
generally obstruct part of the visual field. Recent
studies have also shown that EOG can be recorded
around the ears (Favre-Félix et al., 2017) opening
perspectives for integration of the electrodes in the
pilot's helmet. In this preliminary work, we aim to
estimate the relevance of EOG to study gaze aversion
during internal attention.
2 MATERIALS AND METHODS
2.1 Participants
Data were collected on 4 participants (2 females, aged
between 23 and 28, 1 left-handed) without
oculomotor, visual, neurological, or psychiatric
disorders.
2.2 Experimental Task
We designed a task during which participants
answered autobiographical memory questions. Each
trial started with a fixation cross (5 sec) followed by
an oral question delivered through loudspeakers (5
sec). The questions were generated using
astread.com. After the question, the participant
completed a short visual task to keep attention
towards the external world: for 8 to 12 letters, the
participant pressed a key if the letter contained a
curve line and another if it did not. After the
presentation of the letters, the participant started to
search in autobiographical memory. This “reflection
phase” was divided into an earlier “access” phase
(max 12 sec) and a later “elaboration” phase (5 sec)
during which the participant respectively selected and
explored a personal memory corresponding to a
unique
and short event (< 24 hours), with a defined
Figure 1: This figure illustrates the method used to collect the data. On the left, is the position of the electrodes. On the right,
is the description of the cognitive task.
Can We Use EOG to Identify When Attention Switches Away from the Outside World to Focus on Our Mental Thoughts?
25
spatiotemporal context. An audio “debriefing”
indicated the start of the verbalization. Stimuli
presentation was controlled using OpenSesame 3.2.8
(Mathôt et al., 2012). Then, the patient pressed a key
to start the next trial (see Figure 1 for details).
To isolate the impact of autobiographical
memories, we included two control conditions: a
semantic memory task (also involving internal
attention) concerning famous events or people, and a
visual task (external attention) that required finding
the digit “5” hidden in a visual scene. The whole
experiment was divided into 5 blocks of 15 questions
(5/condition) presented in random order. Each
participant completed 25 trials per cognitive
condition. Explanations about the task were first
provided and then participants had 2 practice trials
per condition (not further analysed).
2.3 EOG Recording
A BioSemi Active Two amplifier was used to record
the EOG with 5 electrodes: a reference on the
forehead, one electrode at each external canthus for
horizontal movement, one electrode above the right
eyebrow, and one electrode on the top of the right
cheek for vertical movements. The frequency rate of
acquisition was 2048 Hz. To guarantee the quality of
the EOG data throughout the session, a calibration
was performed at the beginning of each block. In
order to have a ground truth, we recorded a video of
the participant's face using a SONY FDR-AX33
Handycam, with 1920x1080px resolution, positioned
above the screen.
2.4 Data Analyses
The signal was preprocessed—resampled to 256Hz,
and filtered with a low-pass at 40Hz. All the saccades
were labelled manually by two students. While
labelling the saccades, they were blind concerning the
part of the signal they were processing. Only the
saccades labelled by both judges were kept for
analysis.
The periods of interest were the visual task and
the 2 memory conditions. The visual trials allowed
us to get a sample of natural visual saccades, of the
type usually done while exploring the environment.
During memory retrieval, we focus on the trials
where gaze aversion occurred. With the video as
ground truth, the saccades initiating gaze aversion
were noted as aversion saccades. The
synchronization between the EOG and the video was
performed using BrainStorm (https://neuroimage.
usc.edu/brainstorm/).
Event related potentials (ERPs) were calculated
on epochs aligned on the aversion time (settled to
zero), a baseline of - 300ms before (because 300ms is
the min. time between the start of the memory
retrieval and a gaze aversion), and duration of 600ms
(the min. duration of a gaze aversion). ERPs allow
observing if a similar EOG pattern is repeated
between aversions.
The sensitivity of Eogert (Toivanen, 2015), a
probabilistic online method to detect EOG saccades,
was compared for visual and aversion saccades.
Compared to the saccades labelled manually, false
negatives and true positives were counted:
sensitivity=TP/(TP+FN). For visual saccades, the
sensitivity was 36% against only 9% for the aversions
saccades. The algorithm is more accurate on visual
saccades than aversion saccades, pleading for
different EOG features between these types of eye
movements (see Figure 2 for an illustration of the
signal during a memory trial with gaze aversion and
a visual trial with visual saccades).
Figure 2: Illustration of the EOG signal during A) a memory
trial; B) a visual trial for the same subject.
To investigate the differences between the EOG
features of visual compared to aversion saccades, we
conducted exploratory analyses comparing the gaze
angle and velocity of the two kinds of saccades with
Mann-Whitney U-tests (the size effect is the rank-
biserial correlation (r),
α was corrected with
Bonferroni to .016). For each gaze direction, a
constant of reference was calculated based on the
calibration (in
µV/degree). Gaze angle was calculated
following a linear relation.
Gaze angle =
EOG amplitude (µV)
Constant (µV/degree)
(1)
Velocity =
Gaze angle (degrees)
Duration of the saccade (sec)
(2)
ICCAS 2022 - International Conference on Cognitive Aircraft Systems
26
3 RESULTS
Out of 100, gaze aversion was observed in 35 trials
for autobiographical memory and 26 trials for
semantic memory questions. There was inter-
individual variability in the occurrence of gaze
aversion since the proportion of questions where gaze
aversion was observed varied between participants
from 4% to 68% for autobiographical questions and
from 0% to 56% for semantic questions. A total of
1062 visual saccades were counted during the visual
trials.
The visualization of the ERPs was possible only for 3
subjects because the fourth one did not do enough
gaze aversions. It revealed individual similarity of the
vertical EOG pattern (Figure 3).
The aversion saccades showed different gaze
angle and velocity than visual saccades (Figure 4).
For the gaze angle, Mann-Whitney U-tests showed
significant differences between visual and aversion
saccades. Gaze angles were much larger for
aversions, both in autobiographical (EOGh: U= 7429,
p<.001, r=.48; EOGv: U=3669, p<.001, r=.74) and
semantic memory trials (EOGh: U=6961, p=.007,
r=.33; EOGv: U = 713, p<.001, r=.93). The angle did
not differ significantly between the aversions from
the two memory conditions memory (EOGh: U = 263,
p=.22, r=.20; EOGv: U =424, p=.08, r=-.28). The
velocity of the saccades was also significantly
different between visual saccades and aversion
saccades, which appeared either in autobiographical
memory (EOGh: U= 10957, p=.03, r=.23; EOGv:
U=5901, p<.001, r=.59) or in semantic memory trials
Figure 3: ERPs per participant. Each colour represents the epoch of a trial. Aversions from autobiographical and semantic
trials have been pooled together. Vertical red lines indicate the start of the aversion separating the baseline from the gaze
aversion period. EOGh: EOG signal for horizontal movements; EOGv: EOG signal for vertical movements.
Figure 4: Gaze angle and velocity for visual and aversion saccades. One colour stands for all the saccades of a single
participant. ** p-value < .001.
Can We Use EOG to Identify When Attention Switches Away from the Outside World to Focus on Our Mental Thoughts?
27
(EOGh: U = 11754, p=.32, r=-.12; EOGv: U= 2486,
p=<.001, r=.76). Vertical, but not horizontal,
saccades were much faster during gaze aversion than
visual saccades. The velocity did not differ
significantly between the two memory conditions
(EOGh: U = 218, p=.04, r=.34; EOGv: U =405,
p=.17, r=-.23).
4 DISCUSSION
From these preliminary results, it seems that the EOG
signal associated with eye saccades initiating a gaze
aversion during attentional switch differs from the
EOG signal associated with visual eye saccades that
people do when they explore their environment.
Saccades initiating gaze aversion were faster and had
larger amplitude than visual saccades. As expected,
aversions seemed to be more distinguishable from the
visual saccades on the vertical rather than on the
horizontal EOG.
However, the present study is far from real-time
detection of attentional switches. We are facing many
limits. First, we show that even on a small sample of
participants, there is huge individual variability in the
gaze aversion behaviour. The ERP analysis shows
that the pattern of the aversion differs between
participants. To be applicable, an individual personal
calibration of the system would be necessary. Here,
we used a small sample size because we aimed to
determine the potential interest of the method, but a
validation of the method would require a higher
sample. Second, to refine the results, future studies
should include recordings of head movements and a
better calibration to infer gaze angle more accurately.
Here, we show that gaze aversions induce a large gaze
angle. However, the linear relation with EOG is not
true for gaze at high eccentricities (e.g., Hládek et al.,
2018). Interestingly however, the results for gaze
aversion during autobiographical memory do not
differ significantly from the one during semantic
memory. It seems therefore linked to internal
attention in general, and maybe not specific to
autobiographical memory.
Despite these limits, we want to emphasize the
importance of studying such behaviour in
aeronautics. Although here we have focused on gaze
aversion occurring during memory retrieval for the
sake of developing an experimental paradigm, in our
view such behaviour is similar to what occurs during
mind-wandering and in fact any behaviour requiring
access to internal thoughts including when one is
speaking to an interlocutor. Monotonous tasks
generate higher rates of mind-wandering, which is a
problem given the increasing automation in the
cockpits (Gouraud et al., 2018). Therefore, we
urgently need an objective marker allowing real-time
detection of internal thoughts switching to monitor
attention in critical situations. In this context, our
work aims to open discussions and perspectives.
5 CONCLUSION
Although our results are preliminary, they are
encouraging. First, we propose a protocol to trigger
attentional switches in the lab. Second, we show that
these switches are associated with gaze aversions.
Third, since we observe that gaze aversion have
different EOG features compared to visual saccades,
we think that EOG could be a potential method to
study and detect attentional switch. In an early
laboratory phase, EOG could be coupled with
augmented reality helmets to characterize gaze
aversion better before reaching a reliable detection
using EOG only.
REFERENCES
Benedek, M., Stoiser, R., Walcher, S., & Körner, C. (2017).
Eye behavior associated with internally versus
externally directed cognition. Frontiers in Psychology,
8, 1092. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01092
Casner, S. M., & Schooler, J. W. (2014). Thoughts in flight:
Automation use and pilots
task-related and task-
unrelated thought. Human Factors, 56(3), 433–442.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0018720813501550
Conway, M. A. (2001). Sensory-perceptual episodic
memory and its context: Autobiographical memory.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of
London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 356(1413),
1375–1384. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2001.0940
Doherty-Sneddon, G., & Phelps, F. G. (2005). Gaze
aversion: A response to cognitive or social difficulty?
Memory and Cognition, 33(4), 727–733.
https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03195338
Dorronsoro, G., Álvarez, J. R. & Soto, J. M. (2019).
"Nonlinear analysis of electrooculogram signals using
higher-order statistics." IEEE Transactions on
Biomedical Engineering, 47, 5, 674-680, 2000.
https://doi.org/10.1109/RBME.2019.2951328
Favre-Félix, A., Graversen, C., Dau, T., & Lunner, T.
(2017). Real-time estimation of eye gaze by in-ear
electrodes. 39th Annual International Conference of the
IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society
(EMBC), 2017, 4086–4089. https://doi.org/10.1109/
EMBC.2017.8037754
Fernandes, M. A., & Moscovitch, M. (2000). Divided
attention and memory: Evidence of substantial
ICCAS 2022 - International Conference on Cognitive Aircraft Systems
28
interference effects at retrieval and encoding. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: General, 129(2), 155–
176. https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-3445.129.2.155
Glenberg, A. M., Schroeder, J. L., & Robertson, D. a.
(1998). Averting the gaze disengages the environment
and facilitates remembering. Memory & Cognition,
26(4), 651–658. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03211385
Gouraud, J., Delorme, A., & Berberian, B. (2018). Out of
the loop, in your bubble: Mind wandering is
independent from automation reliability, but influences
task engagement. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience,
12, 1–13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00383
Hládek, Ľ., Porr, B., & Owen Brimijoin, W. (2018). Real-
time estimation of horizontal gaze angle by saccade
integration using in-ear electrooculography. PLoS
ONE, 13(1), 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.
pone.0190420
Mathôt, S., Schreij, D., & Theeuwes, J. (2012).
OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical experiment
builder for the social sciences. Behavior Research
Methods, 44(2), 314-324. https://doi.org/10.3758/
s13428-011-0168-7
Mildner, J. N., & Tamir, D. I. (2019). Spontaneous thought
as an unconstrained memory process. Trends in
Neurosciences, 42(11), 763–777. https://doi.org/10.10
16/j.tins.2019.09.001
National Transportation Safety Board. (2014). Aircraft
Accident Report (NTSB/AAR-14/01 PB2014-105984).
https://www.ntsb.gov/investigations/accidentreports/re
ports/aar1401.pdf
Smallwood, J., Brown, K. S., Tipper, C., Giesbrecht, B.,
Franklin, M. S., Mrazek, M. D., Carlson, J. M., &
Schooler, J. W. (2011). Pupillometric evidence for the
decoupling of attention from perceptual input during
offline thought. PLoS ONE, 6(3), e18298.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0018298
Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind.
Psychological Bulletin, 132(6), 946–958.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.946
Toivanen, M., Pettersson, K., & Lukander, K. (2015). A
probabilistic real-time algorithm for detecting blinks,
saccades, and fixations from EOG data. Journal of Eye
Movement Research, 8(2), 1–14. https://doi.org/
10.16910/jemr.8.2.1
Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: From mind to brain.
Annual Reviews, 53, 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1146/
annurev.psych.53.100901.135114
Yanko, M. R., & Spalek, T. M. (2014). Driving with the
wandering mind: The effect that mind-wandering has
on driving performance. Human Factors, 56(2), 260–
269. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018720813495280
Can We Use EOG to Identify When Attention Switches Away from the Outside World to Focus on Our Mental Thoughts?
29