Fighter Cockpit Adaption to Online Situation Awareness
Measurement
Simon Schwerd and Axel Schulte
Institute of Flight Systems, Universität der Bundeswehr München, 85577 Neubiberg, Germany
Keywords: Adaptive System, Eye-Tracking, Task Analysis, Situation Awareness.
Abstract: Effective attention management is crucial to fighter pilot performance, but attention-related problems can lead
to a loss of situation awareness, which can contribute to performance decrements. To address this issue,
researchers have proposed the development of user-aware systems that can adapt to a pilot's cognitive state.
This contribution briefly describes an approach of on an eye-tracking-based online estimation of the pilot’s
situation awareness, which is used to trigger alerts to guide the pilot’s attention to relevant information. To
transfer this approach to a military domain, we conducted a goal-directed task analysis (GDTA) with eight
fighter pilots. From the resulting task structure, relevant use-cases were identified and implemented for a
fighter cockpit simulation and evaluated with three pilots. In these trials, the approach was rated positively
for realistic task settings but failed to provide useful assistance when pilots wanted to focus on a single task.
1 INTRODUCTION
Fighter pilots face a broad task spectrum in the
cockpit. Their tasks are considerably more
challenging than those of civil pilots due to additional
mission tasks, which include control of sensors,
weapons, and other systems, under consideration of
constraints and uncertainties. In addition, the pilots
coordinate with manned and, as envisioned in fighter
development programs (e.g., FCAS), unmanned
platforms. This adds coordination with manned-
unmanned teams to the pilots’ responsibilities
(Lindner et al., 2022). In this multitasking
environment, efficient management of attention is
crucial to pilot performance (Olivier Lefrancois et al.,
2016). Attention-related problems in human-
automation interaction are well known and often
described as a possible cause of aviation accidents
(Jones & Endsley, 1996; Kelly & Efthymiou, 2019;
Shorrock, 2007). There are different problems
described in literature, such as attentional tunneling
(Wickens, 2005), inattentional blindness or deafness
(Dehais et al., 2019), vigilance performance
decrement (Thomson et al., 2015) or complacency
(Parasuraman & Manzey, 2010). The consequence of
all these phenomena is a loss of situation awareness
(SA) in specific situational aspects when the pilot
fails to attend relevant information in the cockpit
(Jones & Endsley, 1996). Effective training in
monitoring and automation operation is crucial to
improve pilot attention management, but
nevertheless, the interaction between pilots and
cockpit remains ‘hierarchical in the sense, that the
cockpit cannot be aware of the pilot’s errors. In this
context, the idea of developing user-aware systems
has been proposed to enable error-reducing adaption
of a workplace (Brand & Schulte, 2018; Fortmann &
Mengeringhausen, 2014; Peysakhovich et al., 2018).
In this contribution, we focus on the adaption of a
fighter cockpit to an online estimation of the pilot’s
cognitive state, more specifically their attention
allocation and situation awareness. Thus, our design
goal is a system that reduces the number of situations
where a pilot misses critical information while trying
to avoid nuisance notifications (Schwerd & Schulte,
2021b). To achieve automatic online estimation of
SA, we developed an eye-tracking analysis approach
where, for every fixation on the cockpit display, an
application provides the attended object and a
parametric description of their relevant content (e.g.,
not only “altimeter”, but also the altitude; not only
‘hostile aircraft’, but also its heading, speed, and
distance). This information is used to populate nodes
in a dynamic semantic network that represents the
relevant situational features and relationship between
information, e.g., when the pilot is aware of the
position of two objects, he can also infer a distance
30
Schwerd, S. and Schulte, A.
Fighter Cockpit Adaption to Online Situation Awareness Measurement.
DOI: 10.5220/0011950700003622
In Proceedings of the 1st International Conference on Cognitive Aircraft Systems (ICCAS 2022), pages 30-33
ISBN: 978-989-758-657-6
Copyright
c
2023 by SCITEPRESS Science and Technology Publications, Lda. Under CC license (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0)
between them. Each network node contains a measure
of deviation between the assumed pilot’s awareness
and the actual state of this situational feature.
Therefore, the system can estimate the SA in specific
situational features. In prior studies, we validated this
approach in cockpit simulator studies and showed
correlations of our SA measure with performance and
subjective SA ratings (Schwerd & Schulte, 2020,
2021a). We used this SA model in a subsequent
experiment to trigger cockpit adaptions and alerts to
guide the pilot’s attention towards critical system
information when the deviation in relevant situational
features grew above a certain threshold. With that, we
could improve pilot performance in those situations
where a change of task-relevant system state could
not have been predicted by the participants (Schwerd
& Schulte, 2021c).
While our approach worked reasonably well in
our laboratory trials, transfer to real-world military
application is not trivial. Apart from challenges like
eye-tracking measurement in real cockpits, the
adaptions must be useful in the task context and
should provide benefits to the pilot. Thus, our central
question is: which cockpit information is relevant in
which task situation?
2 METHOD
To answer this question, we conducted a goal-
directed task analysis (GDTA) with eight fight pilots.
Based on this task analysis, we identified use-cases,
which were implemented in our cockpit simulator.
Then, we evaluated these use-cases with three fighter
pilots of the German Air Force.
2.1 Task Analysis to Identify Use-Cases
The GDTA was proposed by (Endsley & Jones, 2012)
and can be used to structure a task environment by its
goals, decisions to meet these goals, and information
requirements to make these decisions. It is especially
suitable for our research question because the task
analysis associates information with its context. For
the interview, we prepared different mission scenario
briefings (e.g., Air Interdiction Mission) to structure
the discussion. On basis of these scenarios, we went
through different mission phases to identify relevant
operational decisions. When we identified a decision,
we asked about all relevant information that is
associated with this information.
2.1.1 Procedure & Participants
We interviewed eight fighter pilots from the German
Air force (all male, mean age 36.2y). Only one pilot
was interviewed per session which lasted about two
hours for each pilot. In our setting, pilots could only
talk about non-classified information. After the
interviews, we organized goals, decisions, and
information into a tree-like structure.
2.1.2 Results
The resulting GDTA is structured by five main goals,
which are displayed in Figure 1. For example, the first
goal is to operate the aircraft under consideration of
the mission plan. Every main goal consists of several
subgoals, also illustrated for goal 1.0 in Figure 1.
Decisions are associated in the lower levels of the
structure and always associated with a subgoal.
Figure 2 shows three examples for relevant decisions,
that must be frequently done in the cockpit. For
example, the subgoal ‘assess planned flight altitude’
is associated with the decision if a change to the UAV
Figure 1: Top-level extract from analysis.
Fighter Cockpit Adaption to Online Situation Awareness Measurement
31
Figure 2: Example decisions from different goals.
altitude is necessary because there is a more secure
route available. To make this decision, the pilot must
collect information in the cockpit about the current
position and types of threats, the current UAV
altitude, and its planned UAV altitude.
Given the many decisions and information
requirements from the analysis, we selected suitable
use-cases suitable to evaluate them in the cockpit
simulator.
2.2 Prototype Evaluation
The second step of this study was the implementation
and evaluation of a cockpit adaption in a
representative task setting. For this, we implemented
12 different adaption use cases in our cockpit
simulator (see Figure 3). Cockpit adaptions were
either a computer-generated text message or an
indication on the tactical map.
Figure 3: Cockpit simulator with integrated eye-tracking.
2.2.1 Procedure & Participants
We invited three fighter pilots to evaluate our system.
Mean age was 36y with mean flight hours on a fighter
jet of 600h. After a training of 2 hours, we fully
explained the basic principle of the cockpit adaption
and introduced all use cases. Then, we evaluated the
implemented assistance system in three scenarios.
Each scenario emphasized different mission types
and pilot tasks (Reconnaissance, UAV Control and
MUM-T Air Interdiction). After each scenario, the
participants were asked to fill out a subjective
usability rating about every specific assistance use
case they encountered. After all scenarios, we
replayed a recording of each trial and interviewed the
pilots about specific situations to gain further insight
into their rating and possible improvements.
2.2.2 Results
The subjective rating, debriefings and preliminary
analysis of the logging data showed the following:
The SA-based adaption was accepted well by
the pilots and subjective rating was positive. The
use-cases were considered to be useful in a real-
world task setting. Especially indications in the
tactical map were evaluated as very helpful. In
addition, pilots asked for phase-of-flight
specific use cases (e.g., take-off, landing).
In a few situations, pilots did not monitor a
certain cockpit display because they relied on the
auditive text messages telling them relevant
information. In other cases, pilots criticized when
text messages contained a lot of information.
Pilots ignored system indications as soon as the
workload was high. Because of this, they often
could not recall an encountered use-case in
some trials.
Pilots disliked to system indications that merely
told them information they should know from a
perspective of SA. In their opinion, the SA-
based indication should only appear when there
is action required.
3 DISCUSSION
Our experiment showed that we successfully
implemented use-cases, that are useful in a real-world
fighter jet cockpit. However, one flaw of our
approach is, that in situations where the pilot focuses
ICCAS 2022 - International Conference on Cognitive Aircraft Systems
32
on a single demanding task while ignoring other
information, our system tends to trigger more
indications since SA in task-irrelevant information
suffers from the attentional focus on a single task. But
these indications are often ignored due to high
workload or might even add more workload. This
problem could be solved by two approaches: Either
delaying indications until the pilot is finished with his
task depending on a measurement of activity
(Honecker & Schulte, 2017) or, in critical cases,
interrupt the pilot with more drastic cockpit adaptions
such as cognitive counter measures (Saint-Lot et al.,
2020). We are planning to evaluate these approaches
in our future studies.
REFERENCES
Brand, Y., & Schulte, A. (2018). Design and Evaluation of a
Workload-Adaptive Associate System for Cockpit
Crews. In D. Harris (Ed.), Engineering Psychology and
Cognitive Ergonomics (pp. 3–18). Springer International
Publishing.
Dehais, F., Roy, R. N., & Scannella, S. (2019). Inattentional
deafness to auditory alarms: Inter-individual differences,
electrophysiological signature and single trial
classification. Behavioural Brain Research, 360, 51–59.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2018.11.045
Endsley, M. R., & Jones, D. G. (2012). Designing for
situation awareness: An approach to user-centered
design / Mica R. Endsley and Debra G. Jones (2nd ed.).
CRC Press.
Fortmann, F., & Mengeringhausen, T. (2014). Development
and Evaluation of an Assistant System to Aid Monitoring
Behavior during Multi-UAV Supervisory Control. In C.
Stary (Ed.), Proceedings of the 2014 European
Conference on Cognitive Ergonomics - ECCE '14 (pp. 1–
8). ACM Press. https://doi.org/10.1145/2637248.263
7257
Honecker, F., & Schulte, A. (2017). Automated Online
Determination of Pilot Activity Under Uncertainty by
Using Evidential Reasoning. In D. Harris (Ed.), Lecture
Notes in Computer Science. Engineering Psychology and
Cognitive Ergonomics: Cognition and Design (Vol.
10276, pp. 231–250). Springer International Publishing.
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-58475-1_18
Jones, D. G., & Endsley, M. R. (1996). Sources of situation
awareness errors in aviation. Aviation, Space, and
Environmental Medicine, 67(6), 507–512.
Kelly, D., & Efthymiou, M. (2019). An analysis of human
factors in fifty controlled flight into terrain aviation
accidents from 2007 to 2017. Journal of Safety Research,
69, 155–165. https://doi.org/10.1016/ j.jsr.2019.03.009
Lindner, S., Mund, D., & Schulte, A. (2022). How Human-
Autonomy Teams change the Role of future Fighter
Pilots: An Experimental Assessment. In AIAA SCITECH
Forum. https://doi.org/10.2514/6.2022-2551
Olivier Lefrancois, Nadine Matton, Yves Gourinat, Vsevolod
Peysakhovich, & Mickaël Causse (2016). The role of
Pilots’ monitoring strategies in flight performance. In
European Association for Aviation Psychology
Conference EAAP32. https://oatao.univ-toulouse.fr/16
173/
Parasuraman, R., & Manzey, D. H. (2010). Complacency and
bias in human use of automation: An attentional
integration. Human Factors, 52(3), 381–410.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0018720810376055
Peysakhovich, V., Lefrançois, O., Dehais, F., & Causse, M.
(2018). The Neuroergonomics of Aircraft Cockpits: The
Four Stages of Eye-Tracking Integration to Enhance
Flight Safety. Safety, 4(1), 8. https://doi.org/10.3390/
safety4010008
Saint-Lot, J., Imbert, J.‑P., & Dehais, F. (2020). Red Alert: A
Cognitive Countermeasure to Mitigate Attentional
Tunneling. In R. Bernhaupt, F. '. Mueller, D. Verweij, J.
Andres, J. McGrenere, A. Cockburn, I. Avellino, A.
Goguey, P. Bjørn, S. Zhao, B. P. Samson, & R. Kocielnik
(Eds.), Proceedings of the 2020 CHI Conference on
Human Factors in Computing Systems (pp. 1–6). ACM.
https://doi.org/10.1145/3313831.33 76709
Schwerd, S., & Schulte, A. (2020). Experimental Validation
of an Eye-Tracking-Based Computational Method for
Continuous Situation Awareness Assessment in an
Aircraft Cockpit. In D. Harris & W.-C. Li (Eds.), Lecture
Notes in Computer Science. Engineering Psychology and
Cognitive Ergonomics. Cognition and Design (Vol.
12187, pp. 412–425). Springer International Publishing.
https://doi.org/10.10 07/978-3-030-49183-3_32
Schwerd, S., & Schulte, A. (2021a). Measuring the Deviation
between Ground Truth and Operator Awareness in a
UAV Management Scenario: An Eye-Tracking
Approach. In AIAA Scitech 2021 Forum. American
Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics. https://doi.org/
10.2514/6.2021-1579
Schwerd, S., & Schulte, A. (2021b). Operator State
Estimation to Enable Adaptive Assistance in Manned-
Unmanned-Teaming. Cognitive Systems Research, 67,
73–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogsys.2021.01.002
Schwerd, S., & Schulte, A. (2021c). Situation Awareness
Adaptive Alerting in an Aircraft Cockpit: A Simulator
Study. Preprint. Advance online publication.
https://doi.org/10.31224/osf.io/9gmnv
Shorrock, S. T. (2007). Errors of perception in air traffic
control. Safety Science, 45(8), 890–904.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssci.2006.08.018
Thomson, D. R., Besner, D., & Smilek, D. (2015). A
resource-control account of sustained attention: Evidence
from mind-wandering and vigilance paradigms.
Perspectives on Psychological Science: A Journal of the
Association for Psychological Science, 10(1), 82–96.
https://doi.org/10.1177/174569161455 6681
Wickens, C. D. (2005). Attentional Tunneling and Task
Management. International Symposium on Aviation
Psychology.
Fighter Cockpit Adaption to Online Situation Awareness Measurement
33