
edge about structured note-taking techniques and no
formal training. Thus, they relied on their intuition
(
´
Sl˛ezak
´
Swiat, 2022). The studies done by Qian et
al. and Mueller and Oppenheimer further support this
notion (Qian et al., 2020; Mueller and Oppenheimer,
2014). As outlined above, the findings of Mueller and
Oppenheimer can well be explained by a lack of train-
ing, rather than by the use of technology.
This correlates well with our findings, since the
Sentence- and Outline Methods can be considered in-
tuitive, as they do not rely on any structure that would
necessitate training and knowledge. Thus, based on
our results, we consider that more training is needed
to get more structured and reusable notes.
Taking into account that Fix and Dittmann ob-
served a lack of training as early as 2008 (Fix and
Dittmann, 2008), the deficit we perceive has persisted
for quite some time. Any lack of training and knowl-
edge we observe today, we consider to be knock-on
effects of prior insufficient training, since the students
of 2008 may well be teachers today.
Since a larger number of our participants con-
sider themselves self-taught (N=12), we could assume
that they read training resources themselves. There
are more recent training resources like, for instance,
Király et al.’s ’Jump-Start Your Writing’ (Király et al.,
2023), or even older manuals like Umberto Eco’s
’How to write a thesis’, originally published in 1977
and translated into English in 2015 (Eco, 2015) that
show usages of more structured techniques. Király et
al. propose four-column-reading (Király et al., 2023),
while Eco, being an older resource, does not include
computerised methods. He proposes the use of index
cards and files in filing cabinets (Eco, 2015), a tech-
nique that is still employed by some researchers today
(McCarty, 2023). This list is by no means exhaustive.
However, since we observed a near absence of
structured methods present in teaching materials, we
can assume that our participants, while considering
themselves self-taught, did not consult the these ma-
terials, or did not feel the need to do so. This raises
the question if the notion of self-teaching rather more
refers to ’make it up as you go’, than actual course-
work.
While structured methods for note-taking are not
part of the daily work of our participants, we found
that a sense of efficacy in note-taking was well rep-
resented. Our findings suggest that the new gener-
ation of researchers, represented by our Predocs co-
hort, use their notes as text snippets to a significant
degree, whereas in our Postdocs cohort they were still
mainly used as memory aids.
The use of notes as parts of a new text is one of the
key concepts of Four-Column-Reading, and we argue
that it shows a concern of efficacy and reusability in
note-taking. It also indicates that a degree of synthesis
that must have been taken place, if notes were consid-
ered to be incorporated into a publication. This is fur-
ther corroborated by the fact that the vast majority of
our participants included their own reasoning in their
notes. While the lack of training may still present is-
sues, we argue that it could be offset by the use of
tools that are specifically designed to reevaluate, syn-
thesise and manage notes in ways described by, for
example, Pitura, Eco, or McCarty (Pitura, 2023; Eco,
2015; McCarty, 2023). We also argue that the neces-
sary structure for reuse may also be introduced by the
use of specific tools.
Tool usage in note-taking, however, also shows
signs of knock-on effects that can be traced to a
lack of formal training in students and teachers alike.
While there is a plethora of dedicated note-taking
tools (RoamResearch, Evernote, LogSeq, Obsidian,
Google Keep, Notion.io, Zettelkasten
1
to name a
few), their use did not present itself in our findings.
We assume our participants used the tools they are
familiar with, which are mainly word-processors in
the form of Microsoft Word. Its use is usually taught
in schools, and it is omnipresent. This aligns well
with findings from Qian et al., who also observed that
their participants did not use dedicated note-taking
tools. Their participants often showed a ’make-do’
attitude in adapting known tools to a use they were
never designed for and which thus offered sub-par
performance on the task at hand. One participant
went as far as using a built-in sticky-note tool in their
computer’s operating system to take their notes (Qian
et al., 2020). While their participants clearly sought
and found creative ways to employ their known tools,
Qian et al. found it to be a source of friction, with
researchers switching between up to four (adapted)
tools to perform their daily work (Qian et al., 2020),
hampering their abilities.
Pitura, McCarty, Király et al. and Eco present in
their works methods that could reasonably be com-
puterized and in the case of ’Zettelkasten’ are even
available as finished product. But knowledge of these
tools did not present itself in our findings. To establish
specific reasons, further research would be required,
though we assume from anecdotal evidence that tools
might simply be unknown.
Another indicator that correlates well with a lack
of formal training is what we perceive as the employ-
ment of a single pass reading strategy. We deduce this
from the lack of annotations our participants make in
their sources. This aligns well with research done by
´
Sl˛ezak-
´
Swiat in 2022, where she found that while her
1
http://zettelkasten.danielluedecke.de/
Our Notes Leave too Much to Say: Investigating Note-Taking Practices and Technological Tools in Academia
499