4
DISCUSSION
Both groups improved their average time to succeed in
reaching the goals in the training sets within the days.
However, the success in the training sets for the CVF
group was faster on average although the differences
are not significant. It seems that continuous visual
feedback helped reaching the goals faster although
differences were not significant. This criterion corres-
ponds more to a training effect than a learning effect.
Based on the results from the training sets, the
decrease in the time to reach the correct target could
lead to the assumption that there was an improvement
in the CVF compared to the NCVF.
However, if we now look at the results from the
learning control sets, this does not appear to be
transferable. Since a countdown of three seconds was
displayed in the training sets when the bar was in the
correct area, the sense of timing could be practiced.
Except of day 2, the CVF group spent on average
more than 3 seconds in the correct area. The NCVF
group spent on average very close to three seconds in
the correct range on all three days. This could lead to
the assumption that the CVF group was more
uncertain or more cautious in the execution of the task
which could be a sign of stronger dependency on
feedback. Moreover, the time required to reach the
longest period of continuous time spent at the correct
target decreased in the NCVF group. In the CVF
group, this time increased on the third day compared
to day 1. On the other hand, the number of missed
targets was lower in the CVF group than in the NCVF
group. While some criteria that were defined as motor
learning for the learning control sets were fulfilled by
both of the groups the observed results appear to be
controversial. One could assume that the CVF group
was more insecure in the learning control sets, which
is why they needed more time to reach the targets and
stayed longer in the targets to make sure they had
reached them long enough. Nevertheless, fewer goals
were missed in the CVF group. The observed
improvement in the time to achieve the goals in the
learning control sets for the NCVF group as well as
the more exact duration in the correct goal compared
to the CVF group is consistent with results from
previous studies, which showed that a greater learning
effect occurs with non-continuous feedback
compared to continuous feedback (Marco- Ahulló, et
al. 2024; Sullivan, Kantak und Burtner 2008;
Sülzenbrück und Heuer, 2011).
Nevertheless, the results of our study must be
interpreted with caution, as our results are not
significant and some are controverse. To be able to
make a clear statement regarding the guidance
hypothesis, it would be useful to carry out more than
three training days and more test subjects to see
whether more of the defined criteria for motor
learning are fulfilled and whether the found
differences prove to become significant with a larger
number of subjects.
5
CONCLUSION
Both groups fulfill predefined criteria for motor
learning, even though the results are not statistically
significant. Since some of the results are
controversial, extending the investigation to more
than three test days and including additional subjects
would be beneficial in order to provide a clearer
statement regarding the guidance hypothesis.
ETHICS STATEMENT
The studies involving human participants were
approved by the local Ethical Committee of the
University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany. The
participants provided their written informed consent
to participate in this study.
REFERENCES
Fuchs, Dominik, Martin Knauer, and Petra Friedrich.
„Chancen von akustischem Feedback für die
motorische Rehabilitation am Beispiel.“ DAGA.
München, 2018.
Goodwin, Jeff E. "Scheduling Concurrent Visual Feedback
in Learning a Continuous Balance Task." Journal of
Motor Learning a Development, 2019.
Habenicht, Julia, and Elsa Andrea Kirchner. „Preliminary
Results on the Evaluation of Different Feedback
Methods for the Operation of a Muscle-Controlled
Serious Game.“ 17th International Conference on Bio-
inspired Systems and Signal Processing. Rom, 2024.
Hermens, H J, B Freriks, C Disselhorst-Klug, and G Rau.
„Development of recommendations for SEMG sensors
and sensor placement procedure.“ Journal of
Electromyography and Kinesiology, October 2000.
Marco-Ahulló, Adria, Israel Villarrasa-Sapiña, Jorge
Romero-Martínez, Gonzalo Monfort-Torres, Jose Luis
Toca-Herrera, und Xavier García-Massó. „Effect of
Reduced Feedback Frequencies on Motor Learning in a
Postural Control Task in Young Adults.“ Sensors,
2024.
MathWorks.. https://de.mathworks.com/help/dsp/ug/sliding-
window-method-and-exponential-weighting-method.ht
ml (Last access: March 2024).