Our Notes Leave too Much to Say: Investigating Note-Taking Practices
and Technological Tools in Academia
Yannic Jäckel
a
, Daniel Schiffner
b
and Jan Schneider
c
DIPF, Leibniz-Institut für Bildungsforschung und Bildungsinformation,
Rostocker Straße 6, 60323 Frankfurt am Main, Germany
Keywords:
Note-Taking, Academic Writing, Knowledge Management, Information Gathering, Critical Reading.
Abstract:
This paper explores the role of note-taking as a critical, yet under-researched, practice in academic scholarship,
focusing on how researchers organize, synthesize, and reuse their notes in the context of knowledge produc-
tion. While previous technological advancements such as large language models (LLMs) have transformed
aspects of academic writing, fundamental cognitive tasks—such as capturing and synthesizing information
through note-taking—remain relatively unchanged. Drawing from existing research, we highlight how poor
or ineffective note-taking practices in both students and early-career researchers can lead to inefficient work
processes and diminished synthesis of knowledge. Our study involves a small-scale survey of academic re-
searchers to examine their note-taking techniques, tool usage, and strategies for synthesis. Our findings reveal
that many researchers employ unstructured methods, such as the Sentence and Outline Methods, and lack
formal training in effective note-taking. Furthermore, despite the availability of advanced digital tools, most
participants continue to rely on familiar word processors, often limiting the reusability and efficacy of their
notes. We argue that structured methods and better tool utilization could significantly enhance academic
writing and synthesis. The paper suggests that future research should focus on developing note-taking tools
tailored to researchers’ needs, enabling more effective synthesis and the reuse of notes. Such tools could po-
tentially integrate with LLMs to reduce the time spent on repetitive tasks and improve the quality of scientific
output. This shift could lead to a paradigm where notes evolve from simple memory aids to valuable data that
contributes directly to scientific advancement.
1 INTRODUCTION
Research and scientific advancement further the col-
lective understanding of phenomena in any given so-
ciety, writes Michel Foucault in his famous work "The
Order of Things" (Foucault, 2020). Karl Popper ar-
gues that what researchers do is make statements on
phenomena in the world(Popper, 2010). Thomas S.
Kuhn enters prior knowledge into the mix in argu-
ing that researchers need to first acquire a Paradigm
through study and understanding of textbooks, before
being able to conduct science (Kuhn and Hacking,
2012). Methods, axioms and influential prior research
need to be known in any given field, before one can
become a researcher oneself.
a
https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8065-5768
b
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0794-0359
c
https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8578-6409
The abstract was written with support of ChatGPT 3 on
the basis of the Introduction, Discussion and Conclusion of
this paper.
If we consider the education of new scholars, re-
searchers and scientists, we see these arguments in
practise. Students learn from lectures, discussions,
presentations, seminars and a plethora of other for-
mats. They learn from the written and spoken word
of their peers and teachers. But the delivery of knowl-
edge is not the most crucial part of learning. In
following Bloom’s taxonomy of learning, we argue
that in order to create scientific advancement, a re-
searcher must first remember, understand, apply, anal-
yse and evaluate knowledge, they acquired previously
through reading, discussion or other means (Krath-
wohl, 2002).
To facilitate these steps, we derive that synthesis
through notes, can be considered a major factor in
prior learning, much in alignment with Kuhn’s ob-
servations on how researchers acquire their Paradigm
(Kuhn and Hacking, 2012). Note-taking can act as
a second brain or "external storage" (Mosleh and
Baba, 2013; Pitura, 2023; Mueller and Oppenheimer,
2014), which facilitates recollection in its simplest
Jäckel, Y., Schiffner, D. and Schneider, J.
Our Notes Leave too Much to Say: Investigating Note-Taking Practices and Technological Tools in Academia.
DOI: 10.5220/0013218600003932
Paper published under CC license (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0)
In Proceedings of the 17th International Conference on Computer Supported Education (CSEDU 2025) - Volume 2, pages 493-503
ISBN: 978-989-758-746-7; ISSN: 2184-5026
Proceedings Copyright © 2025 by SCITEPRESS Science and Technology Publications, Lda.
493
form. Notes can be analysed and revised, a process
also known as synthesis (Qian et al., 2020).
As for application, note-taking, should be consid-
ered an exercise in academic writing, which we con-
sider in itself to be among the most fundamental skills
any researcher or scholar will use in their career in
academia. However, with more and more capable
LLMs, a focus on the mere mass-production of sci-
entific publications, as we see today, can certainly be
considered obsolete.
Following from these arguments, we say that
notes are the de facto bedrock of any scientific work.
Qian et al. consider them to be building blocks in a
sense making process (Qian et al., 2020), which in
itself is, as argued, fundamental to science.
However, from anecdotal evidence, we observed
that not many researchers seem to be trained in note-
taking. In this paper, we want to provide a baseline
and explore how researchers in academia take and
use their notes, with a specific focus on the employed
note-taking techniques and strategies.
1.1 Note-Taking as an Integral Part of
Research and Synthesis
The practise of note-taking itself is age-old and be-
gan, according to
´
Sl˛ezak-
´
Swiat, with annotations in
margins to better interpret a text. While the prac-
tise of these in-source annotations can be traced as
far back as 500CE (cf. (
´
Sl˛ezak
´
Swiat, 2022)), dig-
ital note-taking, according to Joanna Pitura’s recent
overview on the subject, started with the use of word-
processors and developed into specific note-taking
software like ’Evernote’ or ’RoamResearch’ (Pitura,
2023). Notes and marginalia can be found to be so en-
compassing that they can even serve as publications of
their own. One such example is Wittgenstein’s "Über
Gewißheit" (On Certainty), which he could not finish
before his death in 1951 (Wittgenstein, 2020). It was
nevertheless published and, despite its fractured ap-
pearance, it allows an interesting view into his thought
process and renders them as understandable as other
scientific publications. The same happened to J.R.R.
Tolkien’s work The Silmarillion, which was also pub-
lished unfinished after his death.
While the publication of notes, may as in
Wittgenstein’s case lack, a well-structured reading
experience, the practise of note-taking offers many
benefits, even beyond scholarly fields, as note-taking
aids in decision-making and problem-solving. It also
improves (academic) writing and information reten-
tion (Deniozou et al., 2020; Pitura, 2023;
´
Sl˛ezak
´
Swiat, 2022). It does so mostly through the process
of encoding knowledge, i.e. writing down informa-
tion in your own words, often by hand (
´
Sl˛ezak
´
Swiat,
2022; Pitura, 2023; Mosleh and Baba, 2013; Mueller
and Oppenheimer, 2014)). Qian et al. argue that
note-taking, is a scholarly primitive. It forms the ba-
sis of synthesis, which itself is the basis of produc-
ing new knowledge and scientific advancement from
previous sources (Qian et al., 2020). Note-taking is
part of a (scientific) discourse (
´
Sl˛ezak
´
Swiat, 2022),
notes are purpose-driven if used for scientific writ-
ing, and should not be mere documentation (Fix and
Dittmann, 2008; Pitura, 2023). Effective note-taking
can speed up the process of scientific writing tremen-
dously. Fix and Dittmann argue that well-executed
notes, will allow for the construction of a text without
reading the corresponding primary sources again (Fix
and Dittmann, 2008), thus providing an ever-growing
collection of building blocks for further construction
of texts that pertain to the same discourse.
In practise, however, Qian et al. found that their
participants often had to return to their sources, cor-
recting misunderstood parts in their notes, or find-
ing their notes inadequate (Qian et al., 2020), indi-
cating less well-executed notes. The fact that other
studies found students to be having trouble taking ef-
fective (especially digital) notes (Fix and Dittmann,
2008;
´
Sl˛ezak
´
Swiat, 2022; Mueller and Oppenheimer,
2014), indicates a greater, systemic issue.
In 2024, Kathleen Carroll provided an overview of
the state of research on the benefits of note-taking and
what cognitive functions it applies in students (Car-
roll, 2024). Concerning digital note-taking, specifi-
cally, Joanna Pitura published an overview in 2023.
It provides a comprehensive study on (digital) note-
taking and tool usage in students and early career re-
searchers. She analysed the benefits and usage of spe-
cific tools and techniques in use by her target group
(Pitura, 2023).
Touching on a similar subject, Mueller and Op-
penheimer conducted a study in 2014, in which they
analysed whether students performed better at tak-
ing lecture notes using a laptop or long form writ-
ing (Mueller and Oppenheimer, 2014). They found
that students using a laptop were prone to transcrib-
ing part of the lecture verbatim, which lead to ineffec-
tive notes and poor recollection (Mueller and Oppen-
heimer, 2014). Mueller and Oppenheimer argue that
due to laptops allowing for more words to be writ-
ten in a given time span, they encourage ineffective
note-taking (Mueller and Oppenheimer, 2014). Ver-
bal interventions proved ineffective in mitigating the
behaviour (Mueller and Oppenheimer, 2014). How-
ever, we argue that a lack of training on on-the-spot
sythesis may also adequatly explain the lack of per-
formance, as opposed to the use of technology itself.
CSEDU 2025 - 17th International Conference on Computer Supported Education
494
Long form writing necessitates this synthesis due to
the constraints Mueller and Oppenheimer identify. If
students were properly trained beforehand to treat a
laptop the same way as they would pen and paper,
they may not have fallen for the temptation of mind-
lessly transcribing. Umberto Eco mentions a similar
effect in 1977, when he reminds his readers to not
simply photocopy texts and then consider them read
(Eco, 2015). Reliance on a photocopier alone, instead
of reading and synthesising, leads to poor understand-
ing of the subject and poor performance in writing.
While note-taking and its apparent benefits are
well understood in the work of students, research con-
cerning note-taking as a scholarly practise in aca-
demic work is poorly examined and its influence
on synthesis and knowledge production is under-
researched (Qian et al., 2020; Carroll, 2024; Pitura,
2023; Deniozou et al., 2020), (Mosleh and Baba,
2013), (Pitura, 2023). Qian et al. performed in 2020
a small-scale qualitative study on first-year PhD stu-
dents in their research group, where they researched
how their participants performed sense-making and
synthesis (Qian et al., 2020). In the process of this
study, some insights were gained on how the partic-
ipants used notes and digital tools to facilitate syn-
thesis. However, their conclusion was the same that
was hinted at earlier in the introduction. Their par-
ticipants repeatedly had to return to their sources to
correct their notes, rendering them ineffective (Qian
et al., 2020). They also identified a lack of tools and
proper training (Qian et al., 2020).
The research presented suggests that issues con-
cerning poorly executed notes present themselves
in students (Mueller and Oppenheimer, 2014) and
researchers alike (Qian et al., 2020), suggesting a
knock-on effect from poor or ineffective training.
1.2 Contribution
The contribution of this paper is to understand how
note-taking is done in academia and explore whether
the systematic issues observed with students are also
presented with our target group. To facilitate this,
we undertook a small-scale survey to achieve a base-
line understanding of the situation. We aim to un-
derstand how notes are used among our participants.
We also aim to identify how note-taking strategies
are employed and how structured the process of note-
taking is in our participant’s day-to-day work. We
also aim to corroborate our findings with findings
from previous research. In doing so, we aim to fur-
ther increase the understanding of systematic issues
that present themselves in the practise of note-taking
in researchers and scholars and, if issues present it-
self, offer ideas on how they could be solved in further
research.
2 SURVEY
To see, whether any of the aforementioned issues
were present or prevalent in Academic research, we
conducted a survey in note-taking techniques and
strategies.
The survey (see Appendix for the survey ques-
tions) was inspired by an overview on note-taking
methods and strategies by Mosleh and Baba, pub-
lished in 2013 (Mosleh and Baba, 2013). As they
implied that these techniques were widely used in
academia, we deemed them a good selection for our
survey. Furthermore, they were the only encom-
passing overview we found. The methods outlined
by Mosleh and Baba can also be found in multiple
educational web-portals, such as for example https:
//e-student.org.
The techniques and strategies, Mosleh and Baba
provide, range from less structured methods like the
Sentence Method, or Outline Method where note-
takers write their thoughts and other information as
unstructured sentences, or as indented sentences be-
low a topic respectively, to well-structured, tabular
methods like Two-Column-Reading or the Cornell
Method, where note-takers write down notes, cues
and reasoning in specific columns or partitions on a
page. (Mosleh and Baba, 2013).
In Two-Column Reading, note-takers split the
page into two columns. One of them contains the in-
formation taken from the currently read text, while
the other contains thoughts and reasoning on the note
taken ((Mosleh and Baba, 2013)).
The Cornell Method structures notes in three
columns, containing a cue word, the thoughts of a
note-taker when the cue was encountered and a sum-
mary of the page of notes, is also mentioned by Pitura
as an example for digital note-taking (Pitura, 2023).
Another well-structured method we selected was
not in Mosleh and Baba’s overview. In Four Column
Reading, note-takers split a page into four columns.
One contains a quote or paraphrase the note-taker has
encountered, the second column contains the number
of the page, where the quote or paraphrase was taken
from. The third column contains the reasoning of the
note-taker, as to why they selected the quote or para-
phrase and how it pertains to their research question.
A fourth column contains information on where the
specific information is to be used in an article or other
form of scientific text (Király et al., 2023).
Other structured methods also include the REAP-
Our Notes Leave too Much to Say: Investigating Note-Taking Practices and Technological Tools in Academia
495
method (Mosleh and Baba, 2013), where information
is read, encoded, annotated and pondered, i.e. notes
are taken (encoded), collated in any way (annotated)
and synthesised (pondered). Furthermore, mapping
methods, such as mind maps and charting methods,
where information is encoded into a table or a spread-
sheet (Mosleh and Baba, 2013) can be considered
structured as well.
The survey was designed with 20 mostly yes/no
or multiple choice questions (see Appendix). Only a
few questions allowed for open-ended ’other’ options.
The answers to the open-ended options were encoded
by hand for analysis. The language chosen for the
survey was English.
The survey was hosted on a LimeSurvey platform,
hosted by a German research institute. Participants
were invited using Messengers, Chatrooms, Social
Media and E-Mails. It was not recorded where par-
ticipants currently work. This was considered of lit-
tle interest, and would possibly have allowed for the
identification of individuals. No personal data was
recorded, rendering the survey anonymous.
2.1 Findings
The target audience of our study was considered to be
active researchers in academia. The link to the survey
was distributed using emails and Social media chan-
nels. In total 33 participants completed the question-
naire.
To better understand where participants were in
their academic career, we asked their current position
(Q. 1). Three options were given: Predoc, i.e. PhD
candidates, graduate students and the like, Postdoc
and Professor. To better identify cohorts, these terms
will be used from this point on to refer to the respec-
tive groups outlined in 1
Table 1: Career stages of participants.
Career stage Number of participants in cohort
Predoc 20
Postdoc 9
Professor 4
Total 33
We hoped to get a better understanding of note-
taking techniques in specific fields, so we asked par-
ticipants in what academic field they currently work
(Q. 3). The results were distributed with a long tail,
thus not usable for statistic evaluation, beyond ob-
serving what fields were present. The two biggest co-
horts were Computer Science (N=6) and Psychology
(N=9). However, the spread included a wide range of
academic fields such as Art History, History, Archival
studies, Linguistics and Education among others.
Since we also wanted to allow a wider, interna-
tional group to participate. Our participants were
mostly of German nationals, or persons having re-
ceived their academic education in Germany. Thus,
the question concerning where academic education
was received (Q. 2) did not yield any valuable in-
sights.
2.1.1 Reading and Note-Taking Strategies
The question most relevant to the goal of this paper
was what reading and note-taking strategies were em-
ployed by participants and gave the aforementioned
techniques as options. (Q. 11) Since we expected
multiple techniques to be employed by a single par-
ticipant, multiple answers were allowed. ’None of the
Above’ excluded all other answers. We found that
overall, none of the very structured methods were em-
ployed widely.
In Predocs (N=20), only three methods could be
considered popular. These were the Outline- (N=7),
Mapping- (N=5) and the Sentence Method (N=4).
Postdocs (N=9) used slightly more structured
methods in using Outline Method (N=2), Two-
Column Reading (N=2) and the Sentence Method
(N=3).
Among Professors (N=4) no real trend could
be observed, but Outline Method (N=2), Mapping
Method (N=2) and Sentence Method (N=2) were the
most prevalent.
Among all groups, ’None of the above’
(N
Predocs
=8, N
Postdocs
=2, N
Prof essors
=1), was used
frequently as well. This indicates that a not insignifi-
cant portion of our participants do not use, or do not
know of, the techniques we proposed as answers.
2.1.2 Digital vs. Print, Screen Inferiority
Since previous research indicated a strong screen in-
feriority effect for academic reading and note-taking
(
´
Sl˛ezak
´
Swiat, 2022), we decided to include questions
on reading habits. When asked about whether par-
ticipants predominantly read their sources in print or
on screen (Q. 5), most participants, through all co-
horts, stated that they read their academic texts ei-
ther on screen (N
Predocs
=8, N
Postdocs
=3, N
Prof essors
=2)
or both on screen and paper, or they could not
say (N
Predocs
=6, N
Postdocs
=4, N
Prof essors
=2). Only
a minority stated to read on print-outs (N
Predocs
=6,
N
Postdocs
=6, N
Prof essors
=0).
Furthermore, we asked whether participants pre-
ferred handwritten notes or used word-processors or
wrote notes directly in text (annotations) or used ded-
icated note-taking software. (Q. 6). The use of word
CSEDU 2025 - 17th International Conference on Computer Supported Education
496
processors and handwritten (long form) notes is most
prevalent, as can be seen in table 2.
Table 2: Distribution of note-taking habits.
Predoc Postdoc Professor
Handwritten
notes
13 6 3
Word proces-
sor
13 5 2
Annotations 5 4 1
Dedicated
Software
1 2 1
As a follow-up question, we asked participants
whether they preferred handwritten notes on paper, or
took them using digital means (Q. 7). Almost all par-
ticipants, through all cohorts, stated they made hand-
written notes on paper. Even those who made hand-
written notes digitally, also made paper notes (3).
Table 3: Distribution of handwritten notes on paper vs. on
screen (Multiple Answers allowed).
Predoc Postdoc Professor
Handwritten
notes on
paper
12 5 2
Handwritten
notes on
screen
7 2 2
Both 6 1 2
Total 13 6 3
We also asked whether annotations (notes directly
in the source material) were used and whether they
were taken on paper or digitally (Q. 9). The results
can be seen in table 4
Table 4: Distribution of annotations on paper vs. on screen.
Predoc Postdoc Professor
Annotations
on paper
3 2 1
Annotations
on screen
4 3 1
Both 1 1 1
Total 5 4 1
2.1.3 Tool Usage
Among our participants, dedicated note-taking soft-
ware was only widely used in the professor (N=2)
cohort. Among Predocs (N=1), and Postdocs(N=2),
dedicated note-taking software was not widely used.
(See table 2).
The software in use was Evernote (N=2), Obsidian
(N=2), Roam Research/LogSeq (N=2) and Google
Keep (N=1).
Overall, the most widely used tool were word pro-
cessors. Among them, Microsoft Word (N=13) was
the most commonly used, with the Google Suite be-
ing a distant second (N=3).
2.1.4 Quality of Notes
To assess the quality of notes that our participants
took, the question was posed whether participants did
publish their notes (Q. 16). The reasoning behind
this question was that if participants published their
notes, we could assume that they had faith in their
notes and would hold them to a high quality standard.
Furthermore, we assumed that researchers who pub-
lished their notes considered them as research data to
be reused and shared. The vast majority (N=29) did
not publish their notes, with only a very small number
(N=4) publishing them. (see table 5)
Table 5: Question: Do you publish your notes?
Predoc Postdoc Professor
Yes 3 1 0
No 17 8 4
To further assess the perception of the quality of
the notes participants took, we asked for reasons why
the participants choose not to publish their notes (Q.
18). Since multiple of the offered reasons might ap-
ply, multiple answers were allowed. As seen in table
6, most participants considered their notes to be pri-
vate. However, quite a large number of participants
(N=13) voiced concerns that their notes may (also)
contain errors. Only a small minority stated that their
notes were useless due to being idiosyncratic (N=4).
Interestingly, a small number of participants stated
that they did not publish their notes because they may
contain cues for further research by others.
Table 6: Reasons why notes would not be published.
Predoc Postdoc Professor
My notes are
private
13 6 3
My notes may
contain errors
7 3 3
They may
contain cues
for further
research
3 1 1
They would not
make sense to
others
2 2 0
Our Notes Leave too Much to Say: Investigating Note-Taking Practices and Technological Tools in Academia
497
2.1.5 Note Usage
Since reasoning and other meta-information may be
encoded with different schemas, we also asked if
note-takers wrote their own reasoning, reading strat-
egy and mental disposition (Qs. 12–14). Király et
al. argue that they all can influence the note-takers
perception and argumentation during critical reading
(Király et al., 2023).
We also wanted to evaluate how our participants
used their notes in their writing process (Q. 19). Since
notes can be used as building blocks to scientific texts
or as memory aids, we posed whether our participants
used them as such. We also asked if participants in-
cluded their own reasoning in their notes. Since not
many participants employed structured note-taking
techniques that included reasoning by default (N=2),
this question is of particular interest.
The majority of participants (N=23), through all
cohorts, claimed to include their own reasoning in
their notes, with only a minority not including their
reasoning in their notes (N
Predocs
=7, N
Postdocs
=1).
Among these participants, only the Mapping
Method (N=1) was used as an example of a structured
note-taking method. The remaining distribution of
techniques used by participants who, do not include
their reasoning, can be seen in table 7. None of the
methods compel the note-taker to include reasoning.
Table 7: Note-taking methods employed by participants not
including their own reasoning.
Predoc Postdoc
Outline Method 1 1
Mapping Method 1 0
Sentence Method 3 1
None of the above 3 0
Furthermore, participants were asked if they used
their notes as memory aids or text snippets. Since
both usages may coexist, multiple answers were al-
lowed. The category ’both’ is calculated from the an-
swers, to allow for better analysis. An open-ended
option was given, but no comment was provided by
most participants who choose it. These answers were
ignored as invalid (N
Predocs
=1, N
Postdocs
=1). One an-
swer was given as ’I use my notes as outlines’, which
we encoded as ’text snippet’ (See table 8).
The Predocs were the only cohort to show a wide-
spread usage of notes as text snippets or both snippet
and memory aid (N=14). Postdocs, used their notes
mainly as memory aids (N=6). Among professors,
usage as a memory aid was dominant (N=2).
Table 8: Note usage.
Predoc Postdoc Professor
Cohort size 20 9 4
Use as snippets
(only)
4 0 1
Use as memory
aid (only)
6 6 1
Both 10 2 1
Other (invalid) 1 1 0
2.1.6 Training
We wanted to explore whether participants have re-
ceived training in note-taking (Q. 15). The results
show that, through all cohorts, most participants
stated that they either received no training or were
self-taught (N
Predocs
=18, N
Postdocs
=7, N
Prof essors
=4),
which could be considered the same in the context
of this question. Only the minority of participants
stated to have received training in school or univer-
sity (N
Predocs
=7, N
Postdocs
=6, N
Prof essors
=0). For a de-
tailed overview, see table 9.
Table 9: Note-taking training received.
Predoc Postdoc Professor
Cohort size 20 9 4
School 3 3 0
University 4 3 0
Self-taught 6 5 1
I did not
receive any
training
12 2 3
2.2 Discussion of Findings
Among our participants, none of the structured meth-
ods that Mosleh and Baba claimed to be widely used
(Mosleh and Baba, 2013) were found to be preva-
lent. Our participants tended to prefer unstructured
methods, that encoded information without (Sentence
Method) or with simple, hierarchical structures (Out-
line Method). While Mapping Methods, such as mind
maps, were the most structured method used in our
participants.
Furthermore, we observed that only a minority of
participants (N=10 out of 33) annotated their sources,
indicating a single step reading process, which is fur-
ther correlated by the absence of the REAP-Method.
More critically, our findings show a lack of formal
training, aligned with a conclusion of Fix and Dittman
made in 2008 (Fix and Dittmann, 2008). Similarly,
´
Sl˛ezak-
´
Swiat’s research in 2022 suggested that her
participants, Polish E2L students, had little knowl-
CSEDU 2025 - 17th International Conference on Computer Supported Education
498
edge about structured note-taking techniques and no
formal training. Thus, they relied on their intuition
(
´
Sl˛ezak
´
Swiat, 2022). The studies done by Qian et
al. and Mueller and Oppenheimer further support this
notion (Qian et al., 2020; Mueller and Oppenheimer,
2014). As outlined above, the findings of Mueller and
Oppenheimer can well be explained by a lack of train-
ing, rather than by the use of technology.
This correlates well with our findings, since the
Sentence- and Outline Methods can be considered in-
tuitive, as they do not rely on any structure that would
necessitate training and knowledge. Thus, based on
our results, we consider that more training is needed
to get more structured and reusable notes.
Taking into account that Fix and Dittmann ob-
served a lack of training as early as 2008 (Fix and
Dittmann, 2008), the deficit we perceive has persisted
for quite some time. Any lack of training and knowl-
edge we observe today, we consider to be knock-on
effects of prior insufficient training, since the students
of 2008 may well be teachers today.
Since a larger number of our participants con-
sider themselves self-taught (N=12), we could assume
that they read training resources themselves. There
are more recent training resources like, for instance,
Király et al.s ’Jump-Start Your Writing’ (Király et al.,
2023), or even older manuals like Umberto Eco’s
’How to write a thesis’, originally published in 1977
and translated into English in 2015 (Eco, 2015) that
show usages of more structured techniques. Király et
al. propose four-column-reading (Király et al., 2023),
while Eco, being an older resource, does not include
computerised methods. He proposes the use of index
cards and files in filing cabinets (Eco, 2015), a tech-
nique that is still employed by some researchers today
(McCarty, 2023). This list is by no means exhaustive.
However, since we observed a near absence of
structured methods present in teaching materials, we
can assume that our participants, while considering
themselves self-taught, did not consult the these ma-
terials, or did not feel the need to do so. This raises
the question if the notion of self-teaching rather more
refers to ’make it up as you go’, than actual course-
work.
While structured methods for note-taking are not
part of the daily work of our participants, we found
that a sense of efficacy in note-taking was well rep-
resented. Our findings suggest that the new gener-
ation of researchers, represented by our Predocs co-
hort, use their notes as text snippets to a significant
degree, whereas in our Postdocs cohort they were still
mainly used as memory aids.
The use of notes as parts of a new text is one of the
key concepts of Four-Column-Reading, and we argue
that it shows a concern of efficacy and reusability in
note-taking. It also indicates that a degree of synthesis
that must have been taken place, if notes were consid-
ered to be incorporated into a publication. This is fur-
ther corroborated by the fact that the vast majority of
our participants included their own reasoning in their
notes. While the lack of training may still present is-
sues, we argue that it could be offset by the use of
tools that are specifically designed to reevaluate, syn-
thesise and manage notes in ways described by, for
example, Pitura, Eco, or McCarty (Pitura, 2023; Eco,
2015; McCarty, 2023). We also argue that the neces-
sary structure for reuse may also be introduced by the
use of specific tools.
Tool usage in note-taking, however, also shows
signs of knock-on effects that can be traced to a
lack of formal training in students and teachers alike.
While there is a plethora of dedicated note-taking
tools (RoamResearch, Evernote, LogSeq, Obsidian,
Google Keep, Notion.io, Zettelkasten
1
to name a
few), their use did not present itself in our findings.
We assume our participants used the tools they are
familiar with, which are mainly word-processors in
the form of Microsoft Word. Its use is usually taught
in schools, and it is omnipresent. This aligns well
with findings from Qian et al., who also observed that
their participants did not use dedicated note-taking
tools. Their participants often showed a ’make-do’
attitude in adapting known tools to a use they were
never designed for and which thus offered sub-par
performance on the task at hand. One participant
went as far as using a built-in sticky-note tool in their
computer’s operating system to take their notes (Qian
et al., 2020). While their participants clearly sought
and found creative ways to employ their known tools,
Qian et al. found it to be a source of friction, with
researchers switching between up to four (adapted)
tools to perform their daily work (Qian et al., 2020),
hampering their abilities.
Pitura, McCarty, Király et al. and Eco present in
their works methods that could reasonably be com-
puterized and in the case of ’Zettelkasten’ are even
available as finished product. But knowledge of these
tools did not present itself in our findings. To establish
specific reasons, further research would be required,
though we assume from anecdotal evidence that tools
might simply be unknown.
Another indicator that correlates well with a lack
of formal training is what we perceive as the employ-
ment of a single pass reading strategy. We deduce this
from the lack of annotations our participants make in
their sources. This aligns well with research done by
´
Sl˛ezak-
´
Swiat in 2022, where she found that while her
1
http://zettelkasten.danielluedecke.de/
Our Notes Leave too Much to Say: Investigating Note-Taking Practices and Technological Tools in Academia
499
participants still read on paper, they did not annotate
their printouts (
´
Sl˛ezak
´
Swiat, 2022). While
´
Sl˛ezak-
´
Swiat interpreted this as a strong screen inferiority ef-
fect in reading (
´
Sl˛ezak
´
Swiat, 2022), we argue that it
may also be a symptom of a lack of formal training.
Resources like Király et al. teach a multistep read-
ing process while taking notes (Király et al., 2023),
whereas Eco advises to underline and annotate im-
portant parts on a photocopy(Eco, 2015). The lack of
such annotations in a source may indicate a single-
step reading process, which would align well with
the observation that participants use intuitive meth-
ods. However, to ascertain this, more research would
be required.
To establish a measure on how our participants
perceived their own notes, we asked if they published
their notes. Our participants generally answered they
did not. They mostly considered their notes pri-
vate or containing errors as a reason for not pub-
lishing. Previous research suggested that note-takers
mainly perceived their notes to be unintelligible to
others (Mosleh and Baba, 2013; Fix and Dittmann,
2008). Our findings differ concerning on this percep-
tion. While unintelligibility may be a reason for keep-
ing notes private, ’being wrong’ seemed to be a big-
ger concern to our participants compared to previous
studies.
As a caveat, it has to be stated that our sur-
vey’s participants consisted of a rather small group
of mostly German academics. As stated above, our
sample consists of a wide range of research fields, all
of whom showed similar answers. As such, the con-
text of the education did not seem to matter, and the
issues highlighted in this paper persist in a variety of
circumstances and fields. We furthermore found that
the issues were already persistent in 2008 in German
schools (Fix and Dittmann, 2008). And judging by
the examined research, we do not expect other coun-
tries to be different. The knock-on effects described
can be expected to manifest themselves in different
countries and similar circumstances as well.
3 CONCLUSION
In general, our findings suggest that our participants
do not use their notes to their full potential, even
though small changes in behaviour could be observed
to what was concluded in previous research. We ar-
gue that the main issue is a lack of teaching and train-
ing on how to take reusable and structured notes, and
a lack of knowledge on tools that would help in this
task. If available tools were simply inadequate, could
not be ascertained.
While we argue that training and knowledge can-
not be replaced by technology, we find that a dedi-
cated tool may help researchers in obtaining better
structured notes that can make synthesis easier and
more reliable.
The tool we envision, would need to be akin to
a ’Virtual Research Environment (VRE)’ or a Dash-
board, which would allow researchers and scholars
more efficient and effective ways to remember, un-
derstand, analyse, and evaluate their notes and pos-
sibly publish them as research data alongside their
publication, allowing for other researchers to inte-
grate them in their work. We argue that with a VRE
specifically designed to work through the act of note-
taking, we could employ Natural Language Process-
ing and other computerized methods, to make annota-
tions in new transcripts, notes or machine-actionable
texts that would allow researchers to see emerging
patterns in notes and sources alike. The ability to
annotate a researcher’s own notes, as if they were a
qualitative dataset, could also enhance further analy-
sis.
While this additional step may require a change in
habit, we argue that it will make notes more power-
ful and enhance the work of researchers, while they
mostly keep working in the way they are accustomed
to, using Outline- and Sentence Methods or mind
maps. Mind maps in turn can help researchers or-
ganize their thoughts, generate ideas, and develop
concepts, which enhances their analytical and under-
standing skills (Shi et al., 2023). Similarly, visual
programming’ could be employed to use the visual
aspect of mind maps to visualise steps in an analysis
with different algorithms connecting to one another.
At the time of writing, we are not aware of any tool
that would implement the functionalities we envision.
An encompassing VRE could also solve another
problem in data reuse. Because, even if notes are
taken using structured methods, they are currently
neither published nor reused. We assume that in writ-
ing, analysing and learning, researchers seldomly use
all the notes they made, rather they use a portion that
pertains to a specific topic or question. With the cur-
rent tools in use, we expect the rest to fall by the way-
side, lost in some word-document somewhere never
to be seen. With dedicated tools, these unused notes
can be exported, shared or be reused and synthesised
in another context. In this sense, note-taking sup-
ports learning and thus also research, through a con-
structivist approach to learning. This approach al-
lows note-takers to use their notes in different scenar-
ios and contexts, allowing for new connections and
a broader understanding (Friedman, 2014). Further-
more, since notes created within an encompassing
CSEDU 2025 - 17th International Conference on Computer Supported Education
500
VRE share a common mode of production, we argue
that these notes are as scientifically valuable as any
qualitative, incidental data.
Following this, notes themselves may even form a
new basis for publication.
´
Sl˛ezak-
´
Swiat argues that
notes, as a stand-alone publication, act as an indi-
cator to what texts a researcher may have read and
which information they did not deem necessary to
their process. Notes can thus act as an indicator of a
researcher’s attention and focus (
´
Sl˛ezak
´
Swiat, 2022).
While this aligns with our previous arguments, it can
directly be confirmed by reading "Über Gewißheit",
where Wittgenstein’s notes allow the reader a glimpse
into his thought process while examining a philosoph-
ical article, argument by argument. Also, a collection
of notes from different researchers might indicate the
attention and focus of an entire field, show a structural
bias or blind spots or illuminate a hitherto unknown
paradigm in the sense of Thomas S. Kuhn (Kuhn and
Hacking, 2012), or expose the episteme (Foucault,
2020) or ’Weltbild’ (Wittgenstein, 2020) of a group
of researchers.
As such, evaluation of available tools is still ongo-
ing and the reuse of notes and the perception of notes
as a form of research data is something that needs to
be studied more in-depth.
REFERENCES
Carroll, K. (2024). The Relationship Between Note tak-
ing, Revision, and Learning in Tertiary Education: A
Review of Literature. All Ireland Journal of Higher
Education, 16(1). Number: 1.
Deniozou, T., Dima, M., and Cox, C. (2020). Designing
a Game to Help Higher Education Students Develop
Their Note-Taking Skills. In Proceedings of the An-
nual Symposium on Computer-Human Interaction in
Play, CHI PLAY ’20, pages 181–192, New York, NY,
USA. Association for Computing Machinery.
Eco, U. (2015). How to Write a Thesis. The MIT Press,
Cambridge (Mass.) London, translation edition edi-
tion.
Fix, G. and Dittmann, J. (2008). Exzerpieren. Eine em-
pirische Studie an Exzerpten von Gymnasialschü-
lerInnen der Oberstufe. Linguistik Online, 33(1).
Number: 1.
Foucault, M. (2020). Die Ordnung der Dinge: eine
Archäologie der Humanwissenschaften. Number 96
in suhrkamp taschenbuch wissenschaft. Suhrkamp,
Frankfurt am Main, 26. auflage edition.
Friedman, M. C. (2014). Notes on note-taking: Review
of research and insights for students and instructors.
Harvard Initiative for Learning and Teaching, pages
1–34.
Király, R. S., Hösl, P., Beerbaum, T., and Samuelsson, U.
(2023). Jump-Start Your Writing : Tips and Methods
for Planning and Writing Academic Texts. Dresden
University of Technology, 2 edition.
Krathwohl, D. R. (2002). A Revision of Bloom’s Taxon-
omy: An Overview. Theory Into Practice, 41(4):212–
218.
Kuhn, T. S. and Hacking, I. (2012). The structure of sci-
entific revolutions. The University of Chicago Press,
Chicago ; London, fourth edition edition.
McCarty, W. (2023). Pursuing a combinatorial habit of
mind and machine. In Nyhan, J., Rockwell, G., Sin-
clair, S., and Ortolja-Baird, A., editors, On Making
in the Digital Humanities, The scholarship of digital
humanities development in honour of John Bradley,
pages 251–266. UCL Press.
Mosleh, M. and Baba, M. S. (2013). Overview of Tradi-
tional Note Taking. Educational Psychology Review.
Mueller, P. A. and Oppenheimer, D. M. (2014). The
Pen Is Mightier Than the Keyboard: Advantages
of Longhand Over Laptop Note Taking. Psy-
chological Science, 25(6):1159–1168. _eprint:
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614524581.
Pitura, J. (2023). Digital Note-Taking for Writing. Springer
International Publishing.
Popper, K. (2010). The logic of scientific discovery. Rout-
ledge, London, special indian edition edition.
Qian, X., Fenlon, K., Lutters, W., and Chan, J. (2020).
Opening up the black box of scholarly synthesis: In-
termediate products, processes, and tools. Proceed-
ings of the Association for Information Science and
Technology, 57(1):e270.
Shi, Y., Yang, H., Dou, Y., and Zeng, Y. (2023). Effects
of mind mapping-based instruction on student cogni-
tive learning outcomes: a meta-analysis. Asia Pacific
Education Review, 24(3):303–317.
Wittgenstein, L. (2020). Über Gewißheit. Number 250
in Bibliothek Suhrkamp. Suhrkamp Verlag, Frankfurt
am Main, 15. auflage edition.
´
Sl˛ezak
´
Swiat, A. M. (2022). Development of digital literacy
- translanguaging and transmedia note taking formats
for academic reading. Theory and Practice of Second
Language Acquisition, 8(1).
APPENDIX
Survey Questions
Q1: At what stage in your academic career are
you currently?
Answer options (Single Choice):
Student
PreDoc (PhD candidate, Post-grad-student)
PostDoc
Professor
I don’t/no longer work in academia
Our Notes Leave too Much to Say: Investigating Note-Taking Practices and Technological Tools in Academia
501
Q2: In what country did you receive your
academic education?
Answer options (Free text field)
Q3: What’s the scientific discipline you currently
work in? Or, if you don’t / no longer work in
academia, what’s the field you last worked in or
studied.
Answer options (Free text field)
Q4: Do you take notes?
Answer options (Single Choice)
Yes
No
Q5: Do you prefer to read material printed on
paper, or do you primarily read on a screen?
Answer options (Single Choice)
Print
Screen
Both/Can’t really say
Q6: How do you take your notes?
Answer options (Multiple Choice)
Handwritten
Word Processor / text editor
Notes directly in text
Dedicated note taking software
Other (i.e. Sketch notes, Mindmaps, Doodles etc.)
(Free text field)
Q7: Do you take your handwritten notes on paper
or do you take them digitally?
Answer options (Multiple Choice)
On Paper
Digitally
Q8:Which word processor or editor do you use?
Answer options (Multiple Choice)
Microsoft Word
LibreOffice / OpenOffice
SublimeText
Visual Studio Code
Apple Pages
Other (Free text field)
Q9: You write your notes directly into the text
you’re reading. Do you do so on paper or
digitally?
Answer options (Multiple Choice)
On Paper
Digitally
Q10: What dedicated note-taking software do you
use
Answer options (Free text field)
Q11: During note-taking, do you employ one or
more of these techniques or strategies?
Answer options (Multiple Choice)
Four Column Reading (Quote or Paraphrase, Page
No., Comment or Reasoning, Where to use)
Two Column (Notes and Notes-on-notes)
Cornell Method (Three columns: Cue, Notes and
Page-summary)
REAP Method (Read, Encode, Annotate, Ponder)
Outline Method (Indented bullet points, grouped
by overarching subjects)
Mapping Method (i.e. Mind Maps)
Charting Method (Tabular notes)
Sentence Method (Unstructured notes in full sen-
tences)
None of the above
2
Q12: Do you record your chosen reading strategy
in your notes?
Hint given: Reading strategies are for instance: Skim-
ming, Close Reading, Critical Reading and the like.
Answer options (Single Choice)
Yes
No
Q13: Do you record your current state of mind in
your notes?
Answer options (Single Choice)
Yes
No
2
This option excluded all other answers.
CSEDU 2025 - 17th International Conference on Computer Supported Education
502
Q14: Do you include your own reasoning in your
notes?
Answer options (Single Choice)
Yes
No
Q15: Where did you learn to take notes?
Answer options (Multiple Choice)
School
University
Self-taught
I did not receive any training
3
Other (Free text field)
Q16: Do you publish your notes? Or,
hypothetically, if you took notes, would you
publish them?
Answer options (Single Choice)
Yes
No
Q17: How do you publish your notes?
Answer options (Multiple Choice)
As research data in a repository
As bundle with the publication
On Github
Other (Free text field)
Q18: Why do you choose to not publish your
notes?
Answer options (Multiple Choice)
My notes are private
They may contain errors
They may contain cues for further research
Other (Free text field)
Q19: When you write, how do you use your notes?
Answer options (Multiple Choice)
As text snippets
As a memory aid
They may contain cues for further research
Not at all
4
Other (Free text field)
3
This option excluded all other answers.
4
This option excluded all other answers.
Q20: Given, you would use any software in
note-taking. Would you prefer a cloud-based
solution or would you prefer to store the notes on
your local machine?
Answer options (Multiple Choice)
Cloud Service (OneDrive, Apple Cloud, Google
Drive or similar third party service)
Owned Cloud (A cloud service you or your em-
ployer control, i.e. NextCloud, OwnCloud or sim-
ilar)
Local Network storage (i.e. Your own NAS,
Shared drive at work)
Local Device
I don’t use software
5
5
This option excluded all other answers.
Our Notes Leave too Much to Say: Investigating Note-Taking Practices and Technological Tools in Academia
503