The worst absolute result was 10% (which means
that only one in ten students saw that there was no
change in the angle between the proximal and distal
segments between the two phases), and this happened
in two cases: in the second measurement point of the
change between the first (initial contact) and second
(loading response) phases in the hip, and in the
second measurement point of the change between the
third (mid stance) and fourth (terminal stance) phases
in the ankle (Figure 4).
On the other hand, the best average result was in
the evaluation of the change between the fifth (pre-
swing) and the sixth (initial swing) phases in the knee
(x
̄
= 88.24%), and the best absolute result was 100%
correct, achieved in the 2
nd
and 3
rd
timepoint of
observation of the change between the second
(loading response) and third (mid stance) phase in the
hip (Figure 5), with the average result of all
timepoints for that change x
̄
= 82.45%.
4 DISCUSSION
In this study, we examined the ability and skill of
first-year students in observational kinematic gait
analysis.
The hypothesis was confirmed: the students
generally did not accurately assess the human gait
(from the initial 43.96%, through 61.95%, to the final
62.45% distribution of correct answers), in the
observational way that experienced experts do in their
clinical practice.
To draw a parallel with the academic
environment, for this task the students received an
average grade of "Sufficient 2", the lowest passing
grade (i.e., to pass the exam, 60% correct answers are
required). However, this was only achieved at the
second and third timepoints; on average, they initially
failed. The lack of better results at the last two
timepoints, along with the lack of progress between
them, can be interpreted as insufficient knowledge
and skills of the students, but mainly in the context of
observational obstacles – perceptive and cognitive. In
a way, the students fell into the same trap that
Théodore Gericault had found himself in 200 years
earlier. Toister (2020) interprets such obstacles as the
temporal incompatibility of photographic technology,
where viewing is certainly a non-participatory
experience, unlike live viewing or observation in
which the observer is also a participant.
Technology-free approaches, such as
observational gait analysis, are commonly used in
clinical practice due to their simplicity and
affordability. However, these are highly subjective
methods, where the assessment results depend on the
interpretation skills and experience of the clinician
(Michelini et al., 2020). This gap should be bridged
with an objective assessment approach, e.g., video-
based two-dimensional (2D) motion analysis or
computerized three-dimensional (3D) motion
analysis. Some of the answers to the question of why
the students were so imprecise in their observational
assessment could be found in article by Toister
(2020), where he contextualizes that there are many
anatomical reasons for rejecting the comparison
between the eye and the camera (e.g., people have
two eyes and not one; human eyes are never fixed and
are always moving; neurological and cognitive
abilities are important for the perception of depth and
movement in the human vision, and the camera does
not offer anything similar to the above capabilities).
Toister (2020) concludes that human vision is more
similar to videography than photography, if at all.
The results of the assessment were the best for the
knee joint in all three levels of measurement. The
reason could be that the knee is the easiest to observe
due to its greatest range of motion. Ross et al. (2015)
also determined that the results of the testers' knee
joint assessment are closest to normal values, while
the results they obtained for the hip joint and
especially the ankle are not promising.
The students had the most difficulties in detecting
cases where there is no change in angle between
phases (e.g., the second measurement point of the
change between the first (initial contact) and second
(loading response) phases in the hip, and the second
measurement point of the change between the third
(mid stance) and fourth (terminal stance) phases in
the ankle. This means that because observers noticed
the femur shifts backwards, 70% of them (N = 136)
thought they were seeing extension, forgetting that at
the same time the pelvis and torso continued to move
forward (or went to the right, speaking in a two-
dimensional way; as Toister (2020) points out, our
field of vision is elliptical and not enclosed in a
rectangular frame), leaving hip angle unchanged
between the two phases. Similarly, visual detection of
heel-off (see Figure 4), led more than 50% of
assessors (N = 97) to be sure they saw (plantar)
flexion. However, the point is that they missed that
both segments moved from phase to phase and their
relationship to each other did not change (i.e., the
ankle angle remained the same).
It is not that the students are not able to recognize
if there is no change between two segments (e.g.,
there were 72.1% (N = 137) correct answers in the
second measurement point of the change between the
third (mid stance) and fourth (terminal stance) phases