disassembly phase is shorter than the assembly phase.
In more detail, the assembly/disassembly stages are
not totally symmetrical: for example, unscrewing
does not require any torque wrench, while it is
mandatory in the assembly phase.
Nonetheless, the ratio of time spent viewing the
documentation is higher in the disassembly phase. In
addition, the cumulative time spent viewing the
document is mostly at the beginning of the
disassembly phase. The two longest phases occur at
the beginning of the operation (Stage I & II)
corresponding to more than 55% of the total viewing
time, while short reading sequences occur in the
following stages.
This consultation pattern reveals an important
information acquisition phase at the beginning of the
operation. This suggests that the operator carries out
an initial preparatory organization and viewing on the
whole documentation and reads specific information
on critical points during each stage (ex. finding a
force momentum torque). We suggest that the quality
of this preparatory phase has a direct impact on the
good and successful progress of the whole operation.
From this first instantiation of our methodology,
we conclude that it allows us to demonstrate 1) the
unbalanced repartition of the different phases of the
operation 2) and a specific pattern of consultation of
the procedural documentation. Our methodology will
allow us to test the generalization of these patterns to
a larger population and to test specific profiles
(novice & expert).
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
Florence Paris’s work is supported by a doctoral grant
from ANRT (l’Association Nationale de la Recherche
et de la Technologie) under N° IPAH13430, in
agreement between Airbus Helicopters and Aix-
Marseille University.
REFERENCES
Duchowski, A. T., 2002. A breadth-first survey of eye-
tracking applications. (Springer, Ed.) Behavior
Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 34(4),
455-470.
Grusenmeyer, C. 2014. Maintenance: organizational
modes, activities and health and safety. Use of a French
national survey and in-situ analyses. (Elsevier, Ed.)
Accident Analysis & Prevention, 187-199.
Henderson, J. M. 2003. Human gaze control during real-
world scene perception. (Elsevier, Ed.) Trends in
cognitive sciences, 7(11), 498-504.
Hutchins, E., Weibel, N., Emmenegger, C., Fouse, A., &
Holder, B. (2013). An integrative approach to
understanding flight crew activity. (C. SAGE
Publications Sage CA: Los Angeles, Ed.) Journal of
Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making, 7(4),
353-376.
Jacob, R. J., & Karn, K. S. (2003). Eye tracking in human-
computer interaction and usability research: Ready to
deliver the promises. In The mind's eye (pp. 573-605).
North-Holland.
Land, M. F. (2006). Eye movements and the control of
actions in everyday life. (Elsevier, Ed.) Progress in
retinal and eye research, 25(3), 296-324.
Leplat, J. (2008). Eléments pour l'étude des documents
prescripteurs. In J. Leplat, Le Travail humain (pp. 93-
130). Presses Universitaires de France.
Ren, X., & Philipose, M. (2009). Egocentric recognition of
handled objects: Benchmark and analysis. IEEE
Computer Society Conference on Computer Vision and
Pattern Recognition Workshops, (pp. 1-8).
Souza, M. L., Pereira-Guizzo, C. S., Santos, A. A. B., &
León-Rubio, J. M. (2021). Visual attention of experts
and novices to a critical industrial maintenance task. (S.
Brasil, Ed.) Gestão & Produção, 28.
Zafiharimalala, H. (2011). Étude ergonomique pour la
consultation sur écran de petite taille de la
documentation de maintenance aéronautique (Doctoral
dissertation, Université Toulouse le Mirail-Toulouse
II).