A Value-Driven Approach to the Online Consent Conundrum: A Study
with the Unemployed
Paul van Schaik and Karen Renaud
Teesside University, University of Strathclyde, U.K.
Keywords:
Values, Value Creators, Consent Forms, Unemployed.
Abstract:
Online services are required to gain informed consent from users to collect, store and analyse their personal
data, both intentionally divulged and derived during their use of the service. There are many issues with these
forms: they are too long, too complex and demand the user’s attention too frequently. Many users consent
without reading so do not know what they are agreeing to. As such, granted consent is effectively uninformed.
In this paper, we report on two studies we carried out to arrive at a value-driven approach to inform efforts
to reduce the length of consent forms. The first study interviewed unemployed users to identify the values
they want these forms to satisfy. The second survey study helped us to quantify the values and value creators.
To ensure that we understood the particular valuation of the unemployed, we compared their responses to
those of an employed demographic and observed no significant differences between their prioritisation on any
of the values. However, we did find substantial differences between values and value creators, with effort
minimisation being most valued by our participants.
1 INTRODUCTION
The requirement for mandating informed consent to
permit online data gathering and processing inherits
the paradigm from the fields of medicine and research
(Beauchamp, 2011). Legislation such as the European
Union’s General Data Protection Regulation (GDPR)
forces online service providers to ask users to con-
sent to collection, storage and processing of their data.
There are, unfortunately, many reasons for the failure
of this mechanism to obtain truly informed consent.
Solove (2012, p. 1888) writes that “consent is not
meaningful in many contexts involving privacy” be-
cause (1) people do not read privacy policies; (2) if
people read them, they do not understand them; (3)
if people read and understand them, they often lack
enough background knowledge to make an informed
choice; and (4) if people read them, understand them,
and can make an informed choice, their choice might
be skewed by various decision making difficulties”.
Solove is suggesting that people are not granting in-
formed consent. Users cope with the frequent unus-
able consent forms they encounter by dismissing them
(without reading them) (Parfenova et al., 2024). This
means that consent forms, in general, do not fulfil
their core purpose (Chomanski and Lauwaert, 2023).
The length of privacy policies deters online users from
engaging with them (McDonald and Cranor (2008)),
given the effort required. If the length can be reduced,
it would likely mitigate the situation, but such short-
ening must be done mindfully. One way to do so is
to ensure that the forms provide only the information
that satisfies users’ values. As yet, we do not know
what these values are, and in the absence of this, con-
sent forms strive towards comprehensive coverage of
all information. We identified these values by inter-
viewing unemployed users. Having derived a set of
values from a qualitative analysis, we carried out a
second study to determine how users (specifically our
target users, the unemployed) would comparatively
rate the derived values (Section 2). Section 3 reports
on our findings.
2 METHOD
2.1 Research Questions and Study
Design
We conducted two studies, with the following re-
search questions and designs. Study 1: What are the
informed consent-related values and value creators
for the unemployed? A laddering interview design
van Schaik, P. and Renaud, K.
A Value-Driven Approach to the Online Consent Conundrum: A Study with the Unemployed.
DOI: 10.5220/0013083300003899
Paper published under CC license (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0)
In Proceedings of the 11th International Conference on Information Systems Security and Privacy (ICISSP 2025) - Volume 1, pages 133-140
ISBN: 978-989-758-735-1; ISSN: 2184-4356
Proceedings Copyright © 2025 by SCITEPRESS Science and Technology Publications, Lda.
133
(Dolan, 1989) was used to elicit values and value cre-
ators that contribute to informed consent in the con-
text of online services. The output of the study was a
hierarchical set of values and value creators. We also
measured privacy literacy (Trepte et al., 2015).
Study 2: How do unemployed people weight the val-
ues and value creators in the informed consent con-
text, and how do these differ from the way the em-
ployed rank these?
A two-group independent measures design was
used. The groups were unemployed and employed
people. The output of the study was a quantified hi-
erarchical structure of values and value creators with
value and value-creator weightings separate for em-
ployed vs unemployed participants.
2.2 Participants
Studied Demographic. We chose to focus on values
of the unemployed, and their experiences in engag-
ing with online consent forms. Seabright (2010) ex-
plains that the unemployed inhabit ‘information is-
lands’. Unlike the employed who benefit from regular
security awareness training, there are no bridges for
the unemployed to gain up-to-date information. This
means that it is easy for misunderstandings to gain
traction because people are out of touch with the latest
security advice. Seabright says that society does not
construct bridges to increasingly isolated unemployed
communities. The cyber security field is dynamic and
fluid due to the sustained and inventive efforts of cy-
ber criminals. This demographic is thus more vulner-
able to losing their privacy. Moreover, declining to
give consent might be infeasible if monetary rewards
are dependent on consent, perhaps more likely a pres-
sure point for the unemployed.
Study 1. Thirteen unemployed participants re-
sponded to an invitation from a previous study
(Van Schaik et al. (2024)). They were compensated
for their time through a voucher or a SIM card 30 gi-
gabytes of free data to use on their smartphone.
Study 2. One hundred and two unemployed par-
ticipants and the same number of employed partic-
ipants, 115 female (68 unemployed, 47 employed)
and 89 male (34 unemployed, 55 employed), were re-
cruited through via an online survey panel and com-
pensated for their time according to the panel’s rate.
Their mean age was 52 (SD = 15).
2.3 Materials
Study 1. A laddering interview guide was created and
used. Two scales were used: the Online Privacy Lit-
eracy Scale (OPLIS) (Trepte et al., 2015) was used to
measure specific privacy knowledge. (Details of our
OPLIS scoring scheme are presented under Study 2,
as the development of this scheme required a larger
sample than that of Study 1.) In this sample the mean
was 12.92 (SD =3.12), which corresponds to approx-
imately a 57 percentile rank according to Masur et al.
(2017).
Study 2. An AHP survey was created according to
guidelines for survey construction (Dolan et al., 1989;
Dolan, 2008), using the output of Study 1 (a hierarchy
of values and value creators) as input. All the possi-
ble pairs of values underlying the higher-order goal
of informed consent were formed as well as all the
possible pairs of value creators underlying each value.
The collective pairs were presented sequentially (first
the value pairs [randomised] and then the value cre-
ator pairs for each value [values randomised; value
creator pairs randomised within values]). Participants
had to evaluate the relative importance within each
pair (for example, the importance of the value of con-
trol relative to that of fairness). As in Study 1, OPLIS
measured privacy literacy. The Affinity for Technol-
ogy Interaction scale (ATI) measured the tendency to
actively engage in intensive technology interaction
(Franke et al., 2019, p. 456).
2.4 Procedure
Study 1. Because of pandemic restrictions and to
reach a geographically UK-wide audience, interviews
were conducted remotely by VoIP or by telephone,
recorded and automatically transcribed. Afterwards,
the recordings were played back and any corrections
were made to the transcripts. In each interview, the
participant was asked to identify value creators (what
an online consent form should provide) and subse-
quently for each value creator one or more values
(why the value creator is important). Interviews lasted
from 14 to 34 minutes. After each interview, the par-
ticipant was directed to an online survey to complete
OPLIS.
Study 2. Participants were directed to an online
survey that administered demographic questions, a se-
ries of AHP pairwise comparisons, OPLIS and the
ATI scale.
Ethics. Research ethics approval was ob-
tained from the University of Strathclyde and from
REPHRAIN, National Research Centre on Privacy,
Harm Reduction and Adversarial Influence Online.
2.5 Data Analysis
Study 1. We used the framework of means-ends chain
analysis to identify people’s needs (value) and how
ICISSP 2025 - 11th International Conference on Information Systems Security and Privacy
134
these could be achieved (value creators) (Kilwinger
and van Dam, 2021). Both researchers coded an ini-
tial set of five transcripts. Their individual coding
schemes were discussed and a final coding scheme
was agreed. One of the authors then coded all the
transcripts.
Study 2. AHP analysis of response consistency
and weightings was conducted (Dolan et al., 1989).
Analysis of variance (ANOVA) techniques were used
to analyse differences between unemployed and em-
ployed participants on the AHP weightings.
3 FINDINGS
3.1 OPLIS & ATI
The original OPLIS has four dimensions to mea-
sure online privacy literacy: institutional practices,
technical aspects of data protection, data protection
law, and data protection strategies. As explained by
Edelsbrunner (2022), knowledge in different domains
are often best assessed with formative measurement.
Therefore, we created a measure consisting of items
from each of these four dimensions. From each di-
mension, we selected two items, based on the percent-
age of the sample that answered correctly: between 25
pct and 50 pct, the item with the minimum correct-
ness and, between 50 pct and 75 pct, the item with
the maximum correctness. This procedure ensured an
equal mix of more difficult and easier items. A t test
showed that online privacy literacy did not differ be-
tween employed (M = 56.86; SD = 23.57) and unem-
ployed (M = 53.19; SD = 20.79) participants, t = 1.18,
df = 198.89, p = 0.24, d = 0.17.
Factor analysis of the Study 2 data was conducted
on the ATI and produced a one-factor solution, with
56 pct of variance explained. Factor scores were cal-
culated and used in subsequent analysis. A t test
showed that affinity for technology interaction was
higher in employed (M = 0.14; SD = 1.02) than in
unemployed (M = -0.18; SD = 0.91) participants, t =
2.84, df = 199.31, p = 0.005, d = 0.40.
3.2 Study 1
Study 1 sought to identify a hierarchical means-end
structure of informed consent for online services (Fig-
ure 1).
Five values were identified: (1) control, (2) un-
certainty avoidance, (3) loss aversion, (4) effort min-
imisation and (5) fairness. Under each value, two or
more value creators were identified. The value cre-
ators (means) underlying each value (end) would con-
tribute to the value. In turn, the values would con-
tribute to the higher-order goal of informed online
consent decision-making. The values and underly-
ing value creators are presented here, with illustrative
quotations, where P<number> represents a quoted
numbered participant.
3.2.1 Control
Control was defined as a user’s feeling that they have
control. Three value creators contributing to the value
of control were identified.
(1) Payment for Data (being paid for giving
data/having one’s data captured by an online service).
Participants expected to be paid for the data they
provided, but this was not often not the case: I’m a
big fan, by the way, of this idea that they pay me for
it ... If they’re making money off it, I should get my
cut. (P55)
(2) Access to Services (not having to sign up for cer-
tain services in order to be able to read the pages) (see
Fairness [value], Should consent be required[value
creator])
(3) Range of Choice Options (having a range
of choice options in responding to an online consent
form, for example, only the options of accept all or
reject all, or a larger set of more fine-grained options
that offers more choice).
Participants expected they would be able to select
which data will be shared: What I want to see in
these forms is ... can I selectively choose? Alright,
you can have my location, but you can’t have some-
thing else. You can’t track me for example, as I’m
using the website. (P26) You see particulars, my lo-
cation or something, but I don’t remember consenting
to that. (P26)
3.2.2 Uncertainty Avoidance
Uncertainty avoidance was defined as a user’s desire
to gain information to reduce or remove uncertainty.
Eight value creators contributing to the value of
uncertainty avoidance were identified. agree to it and
otherwise they would not offer the service.(P29)
(1) Consumers’ Rights (information about con-
sumers’ rights when they use the specific online
service that is provided).
Consumers’ rights in consent documents were
seen as beneficial for both users and service compa-
nies: I suppose it should provide what they expect of
me and what I can expect of them. (P56) I would
A Value-Driven Approach to the Online Consent Conundrum: A Study with the Unemployed
135
EFFORT
MINIMISATION
Conciseness
Summary
Information
How Long
Consent Lasts
Ease of Reading
Information Clarity
UNCERTAINTY
AVOIDANCE
Attracting
Attention
Standardisation
Certification
Summary
Information
Consequences
Information
Processing
Clarity
Consumer
Rights
LOSS
AVERSION
Consequences
Risk
Information
FAIRNESS
Functionality
Legal
Protection
Should it be
Required?
CONTROL
Payment for
Data
Range Choice
Options
Access to
Services
Figure 1: Means-end hierarchy of user values and value creators contributing to informed consent decision making.
like to know what my rights are as a consumer so
if there was just a sheet of main bullet points that
probably would be better. (P73)
(2) Information Processing (information about
what happens to the user’s personal data).
Participants expected that a consent document
would explain how their data would be stored and
be processed, and why: Information in such a con-
sent form would include how they handle data and
privacy [and] whether they hold data on computers
in the USA. (P33) When information is stored or
where it is stored ” (P57)
(3) Attracting your Attention (highlighting important
information to attract a your attention). Participants
expected that critical information in a consent docu-
ment would be highlighted in order to attract users’
attention before they decided to consent: I’m always
very of the idea that it should be clear, and if it is
very important, it should have something like a red
box around it ... should be highlighted ... the abil-
ity to follow you to other websites ... The ability to
sell the information to unknown and any third party
without either compensating me for it or asking my
permission. (P55)
(4) Information Clarity (how easy it is to understand
the online consent text (simplicity and comprehensi-
bility of text) [see effort minimisation].
(5) Consequences (information linked to impor-
tant consequences for the online-service user) [see
loss aversion].
(6) Standardisation (standardisation of forms
according to relevant information categories for
users).
Standardisation, together with shortening consent
documents, would also facilitate users’ reading and
understanding as a basis for making a consent deci-
sion: If it was shorter; I think if it if it was standard-
ised so you knew what certain sentences meant and so
it was just more bullet points and you knew what that
referred to instead of it being a long complex thing all
the time. (P77)
(7) Certification (a sign presented on the consent
form to show certification by a trusted party). Certi-
fication with icon visualization could communicate
consent information quickly and clear. [If] it was
icons that you got used to know what they mean, like
the Facebook icons. Yes, [if] there was an icon ’we
sell your data’. [It] would just be quick when you get
used to seeing [the icon], when you know instantly
what we were signing up to. (P77)
(8) Summary Information (summary consent
information, with links to further details, for exam-
ple, clickable icons that link to detailed information).
The expectation was that providing summary in-
formation with links to detailed information would re-
duce the required reading time to make a consent de-
cision, make it more likely that users would read the
information and cater for users with different needs in
terms of information detail: [A] bit more streamlined
so you’ve got four options, so you don’t have to click
through. You might have the option to go through and
read more information, but you shouldn’t have to. It
should give you a brief summary of each thing that
can select. (P83)
3.2.3 Effort Minimisation
Effort minimisation was defined as a reduction in
the effort required to process the information that
is presented. Five value creators contributing to the
value of effort minimisation were identified.
(1) Conciseness (conciseness of text, for exam-
ple, using bullet points). Concise writing in consent
documents could facilitate reading and understand-
ing: Should follow guidance from the Plain English
Campaign. Online consent forms should be brief and
concise ... [The] benefit should be easily read and
understood in 2 to 3 minutes. (P33)
ICISSP 2025 - 11th International Conference on Information Systems Security and Privacy
136
(2) Ease of Reading (how easy it is to read the on-
line consent text, for example in terms of font size).
Text should facilitate reading, for example by use of
the sufficiently large font, but participants’ experience
was the opposite: Should also make the type (font)
bigger. Should use normal print. (P33)
(3) How Long Consent Lasts (one-time consent
process or consent required each time the online
service is accessed). Participants experienced the
same consent process on repeat use of the same
application, which was seen as inefficient: I always
find it quite strange when they have to ask again.
(P55)
(4) Information Clarity (how easy it is to un-
derstand the online consent text). Participants felt
that online consent text should be understandable
also by non-specialist users in technology and data
protection. Someone like me who is not very techy
doesn’t really understand ... any information. (P26)
A bit more simple, a lot less kind of like jargon and
lingo”. (P57)
(5) Summary Information (summary consent
information, with links to further detail (for example,
clickable icons that link to detailed information) [see
Uncertainty Avoidance]
3.2.4 Fairness
Fairness was defined as a user’s feeling that their
personal rewards and costs and those of another party
are in balance with each other. Three value creators
contributing to the value of fairness were identified.
(1) Should Consent be Required? (feeling that
the user’s need for consent is in balance with the
extent of the functionality that the online service or
website provides to the user).
Participants felt that consent procedures should be
proportionate int that users’ effort should be in bal-
ance with the functionality that consent gives access
to: But you know if all I really want to do is look at
a picture or read a very brief article, I don’t want to
read 30 pages of terms and conditions. (P55)
(2) Functionality (feeling that the nature or volume
of personal data the user provides is in balance with
the functionality they receive from the online service
or website)
Users realised that online services often operate
a business where, as a condition for using an online
service for free of monetary change, users give their
personal data. Users consented although they did not
(fully) agree: You make a deal with the devil. You’ve
got to pay the price. So, yeah, I’m kind of one of those
where if I want to use a service, I have to accept that
I give them my data and they use it the way they see
fit. (P55)
(3) Legal Protection (feeling that the user’s diffi-
culty of understanding the complexity of the text is in
balance with protection that the text gives the online
service provider against legal action).
The use of specialist legal language that was chal-
lenging for users to understand was seen as unfair: I
think it should be clear enough that you don’t have
to have done a law course at university to understand
the basics. (P55) It would be important to avoid
legalese in the phrasing of these standardised forms.
(P64)
3.2.5 Loss Aversion
Loss aversion was defined as a user’s preference to
avoid losses. Two value creators contributing to the
value of loss aversion were identified.
(1) Risk Information (risk-related data and commu-
nication of potential risk). A strategy for users to
reduce risk was to stay with trusted major companies
rather than rely on consent documents: I don’t even
look if I know a company’s name ... I tend to just
trust it anyway. So that’s why I tend to stick with the
big names of the brand. (P57)
(2) Information about Consequences (information
linked to important risky consequences for the user).
Participants believed that about information risky
consequences of consent for online services was not
always available to or not read by users, with serious
potential consequences. In response, a strategy was
for users to reduce the personal information they gave
away: I mean you sign up to one site and you didn’t
know but probably ... access to God knows how many
countries ... data being sold to Russia and China or
whatever exactly that they would be doing on a daily
basis without [you] knowing. (P77)
3.3 Study 2
Study 2 sought to quantify the perceived relative im-
portance of the values and value creators identified in
Study 1.
Consistency. Consistency (of comparative judg-
ment) ratios were calculated for the top-level goal of
informed online consent from the pairwise compared
values and for each of four of the five values from the
pairwise compared values. (Consistency did not ap-
ply to the value of loss aversion, as there were two
value creators.) According to the standard cut-off for
A Value-Driven Approach to the Online Consent Conundrum: A Study with the Unemployed
137
consistency (consistency ratio, CR < 0.10) 27 pct to
36 pct of cases was consistent, and 63 pct to 69 pct
was consistent with a cut-off of 0.20.
Weightings, sensitivity analysis. Sensitivity anal-
ysis was conducted in the subsequent analysis of
weightings for values and value creators to establish
the robustness of the findings. The pattern of results
of means and confidence intervals for informed on-
line consent and each of the five values was the same
for the two cut-offs; the pattern of inferential statistics
was the same for the two cut-offs (see results below).
Weightings. The means with confidence inter-
vals (Diagrams in Figure 2 ) show substantial vari-
ability among the values for informed online consent
and among the value creators for each value (except
for loss aversion). t-tests showed no effect of em-
ployment status on any of the weightings. Two-way
mixed ANOVA, with Greenhouse-Geisser correction
for sphericity, showed no main effect of employment
status and no interaction effect of status with value or
value creator on any of the measures, for informed
online consent or any of the four values. Two-way
mixed ANCOVA showed that neither were ATI and
OPLIS significant covariates. Therefore, the results
of one-way repeated-measures ANOVA with value or
value creator as the independent variable, corrected
for sphericity, are reported here. The results for CR
< 0.10 are reported here (the results for CR < 0.20
[available on request] follow the same pattern). The
main effects of value (for informed online consent)
and value creator (for each of the values) are reported
here as well as pairwise comparisons.
For informed online consent, the effect of value
was significant, F (3.12, 227.75) = 33.67, p < 0.001,
pes = 0.32. Effort minimisation was the dominant
value (greater mean than that of the other values), fol-
lowed by uncertainty avoidance and loss aversion. For
the value of control, the effect of value creator was
significant, F (1.85, 105.62) = 3.74, p = 0.03, pes =
0.06. The mean for payment for data was greater than
that for access to services. For the value of fairness,
the effect of value creator was significant, F (1.97,
140.14) = 8.08, p < 0.001, pes = 0.10. Functional-
ity was the dominant value (greater mean than that of
the other value creators). For the value of uncertainty
avoidance, the effect of value creator was significant,
F (4.89, 246.89) = 20.20, p < 0.001, pes = 0.32. At-
tracting your attention was the most dominant value
creator (higher mean than that of the other value cre-
ators, except for Standardisation), followed by Stan-
dardisation, and summary information and certifica-
tion. For the value of effort minimisation, the effect
of value creator was significant, F (3.26, 205.53) =
10.29, p < 0.001, pes = 0.14. Information clarity had
the lowest weighting (mean smaller than that of the
other value creators); the other value creators were
not significantly different. For the value of loss aver-
sion, the effect of value creator was not significant, F
(1, 203) = 2.61, p = 0.11, pes = 0.01.
4 DISCUSSION & REFLECTION
The relative quantification of values and value cre-
ators shown in Figure 2 is instructive. In particular,
the results show that effort minimisation is most im-
portant. This justifies our proposal of reducing length
of policies so as to reduce effort. However, the sec-
ond most important one is uncertainty avoidance, so
it is important to ensure that in reducing length we
ensure that the information people want is easily ac-
cessed. We can use the value creators as a steer in
terms of what information people want to see in a con-
sent form.
The surprising finding is related to the relatively
low ranking of control. This is interesting because the
very consent form mechanism is based on the assump-
tion that users want to control their privacy, in other
words have control over who has their data and how
these are used (Human Rights Watch, 2018). The rel-
atively low ranking of control, accompanied by the
low ranking of consumer rights as a value creator,
by both unemployed and employed participants, calls
this assumption into question. This apparent indif-
ference might be due to the issues mentioned earlier,
namely the frequency with which users are presented
with these forms, and the effort that is required to pro-
cess them. It might be that they are making a perfectly
reasonable trade-off in order to be able to get anything
done at all.
The other surprising low-ranked value is fairness,
because we know that humans have a deep need to be
treated fairly (Folger et al., 1998; Folger and Cropan-
zano, 2001; Nicklin et al., 2011; Folger and Cropan-
zano, 2011; Ganegoda and Folger, 2015; Folger and
Shukla, 2019). Even so, our participants indicated
that this value did not mean as much to them as the
other values. It might be that people have come to ex-
pect to be treated unfairly in this domain, or that effort
minimisation and uncertainty avoidance are just that
much more important in this context.
ICISSP 2025 - 11th International Conference on Information Systems Security and Privacy
138
Informed Consent Control
Fairness Uncertainty Avoidance 1/2
Effort Minimisation Uncertainty Avoidance 2/2
* p <0.05; ** p <0.01; *** p <0.001; NS: not significant.
Figure 2: Value Creator Weightings for Informed Online Consent Means with Confidence Intervals.
5 CONCLUSION & FUTURE
WORK
To address problems associated with online consent
from a user’s perspective, we suggest a value-driven
approach which can be used to remove information
(and shorten the policy) by providing information that
users value rather than all possible information in the
consent form. To apply this approach, we needed to
understand what users value. We decided to focus on
the values of unemployed users given the particular
challenges they face in protecting their privacy online.
The raison d’
ˆ
etre for this research was thus to in-
form a value-driven approach in reducing the length
of consent forms, with a specific focus on unem-
ployed users. We carried out two studies which, to-
gether, delivered a quantified hierarchy of values and
value creators. This will inform the next stage of our
study where we will carry out an empirical study to
compare user preference and comprehension of a tra-
ditional consent form versus a value-driven consent
form.
As future work, it would be very interesting and
important to explore the reasons behind these rank-
ings so that we understand users’ thought processes
when contemplating and dealing with online consent
forms.
A Value-Driven Approach to the Online Consent Conundrum: A Study with the Unemployed
139
REFERENCES
Beauchamp, T. L. (2011). Informed consent: its history,
meaning, and present challenges. Cambridge Quar-
terly of Healthcare Ethics, 20(4):515–523.
Chomanski, B. and Lauwaert, L. (2023). Online con-
sent: how much do we need to know? AI & SO-
CIETY, pages 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00146-
023-01790-2.
Dolan, J. G. (1989). Medical decision making using the ana-
lytic hierarchy process: choice of initial antimicrobial
therapy for acute pyelonephritis. Medical Decision
Making, 9(1):51–56.
Dolan, J. G. (2008). Shared decision-making - transferring
research into practice: The analytic hierarchy process
(ahp). Patient Education and Counseling, 73(3):418
425. 10.1016/j.pec.2008.07.032.
Dolan, J. G., Isselhardt, B. J., and Cappuccio, J. D.
(1989). The analytic hierarchy process in medi-
cal decision making: a tutorial. Medical Deci-
sion Making, 9(1):40–50. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0272989X8900900108.
Edelsbrunner, P. A. (2022). A model and its fit lie in the eye
of the beholder: Long live the sum score. Frontiers
in Psychology, 13:986767. https://doi.org/10.3389/
fpsyg.2022.986767.
Folger, R. and Cropanzano, R. (2001). Fairness theory: Jus-
tice as accountability. In Greenberg, J. and Cropan-
zano, R., editors, Advances in Organizational Justice,
volume 1, pages 1–55. Stanford University Press.
Folger, R. and Cropanzano, R. (2011). Social hierarchies
and the evolution of moral emotions. In Schminke,
M., editor, Managerial Ethics, pages 225–252. Rout-
ledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203852460.
Folger, R. and Shukla, J. (2019). A fairness theory up-
date. In Lind, E. A., editor, Social Psychology and
Justice, pages 110–133. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.
4324/9780203852460.
Folger, R. G., Folger, R., and Cropanzano, R. (1998). Orga-
nizational Justice and Human Resource Management,
volume 7. Sage.
Franke, T., Attig, C., and Wessel, D. (2019). A Per-
sonal Resource for Technology Interaction: Devel-
opment and Validation of the Affinity for Technol-
ogy Interaction (ATI) Scale. International Journal
of Human-Computer Interaction, 35(6):456 467.
10.1080/10447318.2018.1456150.
Ganegoda, D. B. and Folger, R. (2015). Framing effects
in justice perceptions: Prospect theory and counter-
factuals. Organizational Behavior and Human Deci-
sion Processes, 126:27–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
obhdp.2014.10.002.
Human Rights Watch (2018). The eu general data protec-
tion regulation. https://www.hrw.org/news/2018/06/
06/eu-general-data-protection-regulation.
Kilwinger, F. B. and van Dam, Y. K. (2021). Methodolog-
ical considerations on the means-end chain analysis
revisited. Psychology & Marketing, 38(9):1513–1524.
https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.21521.
Masur, P. K., Teutsch, D., and Trepte, S. (2017).
Entwicklung und validierung der online-
privatheitskompetenzskala (OPLIS). Diagnostica.
McDonald, A. M. and Cranor, L. F. (2008). The cost of
reading privacy policies. Isjlp, 4:543.
Nicklin, J. M., Greenbaum, R., McNall, L. A., Folger, R.,
and Williams, K. J. (2011). The importance of con-
textual variables when judging fairness: An exam-
ination of counterfactual thoughts and fairness the-
ory. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
Processes, 114(2):127–141. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
obhdp.2010.10.007.
Parfenova, D., Niftulaeva, A., and Carr, C. T. (2024).
Words, words, words: participants do not read con-
sent forms in communication research. Communica-
tion Research Reports, pages 1–11.
Seabright, P. (2010). The company of strangers. Princeton
University Press.
Solove, D. J. (2012). Introduction: Privacy self-
management and the consent dilemma. Harv. L. Rev.,
126:1880.
Trepte, S., Teutsch, D., Masur, P. K., Eicher, C., Fischer,
M., Hennh
¨
ofer, A., and Lind, F. (2015). Do peo-
ple know about privacy and data protection strate-
gies? Towards the “Online Privacy Literacy Scale”
(OPLIS). In Gutwirth, S., Leenes, R., and de Hert,
P., editors, Reforming European data protection law,
pages 333–365. Springer.
Van Schaik, P., Irons, A., and Renaud, K. (2024). Privacy in
uk police digital forensics investigations. In HICSS,
pages 1901–1910.
ICISSP 2025 - 11th International Conference on Information Systems Security and Privacy
140